ERP characterization of sustained attention effects in visual lexical categorization

Clara D Martin, Guillaume Thierry, Jean-François Démonet, Clara D Martin, Guillaume Thierry, Jean-François Démonet

Abstract

As our understanding of the basic processes underlying reading is growing, the key role played by attention in this process becomes evident. Two research topics are of particular interest in this domain: (1) it is still undetermined whether sustained attention affects lexical decision tasks; (2) the influence of attention on early visual processing (i.e., before orthographic or lexico-semantic processing stages) remains largely under-specified. Here we investigated early perceptual modulations by sustained attention using an ERP paradigm adapted from Thierry et al. [1]. Participants had to decide whether visual stimuli presented in pairs pertained to a pre-specified category (lexical categorization focus on word or pseudoword pairs). Depending on the lexical category of the first item of a pair, participants either needed to fully process the second item (hold condition) or could release their attention and make a decision without full processing of the second item (release condition). The P1 peak was unaffected by sustained attention. The N1 was delayed and reduced after the second item of a pair when participants released their attention. Release of sustained attention also reduced a P3 wave elicited by the first item of a pair and abolished the P3 wave elicited by the second. Our results are consistent with differential effects of sustained attention on early processing stages and working memory. Sustained attention modulated early processing stages during a lexical decision task without inhibiting the process of stimulus integration. On the contrary, working memory involvement/updating was highly dependent upon the allocation of sustained attention. Moreover, the influence of sustained attention on both early and late cognitive processes was independent of lexical categorization focus.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Behavioral results.
Figure 1. Behavioral results.
W refers to Words, P to pseudowords (lexical category under focus), H to Hold and R to Release (sustained attention). Mean reaction times are depicted by histograms and mean error rates are depicted by circles. Error bars indicate standard errors. The reference time for reaction time values is the onset of the stimulus of the pair which has induced the participant's answer, i.e. the onset of the first stimulus of the pair in the ‘release’ condition and the onset of the second stimulus in the ‘hold’ condition.
Figure 2. ERP results, general overview.
Figure 2. ERP results, general overview.
Event-related potential results over nine major scalp regions (Left Frontal, electrodes F3, F5, FC3, FC5, FT7; FrontoCentral, F1, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2; Right Frontal, F4, F6, FC4, FC6, FT8; Left Temporal, C3, C5, CP3, CP5, TP7; ParietoCentral, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CP2; Right Temporal, C4, C6, CP4, CP6, TP8; Left ParietoOccipital, P3, P5, PO3, PO7, O1; ParietoOccipital, P1, Pz, P2, POz, Oz; Right ParietoOccipital, P4, P6, PO4, PO8, O2). a. Lexical categorization focus on word pairs. b. Lexical categorization focus on pseudoword pairs. ERPs for hold (full line) and release (dotted line) sustained attention conditions. Vertical bars on graphs indicate the onset of the first and second item of a pair.
Figure 3. Event-related potential results for the…
Figure 3. Event-related potential results for the first item of a pair.
ERPs measured over parietooccipital region (PO3, PO4, O1, O2, P5, P6, PO7, PO8) for lexical categorization focus and sustained attention conditions (W-hold = words under focus, hold condition; W-release = words under focus, release condition; P-hold = pseudowords under focus, hold condition; P-release = pseudowords under focus, release condition). Rectangles on the time axis indicate the onset and duration of the first and second item of each pair. Vertical bars indicate reaction times.
Figure 4. Event-related potential results for the…
Figure 4. Event-related potential results for the first item of a pair.
ERPs measured over centroparietal region (C1, C2, Cz, CP1, CP2, CPz, P1, P2) for lexical categorization focus and sustained attention conditions. Rectangles on the time axis indicate the onset and duration of the first and second item of each pair. Vertical bars indicate reaction times. Topographies labelled with stars are significantly different.
Figure 5. Event-related potential results elicited by…
Figure 5. Event-related potential results elicited by the second item of a pair.
ERPs measured over parietooccipital region (PO3, PO4, O1, O2, P5, P6, PO7, PO8) for lexical categorization focus and sustained attention conditions. Rectangles on the time axis indicate the onset time and duration of the first and second item of each pair. Vertical bars indicate reaction times. Topographies labelled with stars are significantly different. Time scale and ERP baseline correction were recalculated in reference to the onset of the second item.
Figure 6. Event-related potential results for the…
Figure 6. Event-related potential results for the second item of a pair.
ERPs measured over centroparietal region (C1, C2, Cz, CP1, CP2, CPz, P1, P2) for lexical categorization focus and sustained attention conditions. Rectangles on the time axis indicate the onset time and duration of the first and second item of each pair. Vertical bars indicate reaction times. Topographies labelled with stars are significantly different. Time scale and ERP baseline correction were recalculated in reference to the onset of the second item.
Figure 7. ERP results, summary.
Figure 7. ERP results, summary.
Mean amplitudes, mean latencies and standard errors associated with each ERP event in the four experimental conditions (Lexical categorization focus on words or pseudowords; Hold or released sustained attention).
Figure 8. Experimental design: Example of one…
Figure 8. Experimental design: Example of one trial.

