Low-dose oxytocin delivered intranasally with Breath Powered device affects social-cognitive behavior: a randomized four-way crossover trial with nasal cavity dimension assessment

D S Quintana, L T Westlye, Ø G Rustan, N Tesli, C L Poppy, H Smevik, M Tesli, M Røine, R A Mahmoud, K T Smerud, P G Djupesland, O A Andreassen, D S Quintana, L T Westlye, Ø G Rustan, N Tesli, C L Poppy, H Smevik, M Tesli, M Røine, R A Mahmoud, K T Smerud, P G Djupesland, O A Andreassen

Abstract

Despite the promise of intranasal oxytocin (OT) for modulating social behavior, recent work has provided mixed results. This may relate to suboptimal drug deposition achieved with conventional nasal sprays, inter-individual differences in nasal physiology and a poor understanding of how intranasal OT is delivered to the brain in humans. Delivering OT using a novel 'Breath Powered' nasal device previously shown to enhance deposition in intranasal sites targeted for nose-to-brain transport, we evaluated dose-dependent effects on social cognition, compared response with intravenous (IV) administration of OT, and assessed nasal cavity dimensions using acoustic rhinometry. We adopted a randomized, double-blind, double-dummy, crossover design, with 16 healthy male adults completing four single-dose treatments (intranasal 8 IU (international units) or 24 IU OT, 1 IU OT IV and placebo). The primary outcome was social cognition measured by emotional ratings of facial images. Secondary outcomes included the pharmacokinetics of OT, vasopressin and cortisol in blood and the association between nasal cavity dimensions and emotional ratings. Despite the fact that all the treatments produced similar plasma OT increases compared with placebo, there was a main effect of treatment on anger ratings of emotionally ambiguous faces. Pairwise comparisons revealed decreased ratings after 8 IU OT in comparison to both placebo and 24 IU OT. In addition, there was an inverse relationship between nasal valve dimensions and anger ratings of ambiguous faces after 8-IU OT treatment. These findings provide support for a direct nose-to-brain effect, independent of blood absorption, of low-dose OT delivered from a Breath Powered device.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT01983514.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean emotional ratings by stimulus and treatment. Angry ratings of emotionally ambiguous faces were reduced after the administration of 8 IU OPN-OT in comparison with placebo and 24 IU OPN-OT (a; FDR correction applied, q=0.05, revised critical value of P<0.017 for post hoc comparisons). There were no main effects for any of the other evaluation categories (bf). Emotion ratings can theoretically range from 1 to 5 and error bars represent standard error of the mean. *P<0.05. **P<0.01. FDR, false discovery rate; IU, international unit; IV, intravenous; OPN-OT, OT delivered with the Breath Powered OptiNose device; OT, oxytocin.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Percentage reduction in anger ratings was greater when presented with ambiguous faces compared with non-ambiguous faces (with corresponding or conflicting cues) after 8 IU OPN-OT compared with both placebo (a) and 24 IU OPN-OT (b) treatments (FDR correction applied, q=0.05, revised critical value of P<0.025 for post hoc comparisons). Ambiguous indicates anger ratings of ambiguous faces; NA-corresponding indicates anger ratings of non-ambiguous faces with corresponding cues; NA-conflicting indicates anger ratings of non-ambiguous faces with conflicting cues. Error bars represent s.e.m. *P<0.05. FDR, false discovery rate; IU, international unit; IV, intravenous; OPN-OT, OT delivered with the Breath Powered OptiNose device; OT, oxytocin.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Pharmacokinetics of plasma OT (a) and AVP (b) after the administration of 8 IU OPN-OT, 24 IU OPN-OT, IV OT and placebo. Error bars represent s.e.m. IU, international unit; IV, intravenous; OPN-OT, OT delivered with the Breath Powered OptiNose device; OT, oxytocin.
Figure 4
Figure 4
The relationship between angry ratings of ambiguous faces after 8 IU OT treatment and bilaterally summed mean cross-sectional areas in cm2 of the nasal valves with best-fit line and 95% confidence band. The significant inverse relationship indicates that individuals with wider nasal valves rate ambiguous faces as less angry after 8 IU OPN-OT administration.

