The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control

Adam Hampshire, Samuel R Chamberlain, Martin M Monti, John Duncan, Adrian M Owen, Adam Hampshire, Samuel R Chamberlain, Martin M Monti, John Duncan, Adrian M Owen

Abstract

There is growing interest regarding the role of the right inferior frontal gyrus (RIFG) during a particular form of executive control referred to as response inhibition. However, tasks used to examine neural activity at the point of response inhibition have rarely controlled for the potentially confounding effects of attentional demand. In particular, it is unclear whether the RIFG is specifically involved in inhibitory control, or is involved more generally in the detection of salient or task relevant cues. The current fMRI study sought to clarify the role of the RIFG in executive control by holding the stimulus conditions of one of the most popular response inhibition tasks-the Stop Signal Task-constant, whilst varying the response that was required on reception of the stop signal cue. Our results reveal that the RIFG is recruited when important cues are detected, regardless of whether that detection is followed by the inhibition of a motor response, the generation of a motor response, or no external response at all.

Copyright 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Activation associated with target detection and response inhibition. Fig. 1 illustrates the similar pattern of activation observed during target detection, and during both successful and failed inhibition in the SST task. Significant clusters are rendered in a region between the inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insula in all three conditions. Target detection data are taken from (Hampshire et al., 2007) whereas SST data are taken from combined published and unpublished data sets including (Chamberlain et al., 2009).
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
ROIs defined on the basis of previously acquired SST data. Fig. 2 illustrates the regions of interest (ROIs) generated from the analysis of 81 participants who previously undertook the fMRI Stop Signal Task. (A) ROIs were rendered bilaterally in the inferior frontal gyri (LIFG & RIFG) and in the inferior parietal cortex (IPC) from the contrast of inhibition minus baseline. (B) Contrasting failed minus successful inhibition rendered ROIs in three locations in the left sensorimotor cortex (SMC1, SMC2, & SMC3) and in the right cerebellum (RCer).
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Results from the ROI analysis. Fig. 3 illustrates the results from the main ROI analyses. The IFG bilaterally, and the left inferior parietal cortex showed significant increases in the BOLD response when the up arrow cue was detected in all three acquisition blocks. By contrast, the motor related ROIs all showed increased BOLD signal to up arrow cues selectively when the subsequent response was a button press. Interestingly, the SMC1 and SMC2 ROIs were also significantly deactivated when the subsequent response was the inhibition of a button press. ⁎ p 

Fig. 4

Results from the whole brain…

Fig. 4

Results from the whole brain analysis. (A) The whole brain analysis revealed significant…

Fig. 4
Results from the whole brain analysis. (A) The whole brain analysis revealed significant increases in BOLD signal when up arrow cues were detected across all three task conditions in a network of brain regions including the IFG bilaterally. There was no significant difference between acquisition blocks in the RIFG even at a liberal uncorrected threshold. (B) There was a significant effect of block within the left sensorimotor cortex ROIs.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Results from the whole brain analysis. (A) The whole brain analysis revealed significant increases in BOLD signal when up arrow cues were detected across all three task conditions in a network of brain regions including the IFG bilaterally. There was no significant difference between acquisition blocks in the RIFG even at a liberal uncorrected threshold. (B) There was a significant effect of block within the left sensorimotor cortex ROIs.