References

    1. Thierry G, Cardebat D, Demonet JF. Electrophysiological comparison of grammatical processing and semantic processing of single spoken nouns. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003;17:535–547.
    1. McClelland JL, Rumelhart DE. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review. 1981;88:375–407.
    1. Coltheart M, Rastle K, Perry C, Langdon R, Ziegler J. DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol Rev. 2001;108:204–256.
    1. Rabovsky M, Alvarez CJ, Hohlfeld A, Sommer W. Is lexical access autonomous? Evidence from combining overlapping tasks with recording event-related brain potentials. Brain Res. 2008;1222:156–165.
    1. Telford CW. The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. J Exp Psychol. 1931;14:1–36.
    1. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–662.
    1. Lien MC, Allen PA, Ruthruff E, Grabbe J, McCann RS, et al. Visual word recognition without central attention: evidence for greater automaticity with advancing age. Psychol Aging. 2006;21:431–447.
    1. McCann RS, Remington RW, Van Selst M. A dual-task investigation of automaticity in visual word processing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2000;26:1352–1370.
    1. O'Malley S, Reynolds MG, Stolz JA, Besner D. Reading aloud: spelling-sound translation uses central attention. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008;34:422–429.
    1. Thierry G, Doyon B, Demonet JF. ERP mapping in phonological and lexical semantic monitoring tasks: A study complementing previous PET results. Neuroimage. 1998;8:391–408.
    1. Coull JT. Neural correlates of attention and arousal: insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology. Prog Neurobiol. 1998;55:343–361.
    1. Shulman GL, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Raichle ME, Fiez JA, et al. Top-down modulation of early sensory cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7:193–206.
    1. Paus T, Zatorre RJ, Hofle N, Caramanos Z, Gotman J, et al. Time-related changes in neural systems underlying attention and arousal during the performance of an auditory vigilance task. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9:392–408.
    1. Ruchkin DS, Johnson R, Jr, Mahaffey D, Sutton S. Toward a functional categorization of slow waves. Psychophysiology. 1988;25:339–353.
    1. Johnson R., Jr . The amplitude of the P300 component of the event-related potential: review and synthesis. In: Ackles PK, Jennings JR, Coles MGH, editors. Advances in Psychophysiology. Greenwich: JAI Press; 1988. pp. 69–138.
    1. Schupp HT, Flaisch T, Stockburger J, Junghofer M. Emotion and attention: event-related brain potential studies. Prog Brain Res. 2006;156:31–51.
    1. Thierry G, Ibarrola D, Demonet JF, Cardebat D. Demand on verbal working memory delays haemodynamic response in the inferior prefrontal cortex. Hum Brain Mapp. 2003;19:37–46.
    1. Luck SJ, Woodman GF, Vogel EK. Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:432–440.
    1. Saalmann YB, Pigarev IN, Vidyasagar TR. Neural mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant locations. Science. 2007;316:1612–1615.
    1. Kanwisher N, Wojciulik E. Visual attention: insights from brain imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000;1:91–100.
    1. Rosazza C, Cai Q, Minati L, Paulignan Y, Nazir T. Early involvement of dorsal and ventral pathways in visual word recognition: an ERP study. Brain Res. 2009;1272:32–44.
    1. Eason R, Harter M, White C. Effects of attention and arousal on visually evoked cortical potentials and reaction time in man. Physiol Behav. 1969;4:283–289.
    1. Hillyard SA, Hink RF, Schwent VL, Picton TW. Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science. 1973;182:177–180.
    1. Hillyard SA, Picton TW. Plum F, editor. Electrophysiology of cognition. Handbook of Physiology: Section 1 The nervous system: Waverly Press. 1987. pp. 519–584.
    1. Martin CD, Nazir T, Thierry G, Paulignan Y, Demonet JF. Perceptual and lexical effects in letter identification: an event-related potential study of the word superiority effect. Brain Res. 2006;1098:153–160.
    1. Doallo S, Lorenzo-Lopez L, Vizoso C, Rodriguez Holguin S, Amenedo E, et al. The time course of the effects of central and peripheral cues on visual processing: an event-related potentials study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115:199–210.
    1. Correa A, Lupianez J, Madrid E, Tudela P. Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: a review and new evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Res. 2006;1076:116–128.
