Increasing cognitive load attenuates right arm swing in healthy human walking

Tim Killeen, Christopher S Easthope, Linard Filli, Lilla Lőrincz, Miriam Schrafl-Altermatt, Peter Brugger, Michael Linnebank, Armin Curt, Björn Zörner, Marc Bolliger, Tim Killeen, Christopher S Easthope, Linard Filli, Lilla Lőrincz, Miriam Schrafl-Altermatt, Peter Brugger, Michael Linnebank, Armin Curt, Björn Zörner, Marc Bolliger

Abstract

Human arm swing looks and feels highly automated, yet it is increasingly apparent that higher centres, including the cortex, are involved in many aspects of locomotor control. The addition of a cognitive task increases arm swing asymmetry during walking, but the characteristics and mechanism of this asymmetry are unclear. We hypothesized that this effect is lateralized and a Stroop word-colour naming task-primarily involving left hemisphere structures-would reduce right arm swing only. We recorded gait in 83 healthy subjects aged 18-80 walking normally on a treadmill and while performing a congruent and incongruent Stroop task. The primary measure of arm swing asymmetry-an index based on both three-dimensional wrist trajectories in which positive values indicate proportionally smaller movements on the right-increased significantly under dual-task conditions in those aged 40-59 and further still in the over-60s, driven by reduced right arm flexion. Right arm swing attenuation appears to be the norm in humans performing a locomotor-cognitive dual-task, confirming a prominent role of the brain in locomotor behaviour. Women under 60 are surprisingly resistant to this effect, revealing unexpected gender differences atop the hierarchical chain of locomotor control.

Keywords: arm swing; central pattern generator; cognitive control; dual-task; gender; motor control.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Experimental set-up. For the normal walking condition (a), subjects walked on an instrumented treadmill while fixating a black cross. They then performed two Stroop colour-naming task (see Material and methods) of differing difficulty. Image (b) shows the simpler task in which word and colour stimuli are congruent. In the more difficult, incongruent task (c) word and colour are discordant.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Performance in congruent and incongruent Stroop tasks; (a) males, (b) females. Relative frequency of Stroop trial error rate trichotomized into no errors, one to five errors or more than five errors. (c) Correlation (Spearman's ρ) between arm swing asymmetry index and error rate during the incongruent Stroop task.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Arm swing asymmetry under increasing cognitive load. Wrist trajectory asymmetry index is calculated using the left and right three-dimensional wrist centre trajectories, with left dominance resulting in a positive value and vice versa. ASI is given as the mean value per gait cycle over a trial of 45 s (approx. 42 gait cycles at 4 km h−1). s.e.m., Standard error of the mean. Statistical significance was determined using a linear mixed model with post hoc t-tests. The p-values are corrected for multiple pairwise within-age group comparisons using the Bonferroni method.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Absolute wrist trajectory length. Three-dimensional wrist joint centre trajectories for younger, middle-aged and older adults during normal walking and during a congruent and an incongruent Stroop dual-task. GC, gait cycle. Error bars indicate 1 s.e.m. Statistical significance was determined using a linear mixed model with post hoc t-tests. The p-values are corrected for multiple pairwise within-age group comparisons using the Bonferroni method.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
(a) Sagittal gait cycle mean joint angle maxima and minima based on the approach used by Roggendorf et al. [25]. (b) Sagittal shoulder angle changes during normal walking and under increased cognitive load (incongruent Stroop task) in older adults walking on a treadmill. Diagrams represent the right (green) and left (red) mean sagittal shoulder angle maxima and minima (thick lines) per gait cycle with associated single standard deviations (thin dark lines). A significant decrease in shoulder flexion in the incongruent Stroop task is indicated by asterisk (*). Elbow flexion was also reduced under increased cognitive load, with preserved extension (not shown; see Results section).
Figure 6.
Figure 6.
Arm swing asymmetry under increasing cognitive load—gender effects. Wrist trajectory asymmetry index is calculated using the left and right three-dimensional wrist centre trajectories, with left dominance resulting in a positive value and vice versa. ASI is given as the mean value per gait cycle over a trial of 45 s (approx. 42 gait cycles at 4 km h−1), s.e.m.; standard error of the mean. Statistical significance was determined using a linear mixed model with post hoc t-tests. The p-values are corrected for pairwise within-group comparisons using the Bonferroni method.