References

    1. Guastella AJ, MacLeod C. A critical review of the influence of oxytocin nasal spray on social cognition in humans: evidence and future directions. Horm Behav. 2012;61:410–418.
    1. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:524–538.
    1. Striepens N, Kendrick KM, Maier W, Hurlemann R. Prosocial effects of oxytocin and clinical evidence for its therapeutic potential. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2011;32:426–450.
    1. Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Hollander E, Ludwig NN, Kolevzon A, et al. Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy. Psychol Sci. 2010;21:1426–1428.
    1. Hurlemann R, Patin A, Onur OA, Cohen MX, Baumgartner T, Metzler S, et al. Oxytocin enhances amygdala-dependent, socially reinforced learning and emotional empathy in humans. J Neurosci. 2010;30:4999–5007.
    1. Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature. 2005;435:673–676.
    1. Shalvi S, De Dreu CK. Oxytocin promotes group-serving dishonesty. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:5503–5507.
    1. Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. A sniff of trust: meta-analysis of the effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on face recognition, trust to in-group, and trust to out-group. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:438–443.
    1. Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Dadds MR. Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:3–5.
    1. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A, Berger C, Herpertz SC. Oxytocin Improves “Mind-Reading” in Humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:731–733.
    1. Guastella AJ, Einfeld SL, Gray KM, Rinehart NJ, Tonge BJ, Lambert TJ, et al. Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:692–694.
    1. Modi ME, Young LJ. The oxytocin system in drug discovery for autism: animal models and novel therapeutic strategies. Horm Behav. 2012;61:340–350.
    1. MacDonald K, Feifel D. Oxytocin in schizophrenia: a review of evidence for its therapeutic effects. Acta Neuropsychiatrica. 2012;24:130–146.
    1. Dadds MR, MacDonald E, Cauchi A, Williams K, Levy F, Brennan J. Nasal oxytocin for social deficits in childhood autism: A randomized controlled trial. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:521–531.
    1. Guastella AJ, Hickie IB, McGuinness MM, Otis M, Woods EA, Disinger HM, et al. Recommendations for the standardisation of oxytocin nasal administration and guidelines for its reporting in human research. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:612–625.
    1. Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Ochsner KN. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:301–309.
    1. MacDonald K, Feifel D. Helping oxytocin deliver: considerations in the development of oxytocin-based therapeutics for brain disorders. Front Neurosci. 2013;7:1–21.
    1. Quintana DS, Alvares GA, Hickie IB, Guastella AJ. Do delivery routes of intranasally administered oxytocin account for observed effects on social cognition and behavior? A two-level model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;49:182–192.
    1. Landgraf R, Neumann ID. Vasopressin and oxytocin release within the brain: a dynamic concept of multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide communication. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2004;25:150–176.
    1. Iliff JJ, Wang M, Liao Y, Plogg BA, Peng W, Gundersen GA, et al. A paravascular pathway facilitates CSF flow through the brain parenchyma and the clearance of interstitial solutes, including amyloid β. Sci Transl Med. 2012;4:147ra111–147ra111.
    1. Dhuria SV, Hanson LR, Frey WH., II Intranasal delivery to the central nervous system: Mechanisms and experimental considerations. J Pharm Sci. 2009;99:1654–1673.
    1. Ermisch A, Barth T, Rühle H, Skopkova J, Hrbas P, Landgraf R. On the blood-brain barrier to peptides: accumulation of labelled vasopressin, DesGlyNH2-vasopressin and oxytocin by brain regions. Endocrinol Exp. 1985;19:29–37.
    1. Djupesland PG, Messina JC, Mahmoud RA. The nasal approach to delivering treatment for brain diseases: an anatomic, physiologic, and delivery technology overview. Ther Deliv. 2014;5:709–733.
    1. Cole P. The four components of the nasal valve. Am J Rhinol. 2003;17:107–110.
    1. Aggarwal R, Cardozo A, Homer J. The assessment of topical nasal drug distribution. Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci. 2004;29:201–205.
    1. Djupesland PG, Skretting A, Winderen M, Holand T. Breath actuated device improves delivery to target sites beyond the nasal valve. Laryngoscope. 2006;116:466–472.
    1. Djupesland PG, Messina JC, Mahmoud RA. Breath powered nasal delivery: a new route to rapid headache relief. Headache. 2013;53:72–84.
    1. Eccles R. Nasal airflow in health and disease. Acta Otolaryngol. 2000;120:580–595.
    1. Merkus P, Ebbens FA, Muller B, Fokkens WJ. Influence of anatomy and head position on intranasal drug deposition. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2006;263:827–832.
