Cogmed Training Does Not Generalize to Real-World Benefits for Adult Hearing Aid Users: Results of a Blinded, Active-Controlled Randomized Trial

Helen Henshaw, Antje Heinrich, Ashana Tittle, Melanie Ferguson, Helen Henshaw, Antje Heinrich, Ashana Tittle, Melanie Ferguson

Abstract

Objectives: Performance on working memory tasks is positively associated with speech-in-noise perception performance, particularly where auditory inputs are degraded. It is suggested that interventions designed to improve working memory capacity may improve domain-general working memory performance for people with hearing loss, to benefit their real-world listening. We examined whether a 5-week training program that primarily targets the storage component of working memory (Cogmed RM, adaptive) could improve cognition, speech-in-noise perception and self-reported hearing in a randomized controlled trial of adult hearing aid users with mild to moderate hearing loss, compared with an active control (Cogmed RM, nonadaptive) group of adults from the same population.

Design: A preregistered randomized controlled trial of 57 adult hearing aid users (n = 27 experimental, n = 30 active control), recruited from a dedicated database of research volunteers, examined on-task learning and generalized improvements in measures of trained and untrained cognition, untrained speech-in-noise perception and self-reported hearing abilities, pre- to post-training. Participants and the outcome assessor were both blinded to intervention allocation. Retention of training-related improvements was examined at a 6-month follow-up assessment.

Results: Per-protocol analyses showed improvements in trained tasks (Cogmed Index Improvement) that transferred to improvements in a trained working memory task tested outside of the training software (Backward Digit Span) and a small improvement in self-reported hearing ability (Glasgow Hearing Aid Benefit Profile, Initial Disability subscale). Both of these improvements were maintained 6-month post-training. There was no transfer of learning shown to untrained measures of cognition (working memory or attention), speech-in-noise perception, or self-reported hearing in everyday life. An assessment of individual differences showed that participants with better baseline working memory performance achieved greater learning on the trained tasks. Post-training performance for untrained outcomes was largely predicted by individuals' pretraining performance on those measures.

Conclusions: Despite significant on-task learning, generalized improvements of working memory training in this trial were limited to (a) improvements for a trained working memory task tested outside of the training software and (b) a small improvement in self-reported hearing ability for those in the experimental group, compared with active controls. We found no evidence to suggest that training which primarily targets storage aspects of working memory can result in domain-general improvements that benefit everyday communication for adult hearing aid users. These findings are consistent with a significant body of evidence showing that Cogmed training only improves performance for tasks that resemble Cogmed training. Future research should focus on the benefits of interventions that enhance cognition in the context in which it is employed within everyday communication, such as training that targets dynamic aspects of cognitive control important for successful speech-in-noise perception.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT01892007.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Copyright © 2021 The Authors. Ear & Hearing is published on behalf of the American Auditory Society, by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Study design.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
CONSORT flow diagram.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
A and B, Pre- to post-training (T2–T3) change in the primary outcome measure Visual Letter Monitoring (A) 2s/letter and (B) 1s/letter for participants in the active control (n = 30) and experimental (n = 27) groups. Improvements in performance are shown as positive values. Error bars = 95% confidence intervals.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
Scatterplot showing baseline working memory (T2 Backward Digit Span, n correct trials) and on-task learning (Cogmed Index Improvement) for participants in the Experimental Group (n = 27).

References

    1. Del Re A. C., (2013). -package: Compute Effect Sizes in R, Version 0.2-2. R Package.
    1. IBM Corp. (2016). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0. Armonk, NY. IBM Corp.
    1. R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing, Version 3.3.3. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
    1. Akeroyd M. A. (2008). Are individual differences in speech reception related to individual differences in cognitive ability? A survey of twenty experimental studies with normal and hearing-impaired adults. Int J Audiol, 47(Suppl 2), S53–S71.
    1. Aksayli N. D., Sala G., Gobet F. (2019). The cognitive and academic benefits of Cogmed: A meta-analysis. Educ Res Rev, 27, 229–243.
    1. Anderson Gosselin P., & Gagné J. P. (2011). Older adults expend more listening effort than young adults recognizing speech in noise. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 54, 944–958.