References

    1. Goudriaan M, Jonkers I, van Dieen JH, Bruijn SM. 2014. Arm swing in human walking: what is their drive? Gait Posture 40, 321–326. ()
    1. Collins SH, Adamczyk PG, Kuo AD. 2009. Dynamic arm swinging in human walking. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 3679–3688. ()
    1. Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP, Jing B. 2012. Activity of upper limb muscles during human walking. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 22, 199–206. ()
    1. Solopova IA, Selionov VA, Zhvansky DS, Gurfinkel VS, Ivanenko Y. 2016. Human cervical spinal cord circuitry activated by tonic input can generate rhythmic arm movements. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 1018–1030. ()
    1. Dietz V. 2002. Do human bipeds use quadrupedal coordination? Trends Neurosci. 25, 462–467. ()
    1. Barthelemy D, Nielsen JB. 2010. Corticospinal contribution to arm muscle activity during human walking. J. Physiol. 588, 967–979. ()
    1. Mirelman A, Bernad-Elazari H, Nobel T, Thaler A, Peruzzi A, Plotnik M, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. 2015. Effects of aging on arm swing during gait: the role of gait speed and dual tasking. PLoS ONE 10, e0136043 ()
    1. Plate A, Sedunko D, Pelykh O, Schlick C, Ilmberger JR, Bötzel K. 2015. Normative data for arm swing asymmetry: how (a)symmetrical are we? Gait Posture 41, 13–18. ()
    1. Lewek MD, Poole R, Johnson J, Halawa O, Huang X. 2010. Arm swing magnitude and asymmetry during gait in the early stages of Parkinson's disease. Gait Posture 31, 256–260. ()
    1. Killeen T, Easthope CS, Filli L, Linnebank M, Curt A, Bolliger M, Zörner B. 2016. Modulating arm swing symmetry with cognitive load: a window on rhythmic spinal locomotor networks in humans? J. Neurotrauma (Epub ahead of print) ()
    1. Milham MP, Erickson KI, Banich MT, Kramer AF, Webb A, Wszalek T, Cohen NJ. 2002. Attentional control in the aging brain: insights from an fMRI study of the Stroop task. Brain Cogn. 49, 277–296. ()
    1. Mohtasib RS, Lumley G, Goodwin JA, Emsley HCA, Sluming V, Parkes LM. 2012. Calibrated fMRI during a cognitive Stroop task reveals reduced metabolic response with increasing age. Neuroimage 59, 1143–1151. ()
    1. Leung HC, Skudlarski P, Gatenby JC, Peterson BS, Gore JC. 2000. An event-related functional MRI study of the Stroop color word interference task. Cereb. Cortex 10, 552–560. ()
    1. Stafford T, Gurney K. 2005. The basal ganglia as the selection mechanism in a cognitive task. In Modelling natural action selection (eds JJ Bryson, TJ Prescott, A Seth), pp. 77–83. Edinburgh, UK: AISB Press.
    1. Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB, Fling BW, Gordon MT, Gwin JT, Kwak Y, Lipps DB. 2011. Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional and biomechanical effects. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 721–733. ()
    1. Dietz V. 1992. Human neuronal control of automatic functional movements: interaction between central programs and afferent input. Physiol. Rev. 72, 33–69.
    1. Hausdorff JM, Yogev G, Springer S, Simon ES, Giladi N. 2005. Walking is more like catching than tapping: gait in the elderly as a complex cognitive task. Exp. Brain Res. 164, 541–548. ()
    1. Jaeger L, Marchal-Crespo L, Wolf P, Riener R, Michels L, Kollias S. 2014. Brain activation associated with active and passive lower limb stepping. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 828 ()
    1. Crenna P, Carpinella I, Lopiano L, Marzegan A, Rabuffetti M, Rizzone M, Lanotte M, Ferrarin M. 2008. Influence of basal ganglia on upper limb locomotor synergies. Evidence from deep brain stimulation and L-DOPA treatment in Parkinson's disease. Brain 131, 3410–3420. ()
    1. Belanger HG, Cimino CR. 2002. The lateralized Stroop: a meta-analysis and its implications for models of semantic processing. Brain Lang. 83, 384–402. ()
    1. Al-Yahya E, Dawes H, Smith L, Dennis A, Howells K, Cockburn J. 2011. Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 715–728. ()
    1. Falgairolle M, de Seze M, Juvin L, Morin D, Cazalets JR. 2006. Coordinated network functioning in the spinal cord: an evolutionary perspective. J. Physiol. Paris 100, 304–316. ()
