Differential impact of chronotype on weekday and weekend sleep timing and duration

Stephanie E Roepke, Jeanne F Duffy, Stephanie E Roepke, Jeanne F Duffy

Abstract

Most recommendations are that adults should obtain 7-8 hours of sleep per night, although there are individual differences in self-reported sleep need. Chronotype (preference for early or late sleep timing), in combination with social demands, may affect the ability to obtain adequate sleep. This questionnaire study assessed perceived sleep need and self-reported sleep timing and duration during the week and on the weekend with respect to chronotype in visitors to the Museum of Science in Boston. Increasing age was associated with greater morningness. After adjusting for age, we found no significant association between chronotype and self-reported sleep need, or between chronotype and weekday sleep duration. However, we did find that greater eveningness was associated with a larger gap between self-reported sleep need and weekday sleep duration. On weekends, greater eveningness was associated with a longer sleep duration and greater extension of sleep, with the sleep extension achieved by later wake times. Together, these findings suggest that evening types accumulate a sleep debt during the week, despite reporting a similar sleep need and duration as morning types, and evening types then attempt to make up for that lost weekday sleep on the weekends. Studies of sleep need and sleep duration should take chronotype into account, and studies of chronotype may be confounded by the association between age and morningness, and must account for this potential confound in selection criteria and/or analysis.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Relationship between MEQ score and delay of waketime on weekends. The delay of waketime (in hours) between weekdays and weekends for each of the 145 participants is plotted with respect to their MEQ score. MEQ scores can range from 18 to 86, with higher numbers indicating greater morningness. Positive wake time delays indicate later weekend than weekday waketimes, while negative delays indicate earlier weekend than weekday waketimes. Abbreviation: MEQ, Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Relationship between MEQ score and weekend sleep duration. The weekend sleep duration (in hours) calculated from self-reported weekend bedtimes and waketimes for each of the 145 participants is plotted with respect to their MEQ score. MEQ scores can range from 18 to 86, with higher numbers indicating greater morningness. Abbreviation: MEQ, Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Relationship between MEQ score and weekend sleep extension. The extension of sleep (in hours) between weekdays and weekends calculated from self- reported weekday and weekend bed and wake for each of the 145 participants is plotted with respect to their MEQ score. MEQ scores can range from 18 to 86, with higher numbers indicating greater morningness. Positive weekend sleep extensions indicate longer weekend than weekday sleep, while negative extensions indicate shorter weekend than weekday sleep. Abbreviation: MEQ, Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Relationship between MEQ score and age. The age (in years) for each of the 145 participants is plotted with respect to their MEQ score. MEQ scores can range from 18 to 86, with higher numbers indicating greater morningness. There was a significant association between age and MEQ score when evaluated with a mixed model (see text) and there was a significant correlation (Pearson correlation coefficient r = 0.33, P < 0.0001). Abbreviation: MEQ, Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire.