    1. Anderson S., White-Schwoch T., Parbery-Clark A., Kraus N. (2013). A dynamic auditory-cognitive system supports speech-in-noise perception in older adults. Hear Res, 300, 18–32.
    1. Anderson S., White-Schwoch T., Parbery-Clark A., Kraus N. (2013). Reversal of age-related neural timing delays with training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 110, 4357–4362.
    1. Arehart K. H., Souza P., Baca R., Kates J. M. (2013). Working memory, age, and hearing loss: Susceptibility to hearing aid distortion. Ear Hear, 34, 251–260.
    1. Barker A. B., Leighton P., Ferguson M. A. (2017). Coping together with hearing loss: A qualitative meta-synthesis of the psychosocial experiences of people with hearing loss and their communication partners. Int J Audiol, 56, 297–305.
    1. Bilger R. C., Nuetzel J. M., Rabinowitz W. M., Rzeczkowski C. (1984). Standardization of a test of speech perception in noise. J Speech Hear Res, 27, 32–48.
    1. Blacker K. J., Negoita S., Ewen J. B., Courtney S. M. (2017). N-back versus complex span working memory training. J Cogn Enhanc, 1, 434–454.
    1. Bolia R. S., Nelson W. T., Ericson M. A., Simpson B. D. (2000). A speech corpus for multitalker communications research. J Acoust Soc Am, 107, 1065–1066.
    1. Boot W. R., Simons D. J., Stothart C., Stutts C. (2013). The pervasive problem with placebos in psychology: Why active control groups are not sufficient to rule out placebo effects. Perspect Psychol Sci, 8, 445–454.
    1. Boothroyd A. (1968). Developments in speech audiometry. Br J Audiol, 2, 3–10.
    1. Brehmer Y., Westerberg H., Bäckman L. (2012). Working-memory training in younger and older adults: Training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Front Hum Neurosci, 6, 63.
    1. British Society of Audiology. (2010). Recommended procedure: Ear examination United Kingdom: British Society of Audiology.
    1. British Society of Audiology. (2011). Recommended procedure: pure-tone air-conduction and bone-conduction threshold audiometry with and without masking. Reading, United Kingdom: British Society of Audiology.
    1. Chase W. G., & Ericsson K. A. (1982). Skill and Working Memory. Bower G.H. (Ed.), In: Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 1–58). Academic Press.
    1. Cherry E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. J Acoust Soc Am, 25, 975–979.
    1. Cicchetti D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol Assess, 6, 284–290.
    1. Classon E., Rudner M., Rönnberg J. (2013). Working memory compensates for hearing related phonological processing deficit. J Commun Disord, 46, 17–29.
    1. Cogmed Working Memory Training. (2011). Coaching manual. In v1.0.11: Pearson.
    1. Cohen J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis For The Behavioral Sciences (2nd Ed.). Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
    1. Conway A. R., Kane M. J., Bunting M. F., Hambrick D. Z., Wilhelm O., Engle R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychon Bull Rev, 12, 769–786.
    1. Coulson N. S., Ferguson M. A., Henshaw H., Heffernan E. (2016). Applying theories of health behaviour and change to hearing health research: Time for a new approach. Int J Audiol, 55(Suppl 3), S99–S104.
    1. Dreschler W.A., Verschuure H, Ludvigsen C., Westermann S. (2001). ICRA noises: Artificial noise signals with speech-like spectral and temporal properties for hearing instrument assessment. International Collegium for Rehabilitative Audiology. Audiology, 40, 148–157.
    1. Del Re A.C. (2013). : Compute Effect Sizes: R Package.
    1. Dryden A., Allen H. A., Henshaw H., Heinrich A. (2017). The association between cognitive performance and speech-in-noise perception for adult listeners: A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Trends Hear, 21, 2331216517744675.
    1. Dunning D. L., & Holmes J (2014). Does working memory training promote the use of strategies on untrained working memory tasks? Mem Cognit, 42, 854–862.
    1. Edwards B. (2008). The effect of hearing loss and hearing aids on cognition. American Academy of Audiology Convention. Charlotte, NC.
    1. Engle R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 11, 19–23.
    1. Ferguson M. A., & Henshaw H. (2015). Auditory training can improve working memory, attention, and communication in adverse conditions for adults with hearing loss. Front Psychol, 6, 556.