    1. Donchin E. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!…Surprise? Psychophysiology. 1981;18:493–513.
    1. Croft RJ, Gonsalvez CJ, Gabriel C, Barry RJ. Target-to-target interval versus probability effects on P300 in one- and two-tone tasks. Psychophysiology. 2003;40:322–328.
    1. Luck SJ. Sources of dual-task interference: evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychol Sci. 1998;9:223–227.
    1. Raymond JE, Shapiro KL, Arnell KM. Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1992;18:849–860.
    1. Vogel EK, Luck SJ, Shapiro KL. Electrophysiological evidence for a postperceptual locus of suppression during the attentional blink. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1998;24:1656–1674.
    1. Luck SJ, Vogel EK, Shapiro KL. Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink. Nature. 1996;383:616–618.
    1. Chun MM, Potter MC. A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1995;21:109–127.
    1. Shapiro KL, Arnell KM, Raymond JE. The attentional blink: A view on attention and a glimpse on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science. 1997;1:291–295.
    1. Alho K, Tottola K, Reinikainen K, Sams M, Naatanen R. Brain mechanism of selective listening reflected by event-related potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1987;68:458–470.
    1. Näätänen R. Event-related potentials in research of cognitive processes - A classification of components. In: Van der Meer E, Hoffman J, editors. Knowledge-Aided Information Processing. North Holland, Amsterdam; 1987. pp. 241–273.
    1. McEvoy LK, Smith ME, Gevins A. Dynamic cortical networks of verbal and spatial working memory: effects of memory load and task practice. Cereb Cortex. 1998;8:563–574.
    1. Rugg MD. Event-related potentials in phonological matching tasks. Brain Lang. 1984;23:225–240.
    1. Näätänen R, Teder W, Alho K, Lavikainen J. Auditory attention and selective input modulation: a topographical ERP study. Neuroreport. 1992;3:493–496.
    1. Walter WG, Cooper R, Aldridge VJ, McCallum WC, Winter AL. Contingent Negative Variation: An Electric Sign of Sensorimotor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain. Nature. 1964;203:380–384.
    1. Douros C, Karrer R, Rosenfeld JP. Effects of attention and slow potential shifts on self-regulation of event-related potentials. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1987;12:39–49.
    1. Weisz N, Schandry R, Jacobs AM, Mialet JP, Duschek S. Early contingent negative variation of the EEG and attentional flexibility are reduced in hypotension. Int J Psychophysiol. 2002;45:253–260.
    1. Gomez CM, Marco J, Grau C. Preparatory visuo-motor cortical network of the contingent negative variation estimated by current density. Neuroimage. 2003;20:216–224.
    1. Curry SH. Contingent negative variation and slow waves in a short interstimulus interval go-nogo task situation. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1984;425:171–176.
    1. Rugg MD, Cowan CP, Nagy ME, Milner AD, Jacobson I, et al. CNV abnormalities following closed head injury. Brain. 1989;112(Pt 2):489–506.
    1. Rosahl SK, Knight RT. Role of prefrontal cortex in generation of the contingent negative variation. Cereb Cortex. 1995;5:123–134.
    1. Filipovic SR, Jahanshahi M, Rothwell JC. Uncoupling of contingent negative variation and alpha band event-related desynchronization in a go/no-go task. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112:1307–1315.
    1. Tecce JT, Cattanach L. Contingent negative variation (CNV). In: Niedermeyer E, Lopes da Silva FH, editors. Electroencephalography: basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins; 1993. pp. 887–910.
    1. Content AM, P. Radeau M. BRULEX: une base de données lexicales informatisée pour le français écrit et parlé. L'Année Psychologique. 1990;90:551.
    1. O'Regan JK, Levy-Schoen A, Pynte J, Brugaillere B. Convenient fixation location within isolated words of different length and structure. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1984;10:250–257.
    1. Nazir TA. On the role of refixations in letter strings: the influence of oculomotor factors. Perception & Psychophysics. 1991;49:373–389.
    1. Greenhouse SW, Geisser S. On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika. 1959;24:95–112.
    1. Picton TW, Bentin S, Berg P, Donchin E, Hillyard SA, et al. Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:127–152.

Source: PubMed

3
購読する