References

    1. Horne J. Is there a sleep debt? Sleep. 2004;27:1047–1049.
    1. Van Dongen HPA, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: Dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26:117–126.
    1. Wingard DL, Berkman LF. Mortality risk associated with sleeping patterns among adults. Sleep. 1983;6:102–107.
    1. Kripke DF, Garfinkel L, Wingard DL, Klauber MR, Marler MR. Mortality associated with sleep duration and insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:131–136.
    1. Spiegel K, Leproult R, Van Cauter E. Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine function. Lancet. 1999;354:1435–1439.
    1. Meier-Ewert HK, Ridker PM, Rifai N, et al. Effect of sleep loss on C-reactive protein, an inflammatory marker of cardiovascular risk. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2004;43:678–683.
    1. Haack M, Sanchez E, Mullington JM. Elevated inflammatory markers in response to prolonged sleep restriction are associated with increased pain experience in healthy volunteers. Sleep. 2007;30:1145–1152.
    1. Van Dongen HPA, Maislin G, Dinges DF. Dealing with inter-individual differences in the temporal dynamics of fatigue and performance: Importance and techniques. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75(Suppl 3):A147–A154.
    1. Wright KP, Jr, Hull JT, Hughes RJ, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA. Sleep and wakefulness out of phase with internal biological time impairs learning in humans. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:508–521.
    1. Roehrs T, Shore E, Papineau K, Rosenthal L, Roth T. A two-week sleep extension in sleepy normals. Sleep. 1996;19:576–582.
    1. Wehr TA. In short photoperiods, human sleep is biphasic. J Sleep Res. 1992;2:103–107.
    1. Klerman EB, Dijk DJ. Age-related reduction in the maximal capacity for sleep — implications for insomnia. Curr Biol. 2008;18:1118–1123.
    1. National Sleep Foundation Executive summary of the 2005 “Sleep in America” poll 2005Available from: Accessed Jul 31, 2010
    1. Hale L. Who has time to sleep? J Public Health (Oxf) 2005;27:205–211.
    1. Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Rose LR, Hall JA, Kupfer DJ. The sleep of healthy people — A diary study. Chronobiol Int. 2000;17:49–60.
    1. Taillard J, Philip P, Bioulac B. Morningness/eveningness and the need for sleep. J Sleep Res. 1999;8:291–295.
    1. Horne JA, Östberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4:97–110.
    1. Duffy JF, Rimmer DW, Czeisler CA. Association of intrinsic circadian period with morningness-eveningness, usual wake time, and circadian phase. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:895–899.
    1. Park YM, Matsumoto K, Seo YJ, Shinkoda H. Sleep in relation to age, sex, and chronotype in Japanese workers. Percept Mot Skills. 1998;87:199–215.
    1. Schneider AM, Randler C. Daytime sleepiness during transition into daylight saving time in adolescents: Are owls at higher risk? Sleep Med. 2009;10:1047–1050.
    1. Hublin C, Kaprio J, Partinen M, Koskenvuo M. Insufficient sleep – A population- based study in adults. Sleep. 2001;24:392–400.
    1. Hildebrandt G. Outline of chronohygiene. Chronobiologia. 1976;3:113–127.
    1. Breithaupt H, Hildebrandt G, Döhre D, Josch R, Sieber U, Werner M. Tolerance to shift of sleep, as related to the individual’s circadian phase position. Ergonomics. 1978;21:767–774.
    1. Hilliker NAJ, Muehlbach MJ, Schweitzer PK, Walsh JK. Sleepiness/alertness on a simulated night shift schedule and morningness-eveningness tendency. Sleep. 1992;15:430–433.
    1. Mongrain V, Carrier J, Dumont M. Circadian and homeostatic sleep regulation in morningness-eveningness. J Sleep Res. 2006;15:162–166.
    1. Ishihara K, Miyake S, Miyasita A, Miyata Y. Morningness-eveningness preference and sleep habits in Japanese office workers of different ages. Chronobiologia. 1991;18:9–16.
    1. Carrier J, Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ. Sleep and morningness-eveningness in the ‘middle’ years of life (20–59 y) J Sleep Res. 1997;6:230–237.
    1. Duffy JF, Dijk DJ, Hall EF, Czeisler CA. Relationship of endogenous circadian melatonin and temperature rhythms to self-reported preference for morning or evening activity in young and older people. J Investig Med. 1999;47:141–150.
    1. Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Age-related change in the relationship between circadian period, circadian phase, and diurnal preference in humans. Neurosci Lett. 2002;318:117–120.
    1. Bliwise DL. Normal aging. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Saunders; 2005.
    1. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, III, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28:193–213.
    1. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1991;14:540–545.
    1. Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: Daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms. 2003;18:80–90.
    1. Costa G, Lievore F, Ferrari P, Gaffuri E. Usual meal times in relation to age, sex, work activity and morningness-eveningness. Chronobiologia. 1987;14:383–391.
    1. Monk TH, Reynolds CF, III, Buysse DJ, et al. Circadian characteristics of healthy 80-year-olds and their relationship to objectively recorded sleep. J Gerontol. 1991;46:M171–M175.
    1. Ishihara K, Miyake S, Miyasita A, Miyata Y. Comparisons of sleep-wake habits of morning and evening types in Japanese worker sample. J Human Ergol. 1988;17:111–118.
    1. Baehr EK, Revelle W, Eastman CI. Individual differences in the phase and amplitude of the human circadian temperature rhythm: With an emphasis on morningness-eveningness. J Sleep Res. 2000;9:117–127.

Source: PubMed

3
購読する