    1. Ferguson M. A., & Henshaw H. (2015a). Auditory training can improve working memory, attention, and communication in adverse conditions for adults with hearing loss. Front Psychol, 6, 556.
    1. Ferguson M. A., Henshaw H., Clark D., Moore D. R. (2014). Benefits of phoneme discrimination training in a randomized controlled trial of 50–74 year olds with mild hearing loss. Ear Hear, 35, e110–e121.
    1. Ferguson M. A., Kitterick P., Chong L., Edmondson-Jones M., Barker F., Hoare D. J. (2017). Hearing aids for mild to moderate hearing loss in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 9, CD012023.
    1. Ferguson M., & Henshaw H. (2015b). Computer and internet interventions to optimize listening and learning for people with hearing loss: accessibility, use, and adherence. Am J Audiol, 24, 338–343.
    1. Ferguson M., & Henshaw H. (2015c). How does auditory training work? Joined up thinking and listening. Semin Hear, 36, 237.
    1. Ferguson M., Maidment D., Henshaw H., Heffernan E. (2019). Evidence-based interventions for adult aural rehabilitation: That was then, this is now. Semin Hear, 40, 68–84.
    1. Francis A. L., & Nusbaum H. C. (2009). Effects of intelligibility on working memory demand for speech perception. Atten Percept Psychophys, 71, 1360–1374.
    1. Gatehouse S. (1999). Glasgow hearing aid benefit profile: Derivation and validation of client-centred outcome measures for hearing aid services. J Am Acad Audiol, 10, 80–103.
    1. Gatehouse S., Naylor G., Elberling C. (2006). Linear and nonlinear hearing aid fittings–1. Patterns of benefit. Int J Audiol, 45, 130–152.
    1. Gathercole S. E., Dunning D. L., Holmes J. (2012). Cogmed training: Let’s be realistic about intervention research. J Appl Res Mem Cogn, 1, 201–203.
    1. Gathercole S. E., Dunning D. L., Holmes J., Norris D. (2016). Working memory training involves learning new skills. J Mem Lang, 105, 19–42.
    1. Gibson B. S., Gondoli D. M., Johnson A. C., et al. . (2012). The future promise of Cogmed working memory training. J Appl Res Mem Cogn, 1, 214–216.
    1. Gordon-Salant S., & Fitzgibbons P. J. (1997). Selected cognitive factors and speech recognition performance among young and elderly listeners. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 40, 423–431.
    1. Granberg S., Pronk M., Swanepoel d. e. W., Kramer S. E., Hagsten H., Hjaldahl J., Möller C., Danermark B. (2014). The ICF core sets for hearing loss project: Functioning and disability from the patient perspective. Int J Audiol, 53, 777–786.
    1. Green C. S., & Bavelier D. (2012). Learning, attentional control, and action video games. Curr Biol, 22, R197–R206.
    1. Guye S., De Simoni C., von Bastian C. C. (2017). Do individual differences predict change in cognitive training performance? A latent growth curve modeling approach. J Cogn Enhanc, 1, 374–393.
    1. Hafter E. R. (2010). Is there a hearing aid for the thinking person? J Am Acad Audiol, 21, 594–600.
    1. Harrison T. L., Shipstead Z., Hicks K. L., Hambrick D. Z., Redick T. S., Engle R. W. (2013). Working memory training may increase working memory capacity but not fluid intelligence Psychol Sci, 24, 2409–2419.
    1. Hazan V., Messaoud-Galusi S., Rosen S., Nouwens S., Shakespeare B. (2009). Speech perception abilities of adults with dyslexia: Is there any evidence for a true deficit? J Speech Lang Hear Res, 52, 1510–1529.
    1. Hedges L. V., & Olkin I. (1985). Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. Academic Press.
    1. Heffernan E., Coulson N. S., Henshaw H., Barry J. G., Ferguson M. A. (2016). Understanding the psychosocial experiences of adults with mild-moderate hearing loss: An application of Leventhal’s self-regulatory model. Int J Audiol, 55(Suppl 3), S3–S12.
    1. Heinrich A., & Knight S. (2020). Reproducibility in cognitive hearing research: theoretical considerations and their practical application in multi-lab studies. Front Psychol, 11, 1590.
    1. Heinrich A., Gagne J. P., Viljanen A., et al. . (2016a). Effective communication as a fundamental aspect of active aging and well-being: paying attention to the challenges older adults face in noisy environments. Social Inquiry into Well-Being, 2, 51–69.