    1. Zehr EP, Carroll TJ, Chua R, Collins DF, Frigon A, Haridas C, Hundza SR, Thompson AK. 2004. Possible contributions of CPG activity to the control of rhythmic human arm movement. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 82, 556–568. ()
    1. Umberger BR. 2008. Effects of suppressing arm swing on kinematics, kinetics, and energetics of human walking. J. Biomech. 41, 2575–2580. ()
    1. Roggendorf J, Chen S, Baudrexel S, van de Loo S, Seifried C, Hilker R. 2012. Arm swing asymmetry in Parkinson's disease measured with ultrasound based motion analysis during treadmill gait. Gait Posture 35, 116–120. ()
    1. Stroop J. 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 121, 15–23. ()
    1. Cohn NB, Dustman RE, Bradford DC. 1984. Age-related decrements in Stroop color test performance. J. Clin. Psychol. 40, 1244–1250. ()
    1. West R, Alain C. 2000. Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. Psychophysiology 37, 179–189. ()
    1. Coren S, Porac C, Duncan P. 1979. A behaviorally validated self-report inventory to assess four types of lateral preference. J. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1, 55–64. ()
    1. Bohannon R. 1997. Comfortable and maximum walking speed of adults aged 20–79 years: reference values and determinants. Age Ageing 26, 15–19. ()
    1. Rossier P, Wade DT. 2001. Validity and reliability comparison of 4 mobility measures in patients presenting with neurologic impairment. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 82, 9–13. ()
    1. Vicon Motion Systems. 2010. Plug-in Gait Product Guide.
    1. Svoboda B, Kranzl A. 2012. A study of the reproducibility of the marker application of the Cleveland Clinic Marker Set including the Plug-In Gait Upper Body Model in clinical gait analysis. Gait Posture 36, S62–S63. ()
    1. Macleod CM. 1991. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109, 163–203. ()
    1. Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP, Brockmann K, Gilster R, Koch A, Stolze H. 2008. Asymmetry of arm-swing not related to handedness. Gait Posture 27, 447–454. ()
    1. Kloos AD, Fisher LC, Detloff MR, Hassenzahl DL, Basso DM. 2005. Stepwise motor and all-or-none sensory recovery is associated with nonlinear sparing after incremental spinal cord injury in rats. Exp. Neurol. 191, 251–265. ()
    1. Filli L, Zörner B, Weinmann O, Schwab ME. 2011. Motor deficits and recovery in rats with unilateral spinal cord hemisection mimic the Brown-Séquard syndrome. Brain 134, 2261–2273. ()
    1. Hamacher D, Hamacher D, Schega L. 2014. Towards the importance of minimum toe clearance in level ground walking in a healthy elderly population. Gait Posture 40, 727–729. ()
    1. Meester D, Al-Yahya E, Dawes H, Martin-Fagg P, Piñon C. 2014. Associations between prefrontal cortex activation and H-reflex modulation during dual task gait. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 78 ()
    1. Jahn K, Deutschländer A, Stephan T, Strupp M, Wiesmann M, Brandt T. 2004. Brain activation patterns during imagined stance and locomotion in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 22, 1722–1731. ()
    1. Dietz V, Fouad K, Bastiaanse CM. 2001. Neuronal coordination of arm and leg movements during human locomotion. Eur. J. Neurosci. 14, 1906–1914. ()
    1. Haridas C, Zehr EP. 2003. Coordinated interlimb compensatory responses to electrical stimulation of cutaneous nerves in the hand and foot during walking. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 2850–2861. ()
    1. Lundberg A. 1999. Descending control of forelimb movements in the cat. Brain Res. Bull. 50, 323–324. ()
    1. Perret E. 1974. The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour. Neuropsychologia 12, 323–330. ()
    1. Riley TL, Ray WF, Massey EW. 1977. Gait mechanisms: asymmetry of arm motion in normal subjects. Mil. Med. 142, 467–468.
    1. Regnaux JP, Roberston J, Smail D, Daniel O, Bussel B. 2006. Human treadmill walking needs attention. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 3, 19 ()
    1. Clark DJ, Christou EA, Ring SA, Williamson JB, Doty L. 2014. Enhanced somatosensory feedback reduces prefrontal cortical activity during walking in older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 69, 1422–1428. ()
    1. Frigon A, Gossard J-P. 2009. Asymmetric control of cycle period by the spinal locomotor rhythm generator in the adult cat. J. Physiol. 587, 4617–4628. ()