    1. Djupesland PG, Mahmoud RA, Messina JC. Accessing the brain: the nose may know the way. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2013;33:793–794.
    1. Djupesland PG, Skretting A. Nasal deposition and clearance in man: comparison of a bidirectional powder device and a traditional liquid spray pump. J Aerosol Med Pulm Drug Deliv. 2012;25:280–289.
    1. Djupesland PG. Nasal drug delivery devices: characteristics and performance in a clinical perspective—a review. Drug Deliv Transl Res. 2012;3:42–62.
    1. Hollander E, Novotny S, Hanratty M, Yaffe R, DeCaria CM, Aronowitz BR, et al. Oxytocin infusion reduces repetitive behaviors in adults with autistic and Asperger's disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:193–198.
    1. Hollander E, Bartz J, Chaplin W, Phillips A, Sumner J, Soorya L, et al. Oxytocin increases retention of social cognition in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:498–503.
    1. Striepens N, Kendrick KM, Hanking V, Landgraf R, Wüllner U, Maier W, et al. Elevated cerebrospinal fluid and blood concentrations of oxytocin following its intranasal administration in humans. Sci Rep. 2013;3:3440.
    1. MacDonald E, Dadds MR, Brennan JL, Williams K, Levy F, Cauchi AJ. A review of safety, side-effects and subjective reactions to intranasal oxytocin in human research. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:1114–1126.
    1. Bakermans-Kranenburg M, Van Ijzendoorn M. Sniffing around oxytocin: review and meta-analyses of trials in healthy and clinical groups with implications for pharmacotherapy. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3:e258.
    1. de Oliveira DC, Zuardi AW, Graeff FG, Queiroz RH, Crippa JA. Anxiolytic-like effect of oxytocin in the simulated public speaking test. J Psychopharmacol. 2012;26:497–504.
    1. Butwick A, Coleman L, Cohen S, Riley E, Carvalho B. Minimum effective bolus dose of oxytocin during elective Caesarean delivery. Br J Anaesth. 2010;104:338–343.
    1. Rault J-L, Carter CS, Garner JP, Marchant-Forde JN, Richert BT, Lay DC., Jr Repeated intranasal oxytocin administration in early life dysregulates the HPA axis and alters social behavior. Physiol Behav. 2013;112-113:40–48.
    1. Gimpl G, Fahrenholz F. The oxytocin receptor system: structure, function, and regulation. Physiol Rev. 2001;81:629–683.
    1. Mayer-Hubner B. Pseudotumour cerebri from intranasal oxytocin and excessive fluid intake. Lancet. 1996;347:623–623.
    1. Kanat M, Heinrichs M, Schwarzwald R, Domes G. Oxytocin attenuates neural reactivity to masked threat cues from the eyes. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:287–295.
    1. Evans S, Shergill SS, Averbeck BB. Oxytocin decreases aversion to angry faces in an associative learning task. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:2502–2509.
    1. Domes G, Steiner A, Porges SW, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin differentially modulates eye gaze to naturalistic social signals of happiness and anger. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:1198–1202.
    1. Jesso S, Morlog D, Ross S, Pell MD, Pasternak SH, Mitchell DG, et al. The effects of oxytocin on social cognition and behaviour in frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2011;134:2493–2501.
    1. Bertsch K, Gamer M, Schmidt B, Schmidinger I, Walther S, Kästel T, et al. Oxytocin and reduction of social threat hypersensitivity in women with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170:1169–1177.
    1. MacDonald K, Feifel D. Oxytocin's role in anxiety: a critical appraisal. Brain Res. 2014;1580:22–56.
    1. Neumann ID, Landgraf R. Balance of brain oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for anxiety, depression, and social behaviors. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:649–659.
    1. Legros J, Chiodera P, Geenen V, Smitz S, von Frenckell R. Dose-response relationship between plasma oxytocin and cortisol and adrenocorticotropin concentrations during oxytocin infusion in normal men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1984;58:105–109.
    1. Neumann ID. Involvement of the brain oxytocin system in stress coping: interactions with the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Prog Brain Res. 2002;139:147–162.
    1. Wechsler D. Weschsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA; 1999.
    1. Lecrubier Y, Sheehan D, Weiller E, Amorim P, Bonora I, Harnett Sheehan K, et al. The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI. Eur Psychiatry. 1997;12:224–231.
    1. Spielberger CD. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (form Y)("self-evaluation questionnaire") Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA; 1983.
    1. Leknes S, Wessberg J, Ellingsen DM, Chelnokova O, Olausson H, Laeng B. Oxytocin enhances pupil dilation and sensitivity to 'hidden' emotional expressions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012;8:741–749.
    1. Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A. The Karolinska directed emotional faces (KDEF) CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet. 1998. pp. 91–630.