    1. Heinrich A., Henshaw H., Ferguson M. A. (2015). The relationship of speech intelligibility with hearing sensitivity, cognition, and perceived hearing difficulties varies for different speech perception tests. Front Psychol, 6, 782.
    1. Heinrich A., Henshaw H., Ferguson M. A. (2016b). Only behavioral but not self-report measures of speech perception correlate with cognitive abilities. Front Psychol, 7, 576.
    1. Heinrich A., Mikkola T. M., Polku H., Törmäkangas T., Viljanen A. (2019). Hearing in real-life environments (HERE): Structure and reliability of a questionnaire on perceived hearing for older adults. Ear Hear, 40, 368–380.
    1. Henshaw H., & Ferguson M. A. (2013a). Efficacy of individual computer-based auditory training for people with hearing loss: A systematic review of the evidence. PLoS One, 8, e62836.
    1. Henshaw H., & Ferguson M. A. (2013b). Working memory training for adult hearing aid users: Study protocol for a double-blind randomized active controlled trial. Trials, 14, 417.
    1. Henshaw H., Ferguson M. A. (2014). Assessing the benefits of auditory training to real-world listening: identifying appropriate and sensitive outcomes. Dau T., Santurette S., Dalsgaard J. C., et al.. (Eds.), In: Proceedings of ISAAR 2013: Auditory Plasticity - Listening with the Brain. 4th Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research (pp. 45–52). The Danavox Jubilee Foundation.
    1. Henshaw H., McCormack A., Ferguson M. A. (2015). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation is associated with computer-based auditory training uptake, engagement, and adherence for people with hearing loss. Front Psychol, 6, 1067.
    1. Henshaw H., Sharkey L., Crowe D., Ferguson M. (2015). Research priorities for mild-to-moderate hearing loss in adults. Lancet, 386, 2140–2141.
    1. Hoffmann T. C., Glasziou P. P., Boutron I., Milne R., Perera R., Moher D., Altman D. G., Barbour V., Macdonald H., Johnston M., Lamb S. E., Dixon-Woods M., McCulloch P., Wyatt J. C., Chan A. W., Michie S. (2014). Better reporting of interventions: Template for intervention description and replication (TIDieR) checklist and guide. BMJ, 348, g1687.
    1. Hogrefe A. B., Studer-Luethi B., Kodzhabashev S., et al. . (2017). Mechanisms underlying N-back training: Response consistency during training influences training outcome. J Cogn Enhanc, 1, 406–418.
    1. Højsgaard S., Halekoh U., Yan J. (2006). The R package geepack for generalized estimating equations. J Stat Softw, 15, 1–11.
    1. Holmes J., Gathercole S. E., Dunning D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Dev Sci, 12, F9–15.
    1. Holmes J., Gathercole S. E., Place M., et al. . (2010). Working memory deficits can be overcome: Impacts of training and medication on working memory in children with ADHD. Appl Cogn Psychol, 24, 827–836.
    1. Hopkins W. G. (2000). Measures of reliability in sports medicine and science. Sports Med, 30, 1–15.
    1. Howard C. S., Munro K. J., Plack C. J. (2010). Listening effort at signal-to-noise ratios that are typical of the school classroom. Int J Audiol, 49, 928–932.
    1. Jaeggi S. M., Buschkuehl M., Jonides J., Shah P. (2011). Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 108, 10081–10086.
    1. Jaeggi S. M., Buschkuehl M., Shah P., Jonides J. (2013). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Mem Cogn, 42, 464–480.
    1. Jakobsen J. C., Gluud C., Wetterslev J., Winkel P. (2017). When and how should multiple imputation be used for handling missing data in randomised clinical trials - a practical guide with flowcharts. BMC Med Res Methodol, 17, 162.
    1. Kalikow D. N., Stevens K. N., Elliott L. L. (1977). Development of a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence materials with controlled word predictability. J Acoust Soc Am, 61, 1337–1351.
    1. Kane M. J., & Engle R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. J Exp Psychol Gen, 132, 47–70.
    1. Karbach J., & Verhaeghen P. (2014). Making working memory work: A meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychol Sci, 25, 2027–2037.