    1. Dietz V. 2002. Proprioception and locomotor disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 781–790. ()
    1. Palmer E, Ashby P. 1992. Corticospinal projections to upper limb motoneurones in humans. J. Physiol. 448, 397–412. ()
    1. Brouwer B, Ashby P. 1992. Corticospinal projections to lower limb motoneurons in man. Exp. Brain Res. 89, 649–654. ()
    1. Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP, Frendel A, Jing B. 2014. Arm swing during human gait studied by EMG of upper limb muscles. In Applications, challenges, and advancements in electromyography signal processing (ed. Naik GR.), pp. 129–160. IGI Global.
    1. Montague D, Weickert CS, Tomaskovic-Crook E, Rothmond DA, Kleinman JE, Rubinow DR. 2008. Oestrogen receptor α localisation in the prefrontal cortex of three mammalian species. J. Neuroendocrinol. 20, 893–903. ()
    1. Hao J, et al. 2006. Estrogen alters spine number and morphology in prefrontal cortex of aged female rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 26, 2571–2578. ()
    1. Hjelmervik H, Westerhausen R, Osnes B, Endresen CB, Hugdahl K, Hausmann M, Specht K. 2012. Language lateralization and cognitive control across the menstrual cycle assessed with a dichotic-listening paradigm. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1866–1875. ()
    1. Keenan PA, Ezzat WH, Ginsburg K, Moore GJ. 2001. Prefrontal cortex as the site of estrogen's effect on cognition. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 577–590. ()
    1. Krug R, Born J, Rasch B. 2006. A 3-day estrogen treatment improves prefrontal cortex-dependent cognitive function in postmenopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 965–975. ()
    1. Egner T. 2009. Prefrontal cortex and cognitive control: motivating functional hierarchies. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 821–822. ()
    1. Gwin JT, Gramann K, Makeig S, Ferris DP. 2011. Electrocortical activity is coupled to gait cycle phase during treadmill walking. Neuroimage 54, 1289–1296. ()
    1. Heekeren HR, Marrett S, Bandettini PA, Ungerleider LG. 2004. A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain. Nature 431, 859–862. ()
    1. Burhan AM, Subramanian P, Pallaveshi L, Barnes B, Montero-Odasso M. 2015. Modulation of the left prefrontal cortex with high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation facilitates gait in multiple sclerosis. Case Rep. Neurol. Med. 2015, 251829 ()
    1. Juvin L, Le Gal J-P, Simmers J, Morin D. 2012. Cervicolumbar coordination in mammalian quadrupedal locomotion: role of spinal thoracic circuitry and limb sensory inputs. J. Neurosci. 32, 953–965. ()
    1. Nathan PW, Smith M, Deacon P. 1996. Vestibulospinal, reticulospinal and descending propriospinal nerve fibres in man. Brain 119, 1809–1833. ()
    1. Sejdić E, Fu Y, Pak A, Fairley JA, Chau T. 2012. The effects of rhythmic sensory cues on the temporal dynamics of human gait. PLoS ONE 7, e43104 ()
    1. Kaipust JP, McGrath D, Mukherjee M, Stergiou N. 2013. Gait variability is altered in older adults when listening to auditory stimuli with differing temporal structures. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 41, 1595–1603. ()
    1. Zifchock RA, Davis I, Higginson J, Royer T. 2008. The symmetry angle: a novel, robust method of quantifying asymmetry. Gait Posture 27, 622–627. ()
    1. Błazkiewicz M, Wiszomirska I, Wit A. 2014. Comparison of four methods of calculating the symmetry of spatial-temporal parameters of gait. Acta Bioeng. Biomech. 16, 29–35. ()
    1. Araújo R, Ferreira JJ, Antonini A, Bloem BR. 2015. ‘Gunslinger's gait’: a new cause of unilaterally reduced arm swing. Br. Med. J. 351, 1–5. ()
    1. Harvey M, Milner AD, Roberts RC. 1995. An investigation of hemispatial neglect using the landmark task. Brain Cogn. 27, 59–78. ()
    1. Ciçek M, Deouell LY, Knight RT. 2009. Brain activity during landmark and line bisection tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3, 7 ()
    1. Killeen T, Easthope C, Filli L, Lőrincz L, Schrafl-Altermatt M, Brugger P, Linnebank M, Curt A, Zörner B, Bolliger M. 2016. Data from: Increasing cognitive load attenuates right arm swing in healthy human walking. Dryad Digital Repository. ()

Source: PubMed

3
購読する