    1. McCullough ME, Churchland PS, Mendez AJ. Problems with measuring peripheral oxytocin: can the data on oxytocin and human behavior be trusted. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:1485–1492.
    1. Hamer R, Simpson P. Last observation carried forward versus mixed models in the analysis of psychiatric clinical trials. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:639–641.
    1. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B. 1995;57:289–300.
    1. Wetzels R, Wagenmakers E-J. A default Bayesian hypothesis test for correlations and partial correlations. Psychon Bull Rev. 2012;19:1057–1064.
    1. Zou GY. Toward using confidence intervals to compare correlations. Psychol Methods. 2007;12:399.
    1. Raghunathan T, Rosenthal R, Rubin DB. Comparing correlated but nonoverlapping correlations. Psychol Methods. 1996;1:178.
    1. Dienes Z. Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1–17.
    1. Theodoridou A, Rowe AC, Penton-Voak IS, Rogers PJ. Oxytocin and social perception: oxytocin increases perceived facial trustworthiness and attractiveness. Horm Behav. 2009;56:128–132.
    1. Van IJzendoorn MH, Bhandari R, Van der Veen R, Grewen KM, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Elevated salivary levels of oxytocin persist more than 7h after intranasal administration. Front Neurosci. 2012;6:1–6.
    1. Cardoso C, Ellenbogen MA, Orlando MA, Bacon SL, Joober R. Intranasal oxytocin attenuates the cortisol response to physical stress: a dose–response study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:399–407.
    1. Hall SS, Lightbody AA, McCarthy BE, Parker KJ, Reiss AL. Effects of intranasal oxytocin on social anxiety in males with fragile X syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:509–518.
    1. Bales KL, Perkeybile AM, Conley OG, Lee MH, Guoynes CD, Downing GM, et al. Chronic intranasal oxytocin causes long-term impairments in partner preference formation in male prairie voles. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74:180–188.
    1. Benelli A, Bertolini A, Poggioli R, Menozzi B, Basaglia R, Arletti R. Polymodal dose-response curve for oxytocin in the social recognition test. Neuropeptides. 1995;28:251–255.
    1. Popik P, Vetulani J, Van Ree JM. Low doses of oxytocin facilitate social recognition in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1992;106:71–74.
    1. Bloom DE, Cafiero E, Jané-Llopis E, Abrahams-Gessel S, Bloom LR, Fathima S, et al. The Global Economic Burden of Noncommunicable Diseases: Program on the Global Demography of Aging, 2012.
    1. Miller G. Is pharma running out of brainy ideas. Science. 2010;329:502–504.
    1. Abbott A. Novartis to shut brain research facility. Nature. 2011;480:161–162.
    1. Frank E, Landgraf R. The vasopressin system—from antidiuresis to psychopathology. Eur J Pharmacol. 2008;583:226–242.
    1. Li C, Wang W, Summer SN, Westfall TD, Brooks DP, Falk S, et al. Molecular mechanisms of antidiuretic effect of oxytocin. J Am Soc Nephrol. 2008;19:225–232.
    1. Weisman O, Schneiderman I, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Salivary vasopressin increases following intranasal oxytocin administration. Peptides. 2013;40:99–103.
    1. Burri A, Heinrichs M, Schedlowski M, Kruger TH. The acute effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on endocrine and sexual function in males. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33:591–600.
    1. Gossen A, Hahn A, Westphal L, Prinz S, Schultz R, Gründer G, et al. Oxytocin plasma concentrations after single intranasal oxytocin administration–A study in healthy men. Neuropeptides. 2012;46:211–215.
    1. Charlton S, Davis S, Illum L. Nasal administration of an angiotensin antagonist in the rat model: effect of bioadhesive formulations on the distribution of drugs to the systemic and central nervous systems. Int J Pharm. 2007;338:94–103.
    1. Dale O, Nilsen T, Loftsson T, Tønnesen HH, Klepstad P, Kaasa S, et al. Intranasal midazolam: a comparison of two delivery devices in human volunteers. J Pharm Pharmacol. 2006;58:1311–1318.
    1. Shahrestani S, Kemp AH, Guastella AJ. The impact of a single administration of intranasal oxytocin on the recognition of basic emotions in humans: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:1929–1936.
    1. Guastella AJ, Howard AL, Dadds MR, Mitchell P, Carson DS. A randomized controlled trial of intranasal oxytocin as an adjunct to exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:917–923.
    1. Elford RC, Nathan PJ, Auyeung B, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Sule A, et al. Effects of oxytocin on attention to emotional faces in healthy volunteers and highly socially anxious males. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;18:1–11.
    1. Alvares GA, Chen NTM, Balleine BW, Hickie IB, Guastella AJ. Oxytocin selectively moderates negative cognitive appraisals in high trait anxious males. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:2022–2031.

Source: PubMed

3
購読する