    1. Karbach J., Könen T., Spengler M. (2017). Who benefits the most? Individual differences in the transfer of executive control training across the lifespan. J Cogn Enhanc, 1, 394–405.
    1. Kiessling J., Pichora-Fuller M. K., Gatehouse S., Stephens D., Arlinger S., Chisolm T., Davis A. C., Erber N. P., Hickson L., Holmes A., Rosenhall U., von Wedel H. (2003). Candidature for and delivery of audiological services: Special needs of older people. Int J Audiol, 42(Suppl 2), 2S92–2101.
    1. Klingberg T., Fernell E., Olesen P. J., Johnson M., Gustafsson P., Dahlström K., Gillberg C. G., Forssberg H., Westerberg H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD-a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177–186.
    1. Klingberg T., Forssberg H., Westerberg H. (2002). Training of working memory in children with ADHD. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 24, 781–791.
    1. Knight S., & Heinrich A. (2017). Different measures of auditory and visual stroop interference and their relationship to speech intelligibility in noise. Front Psychol, 8, 230.
    1. Kochkin S. (2002). 10-year customer satisfaction trends in the US hearing instrument market. Hear Rev, 9, 14–25.
    1. Kronenberger W. G., Pisoni D. B., Henning S. C., Colson B. G., Hazzard L. M. (2011). Working memory training for children with cochlear implants: A pilot study. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 54, 1182–1196.
    1. Lampit A., Hallock H., Valenzuela M. (2014). Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med, 11, e1001756.
    1. Lawrence B. J., Jayakody D. M. P., Henshaw H., Ferguson M. A., Eikelboom R. H., Loftus A. M., Friedland P. L. (2018). Auditory and cognitive training for cognition in adults with hearing loss: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Trends Hear, 22, 2331216518792096.
    1. Lin F. R., Ferrucci L., Metter E. J., An Y., Zonderman A. B., Resnick S. M. (2011a). Hearing loss and cognition in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neuropsychology, 25, 763–770.
    1. Lin F. R., Metter E. J., O’Brien R. J., Resnick S. M., Zonderman A. B., Ferrucci L. (2011b). Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch Neurol, 68, 214–220.
    1. Lin F. R., Yaffe K., Xia J., Xue Q. L., Harris T. B., Purchase-Helzner E., Satterfield S., Ayonayon H. N., Ferrucci L., Simonsick E. M.; Health ABC Study Group. (2013). Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern Med, 173, 293–299.
    1. Livingston G., Huntley J., Sommerlad A., Ames D., Ballard C., Banerjee S., Brayne C., Burns A., Cohen-Mansfield J., Cooper C., Costafreda S. G., Dias A., Fox N., Gitlin L. N., Howard R., Kales H. C., Kivimäki M., Larson E. B., Ogunniyi A., Orgeta V., et al. . (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet, 396, 413–446.
    1. Livingston G., Sommerlad A., Orgeta V., Costafreda S. G., Huntley J., Ames D., Ballard C., Banerjee S., Burns A., Cohen-Mansfield J., Cooper C., Fox N., Gitlin L. N., Howard R., Kales H. C., Larson E. B., Ritchie K., Rockwood K., Sampson E. L., Samus Q., et al. . (2017). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet, 390, 2673–2734.
    1. Loughrey D. G., Kelly M. E., Kelley G. A., Brennan S., Lawlor B. A. (2018). Association of age-related hearing loss with cognitive function, cognitive impairment, and dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg, 144, 115–126.
    1. Lunner T. (2003). Cognitive function in relation to hearing aid use. Int J Audiol, 42(Suppl 1), S49–S58.
    1. Lunner T., & Sundewall-Thorén E. (2007). Interactions between cognition, compression, and listening conditions: effects on speech-in-noise performance in a two-channel hearing aid. J Am Acad Audiol, 18, 604–617.
    1. Lunner T., Rudner M., Rönnberg J. (2009). Cognition and hearing aids. Scand J Psychol, 50, 395–403.
    1. Mattys S. L., Davis M. H., Bradlow A. R., et al. . (2012). Speech recognition in adverse conditions: A review. Lang Cogn Process, 27, 953–978.
    1. McArthur G. (2007). Test-retest effects in treatment studies of reading disability: The devil is in the detail. Dyslexia, 13, 240–252.