References

    1. Anderson M.C., Ochsner K.N., Kuhl B., Cooper J., Robertson E., Gabrieli S.W., Glover G.H., Gabrieli J.D. Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science. 2004;303:232–235.
    1. Aron A.R., Dowson J.H., Sahakian B.J., Robbins T.W. Methylphenidate improves response inhibition in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2003;54:1465–1468.
    1. Aron A.R., Fletcher P.C., Bullmore E.T., Sahakian B.J., Robbins T.W. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans (vol 6, pg 115, 2003) Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:1329.
    1. Aron A.R., Poldrack R.A. Cortical and subcortical contributions to Stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:2424–2433.
    1. Aron A.R., Robbins T.W., Poldrack R.A. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004;8:170–177.
    1. Band G.P.H., van der Molen M.W., Logan G.D. Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. Acta Psychol. 2003;112:105–142.
    1. Braver T.S., Barch D.M., Gray J.R., Molfese D.L., Snyder A. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cereb. Cortex. 2001;11:825–836.
    1. Brett, M., Anton, J., Valabregue, R. and Poline, J., 2002. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox [abstract]. Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, Sendai, Japan, pp.
    1. Chamberlain S.R., Hampshire A., Muller U., Rubia K., Campo N.D., Craig K., Regenthal R., Suckling J., Roiser J.P., Grant J.E., Bullmore E.T., Robbins T.W., Sahakian B.J. Atomoxetine modulates right inferior frontal activation during inhibitory control: a pharmacological functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry. 2009
    1. Chaudhry, A., Hamphire, A., Owen, A. M. and Roberts, A. C., Under Submission. Differentiating the contribution of lateral orbitofrontal cortex and inferior frontal gyrus to reversal learning with functional MRI.
    1. Chelazzi L., Duncan J., Miller E.K., Desimone R. Responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual search. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;80:2918–2940.
    1. Cools R., Clark L., Owen A.M., Robbins T.W. Defining the neural mechanisms of probabilistic reversal learning using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:4563–4567.
    1. Corbetta M., Shulman G.L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215.
    1. Dehaene S., Kerszberg M., Changeux J.P. A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1998;95:14529–14534.
    1. Dove A., Brett M., Cusack R., Owen A.M. Dissociable contributions of the mid-ventrolateral frontal cortex and the medial temporal lobe system to human memory. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1790–1801.
    1. Dove A., Pollmann S., Schubert T., Wiggins C.J., von Cramon D.Y. Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 2000;9:103–109.
    1. Duann J.R., Ide J.S., Luo X., Li C.S. Functional connectivity delineates distinct roles of the inferior frontal cortex and presupplementary motor area in stop signal inhibition. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:10171–10179.
    1. Duncan J. An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;2:820–829.
    1. Duncan J. EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: brain mechanisms of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Colchester) 2006;59:2–27.
    1. Duncan J., Owen A.M. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:475–483.
    1. Everling S., Tinsley C.J., Gaffan D., Duncan J. Filtering of neural signals by focused attention in the monkey prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:671–676.
    1. Freedman D.J., Riesenhuber M., Poggio T., Miller E.K. Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science. 2001;291:312–316.
    1. Funahashi S., Bruce C.J., Goldman-Rakic P.S. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1989;61:331–349.
    1. Fuster J.M., Alexander G.E. Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science. 1971;173:652–654.
    1. Hampshire A., Duncan J., Owen A.M. Selective tuning of the blood oxygenation level-dependent response during simple target detection dissociates human frontoparietal subregions. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:6219–6223.
    1. Hampshire A., Gruszka A., Fallon S.J., Owen A.M. Inefficiency in self-organized attentional switching in the normal aging population is associated with decreased activity in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008;20:1670–1686.
    1. Hampshire A., Owen A.M. Fractionating attentional control using event-related fMRI. Cerebral. Cortex. 2006;16:1679–1689.
    1. Hampshire A., Thompson R., Duncan J., Owen A.M. The target selective neural response–similarity, ambiguity, and learning effects. PLoS ONE. 2008
    1. Hampshire A., Thompson R., Duncan J., Owen A.M. Selective tuning of the right inferior frontal gyrus during target detection. Cogn. Affect Behav. Neurosci. 2009;9:103–112.
    1. Henson R.N.A., Shallice T., Dolan R.J. Right prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval: a functional MRI test of the monitoring hypothesis. Brain. 1999;122:1367–1381.
    1. Hon N., Epstein R.A., Owen A.M., Duncan J. Frontoparietal activity with minimal decision and control. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:9805–9809.
    1. Kastner S., Pinsk M.A., De Weerd P., Desimone R., Ungerleider L.G. Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron. 1999;22:751–761.
    1. Li C.S., Huang C., Constable R.T., Sinha R. Imaging response inhibition in a stop-signal task: neural correlates independent of signal monitoring and post-response processing. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:186–192.
    1. Linden D.E.J., Prvulovic D., Formisano E., Vollinger M., Zanella F.E., Goebel R., Dierks T. The functional neuroanatomy of target detection: an fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. Cereb. Cortex. 1999;9:815–823.
    1. Logan G.D., Cowan W.B. On the ability to inhibit thought and action–a theory of an act of control. Psychol. Rev. 1984;91:295–327.
    1. Menon V., Adleman N.E., White C.D., Glover G.H., Reiss A.L. Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2001;12:131–143.
    1. Miller E.K., Cohen J.D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202.
    1. Monsell S. Task switching. Trends. Cogn. Sci. 2003;7:134–140.
    1. Norman D.A., Shallice T. Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behaviour. In: Davidson R., editor. Vol. 1. Plenun Times; New York: 1980. (Consciousness and self-regulation).
    1. Petrides M., Pandya D.N. Comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human and the macaque ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and corticocortical connection patterns in the monkey. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2002;16:291–310.
    1. Picton T.W., Stuss D.T., Alexander M.P., Shallice T., Binns M.A., Gillingham S. Effects of focal frontal lesions on response inhibition. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:826–838.
    1. Prodoehl J., Yu H., Little D.M., Abraham I., Vaillancourt D.E. Region of interest template for the human basal ganglia: comparing EPI and standardized space approaches. Neuroimage. 2008;39:956–965.
    1. Purdon C., Clark D.A. Metacognition and obsessions. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 1999;6:102–110.
    1. Rao S.C., Rainer G., Miller E.K. Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science. 1997;276:821–824.
    1. Rubia K., Overmeyer S., Taylor E., Brammer M., Williams S.C., Simmons A., Bullmore E.T. Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: a study with functional MRI. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1999;156:891–896.
    1. Rubia K., Smith A.B., Brammer M.J., Taylor E. Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection. Neuroimage. 2003;20:351–358.
    1. Shallice T., Stuss D.T., Alexander M.P., Picton T.W., Derkzen D. The multiple dimensions of sustained attention. Cortex. 2008;44:794–805.
    1. Shallice T., Stuss D.T., Picton T.W., Alexander M.P., Gillingham S. Mapping task switching in frontal cortex through neuropsychological group studies. Front. Neurosci. 2008;2:79–85.
    1. Simmonds D.J., Pekar J.J., Mostofsky S.H. Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:224–232.
    1. van Boxtel G.J.M., van der Molen M.W., Jennings J.R., Brunia C.H.M. A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm. Biol. Psychol. 2001;58:229–262.
    1. Verbruggen F., Logan G.D. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008;12:418–424.
    1. Wagner A.D., Pare-Blagoev E.J., Clark J., Poldrack R.A. Recovering meaning: Left prefrontal cortex guides controlled semantic retrieval. Neuron. 2001;31:329–338.
    1. Wegner D.M. Try not to think of a white bear. Psychol. Today. 1989;23:64–66.
    1. Wegner D.M., Schneider D.J., Carter S.R., White T.L. Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. J.Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1987;53:5–13.
    1. Wenzlaff E.M., Wegner D.M. Thought suppression. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2000;51:59–91.
    1. Williams B.R., Ponesse J.S., Schachar R.J., Logan G.D., Tannock R. Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Dev. Psychol. 1999;35:205–213.

Source: PubMed

3