    1. McCabe D. P., Roediger H. L., McDaniel M. A., Balota D. A., Hambrick D. Z. (2010). The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology, 24, 222–243.
    1. McNamara D. S., & Scott J. L. (2001). Working memory capacity and strategy use. Mem Cognit, 29, 10–17.
    1. Melby-Lervåg M., & Hulme C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev Psychol, 49, 270–291.
    1. Melby-Lervåg M., Redick T. S., Hulme C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer”: Evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci, 11, 512–534.
    1. Moore D. R., Edmondson-Jones M., Dawes P., Fortnum H., McCormack A., Pierzycki R. H., Munro K. J. (2014). Relation between speech-in-noise threshold, hearing loss and cognition from 40-69 years of age. PLoS One, 9, e107720.
    1. Moore D. R., Rosenberg J. F., Coleman J. S. (2005). Discrimination training of phonemic contrasts enhances phonological processing in mainstream school children. Brain Lang, 94, 72–85.
    1. Morrison A. B., & Chein J. M. (2011). Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychon Bull Rev, 18, 46–60.
    1. Morrison A. B., Chein J. M. (2012). The controversy over Cogmed. J Appl Res Mem Cogn, 1, 208–210.
    1. Nasreddine Z. S., Phillips N. A., Bédirian V., Charbonneau S., Whitehead V., Collin I., Cummings J. L., Chertkow H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc, 53, 695–699.
    1. Ng E. H., Classon E., Larsby B., Arlinger S., Lunner T., Rudner M., Rönnberg J. (2014). Dynamic relation between working memory capacity and speech recognition in noise during the first 6 months of hearing aid use. Trends Hear, 18, 2331216514558688.
    1. NICE. (2018). Hearing Loss in Adults: Assessment and Management. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence.
    1. Office for National Statistics. (2019). Population Estimates for the UK, England and Wales, Scotland and Northern Ireland: Mid-2018. Office for National Statistics.
    1. Olesen P. J., Westerberg H., Klingberg T. (2004). Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nat Neurosci, 7, 75–79.
    1. Panza F., Solfrizzi V., Seripa D., Imbimbo B. P., Capozzo R., Quaranta N., Pilotto A., Logroscino G. (2015). Age-related hearing impairment and frailty in Alzheimer’s disease: Interconnected associations and mechanisms. Front Aging Neurosci, 7, 113.
    1. Pichora-Fuller M. K. (2007). Audition and cognition: What audiologists need to know about listening. Palmer C. & Seewald R. (Eds.), In: Hearing Care for Adults. (pp. 71–85). Stäfa, Switzerland: Phonak.
    1. Pichora-Fuller M. K., Kramer S. E., Eckert M. A., Edwards B., Hornsby B. W., Humes L. E., Lemke U., Lunner T., Matthen M., Mackersie C. L., Naylor G., Phillips N. A., Richter M., Rudner M., Sommers M. S., Tremblay K. L., Wingfield A. (2016). Hearing impairment and cognitive energy: The framework for understanding effortful listening (FUEL). Ear Hear, 37, 5S–27S.
    1. Pichora-Fuller M. K., Kramer S. E., Eckert M. A., et al. . (2016). Hearing impairment and cognitive energy: The framework for understanding effortful listening (FUEL). Ear Hear, 37(Suppl 1), 5s–27s.
    1. Pichora-Fuller M. K., Phillips N. (2017). Tackling the toll of hearing loss on executive function: We need to think beyond the speech chain—and hearing aids—to fully address effects of hearing loss on older adults’ cognitive health. ASHA Leader, 22, 6–8.
    1. Redick T. S., Shipstead Z., Harrison T. L., Hicks K. L., Fried D. E., Hambrick D. Z., Kane M. J., Engle R. W. (2012). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. J Exp Psychol Gen, 142, 359–379.
    1. Richmond L. L., Morrison A. B., Chein J. M., Olson I. R. (2011). Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychol Aging, 26, 813–822.
    1. RNID. (2015). Hearing Matters. January 27, 2021. .
    1. RNID. (2021). Facts and Figures. January 27, 2021. .
    1. Robertson I. H., Ward T., Ridgeway V., Nimmo-Smith I. (1994). The Test of Everyday Attention. Thames Valley Test Company.
    1. R Core Team (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing, Version 3.3.3. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
    1. Rönnberg J., Holmer E., Rudner M. (2019). Cognitive hearing science and ease of language understanding. Int J Audiol, 58, 247–261.
    1. Rönnberg J., Lunner T., Zekveld A., Sörqvist P., Danielsson H., Lyxell B., Dahlström O., Signoret C., Stenfelt S., Pichora-Fuller M. K., Rudner M. (2013). The ease of language understanding (ELU) model: Theoretical, empirical, and clinical advances. Front Syst Neurosci, 7, 31.
    1. Rönnberg J., Rudner M., Foo C., Lunner T. (2008). Cognition counts: A working memory system for ease of language understanding (ELU). Int J Audiol, 47(Suppl 2), S99–105.
    1. Rönnberg J., Rudner M., Lunner T., Zekveld A. A. (2010). When cognition kicks in: Working memory and speech understanding in noise. Noise Health, 12, 263–269.
    1. Rönnberg J., Rudner M., Zekveld A. A. (2009). Cognitive hearing science: The role of a working memory system for speech understanding in old age. In Hickson L. (Ed.), Hearing Care for Adults. Phonak.
    1. Rudner M., & Lunner T. (2013). Cognitive spare capacity as a window on hearing aid benefit. In Seminars in Hearing (pp. 298–307). Thieme Medical Publishers.
    1. Rudner M., Lunner T., Behrens T., Thorén E. S., Rönnberg J. (2012). Working memory capacity may influence perceived effort during aided speech recognition in noise. J Am Acad Audiol, 23, 577–589.
    1. Rudner M., Rönnberg J., Lunner T. (2011). Working memory supports listening in noise for persons with hearing impairment. J Am Acad Audiol, 22, 156–167.
    1. Saunders G. H., Smith S. L., Chisolm T. H., Frederick M. T., McArdle R. A., Wilson R. H. (2016). A randomized control trial: Supplementing hearing aid use with listening and communication enhancement (LACE) auditory training. Ear Hear, 37, 381–396.
    1. Schow R., & Nerbonne M. (2006). Introduction to Audiologic Rehabilitation. (5th Edition ed.). Pearson Education.
    1. Schulz K. F., Altman D. G., Moher D. (2010). CONSORT 2010 statement: Updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. J Pharmacol Pharmacother, 1, 100–107.
    1. Scott N. W., McPherson G. C., Ramsay C. R., Campbell M. K. (2002). The method of minimization for allocation to clinical trials. A review. Control Clin Trials, 23, 662–674.
    1. Shah T. M., Weinborn M., Verdile G., Sohrabi H. R., Martins R. N. (2017). Enhancing cognitive functioning in healthly older adults: a systematic review of the clinical significance of commercially available computerized cognitive training in preventing cognitive decline. Neuropsychol Rev, 27, 62–80.
    1. Shinaver C. S., III, Entwistle P. C., Söderqvist S. (2014). Cogmed WM training: Reviewing the reviews. Appl Neuropsychol Child, 3, 163–172.
    1. Shipstead Z., Hicks K. L., Engle R. W. (2012). Cogmed working memory training: Does the evidence support the claims? J Appl Res Mem Cogn, 1, 185–193.
    1. Shipstead Z., Redick T. S., Engle R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychol Bull, 138, 628–654.
    1. Simons D. J., Boot W. R., Charness N., Gathercole S. E., Chabris C. F., Hambrick D. Z., Stine-Morrow E. A. (2016). Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol Sci Public Interest, 17, 103–186.
    1. Sörqvist P., Ljungberg J. K., Ljung R. (2010). A sub-process view of working memory capacity: Evidence from effects of speech on prose memory Memory, 18, 310–326.
    1. Strait D. L., Parbery-Clark A., Hittner E., Kraus N. (2012). Musical training during early childhood enhances the neural encoding of speech in noise. Brain Lang, 123, 191–201.
    1. Strobach T., & Huestegge L. (2017). Evaluating the effectiveness of commercial brain game training with working-memory tasks. J Cogn Enhanc, 1, 539–558.
    1. Sweetow R. W., & Sabes J. H. (2006). The need for and development of an adaptive Listening and Communication Enhancement (LACE) program. J Am Acad Audiol, 17, 538–558.
    1. Sweetow R., & Palmer C. V. (2005). Efficacy of individual auditory training in adults: A systematic review of the evidence. J Am Acad Audiol, 16, 494–504.
    1. Thorell L. B., Lindqvist S., Bergman Nutley S., Bohlin G., Klingberg T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev Sci, 12, 106–113.
    1. Tiego J., Testa R., Bellgrove M. A., Pantelis C., Whittle S. (2018). A hierarchical model of inhibitory control. Front Psychol, 9, 1339.
    1. Unsworth N., & Engle R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychol Rev, 114, 104–132.
    1. van Buuren S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. (2011). mice: Multivatiate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw, 45, 1–67.
    1. van Buuren S., Boshuizen H. C., Knook D. L. (1999). Multiple imputation of missing blood pressure covariates in survival analysis. Stat Med, 18, 681–694.
    1. Varnet L., Wang T., Peter C., Meunier F., Hoen M. (2015). How musical expertise shapes speech perception: Evidence from auditory classification images. Sci Rep, 5, 14489.
    1. Ventry I. M., & Woods R. W. (1971). Most comfortable loudness for pure tones, noise, and speech. J Acoust Soc Am, 49, 1805–1813.
    1. von Bastian C. C., & Oberauer K. (2014). Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: A review. Psychol Res, 78, 803–820.
    1. von Bastian C. C., Blais C., Brewer G., et al. . (2020). Advancing the understanding of individual differences in attentional control: Theoretical, methodological, and analytical considerations. PsyArXiv: .
    1. Vos T., Barber R. M., Bell B., et al. . (2015). Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 301 acute and chronic diseases and injuries in 188 countries, 1990–2013: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet, 386, 743–800.
    1. Wayne R. V., & Johnsrude I. S. (2015). A review of causal mechanisms underlying the link between age-related hearing loss and cognitive decline. Ageing Res Rev, 23(Pt B), 154–166.
    1. Wayne R. V., Hamilton C., Jones Huyck J., Johnsrude I. S. (2016). Working memory training and speech in noise comprehension in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci, 8, 49.
    1. Wechsler D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Third Edition. The Psychological Corporation.
    1. Weir J. P. (2005). Quantifying test-retest reliability using the intraclass correlation coefficient and the SEM. J Strength Cond Res, 19, 231–240.
    1. Westerberg H., & Klingberg T. (2007). Changes in cortical activity after training of working memory–a single-subject analysis. Physiol Behav, 92, 186–192.
    1. Whitmer W. M., Howell P., Akeroyd M. A. (2014). Proposed norms for the Glasgow hearing-aid benefit profile (Ghabp) questionnaire. Int J Audiol, 53, 345–351.
    1. Wickham H. Francois R. Henry L. & Müller K. (2017). dplyr: A Grammar of Data Manipulation: RStudio.
    1. Wilson B. S., Tucci D. L., O’Donoghue G.M., Merson M. H., Frankish H. (2019). A Lancet Commission to address the global burden of hearing loss. Lancet, 393, 2106–2108.
    1. World Health Organization. (2017). Global costs of unaddressed hearing loss and cost-effectiveness of interventions: a WHO report. Geneva: World Health Organization.
    1. World Health Organization. (2018). Addressing the Rising Prevalence of Hearing Loss. World Health Organization.
    1. World Health Organization. (2019). Risk Reduction of Cognitive Decline and Dementia: WHO Guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.
    1. Wright B. A., & Zhang Y. (2009). A review of the generalization of auditory learning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 364, 301–311.
    1. Zeger S. L., Liang K. Y., Albert P. S. (1988). Models for longitudinal data: A generalized estimating equation approach. Biometrics, 44, 1049–1060.
    1. Zekveld A. A., Rudner M., Johnsrude I. S., Heslenfeld D. J., Rönnberg J. (2012). Behavioral and fMRI evidence that cognitive ability modulates the effect of semantic context on speech intelligibility. Brain Lang, 122, 103–113.
    1. Zelinski E. M. (2009). Far transfer in cognitive training of older adults. Restor Neurol Neurosci, 27, 455–471.
    1. Zhang Y. X., Barry J. G., Moore D. R., Amitay S. (2012). A new test of attention in listening (TAIL) predicts auditory performance. PLoS One, 7, e53502.

Source: PubMed

3
購読する