Can motivation normalize working memory and task persistence in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? The effects of money and computer-gaming

Sebastiaan Dovis, Saskia Van der Oord, Reinout W Wiers, Pier J M Prins, Sebastiaan Dovis, Saskia Van der Oord, Reinout W Wiers, Pier J M Prins

Abstract

Visual-spatial Working Memory (WM) is the most impaired executive function in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Some suggest that deficits in executive functioning are caused by motivational deficits. However, there are no studies that investigate the effects of motivation on the visual-spatial WM of children with- and without ADHD. Studies examining this in executive functions other than WM, show inconsistent results. These inconsistencies may be related to differences in the reinforcement used. The effects of different reinforcers on WM performance were investigated in 30 children with ADHD and 31 non-ADHD controls. A visual-spatial WM task was administered in four reinforcement conditions: Feedback-only, 1 euro, 10 euros, and a computer-game version of the task. In the Feedback-only condition, children with ADHD performed worse on the WM measure than controls. Although incentives significantly improved the WM performance of children with ADHD, even the strongest incentives (10 euros and Gaming) were unable to normalize their performance. Feedback-only provided sufficient reinforcement for controls to reach optimal performance, while children with ADHD required extra reinforcement. Only children with ADHD showed a decrease in performance over time. Importantly, the strongest incentives (10 euros and Gaming) normalized persistence of performance in these children, whereas 1 euro had no such effect. Both executive and motivational deficits give rise to visual-spatial WM deficits in ADHD. Problems with task-persistence in ADHD result from motivational deficits. In ADHD-reinforcement studies and clinical practice (e.g., assessment), reinforcement intensity can be a confounding factor and should be taken into account. Gaming can be a cost-effective way to maximize performance in ADHD.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
A trial on the Chessboardtask. a To start a trial the arrowhead-button in the bottom-right corner of the screen has to be clicked. b Then the focus screen (a black screen with a little white cross) is presented. c Subsequently, a sequence of stimuli (squares that light up) is presented one by one on a 4 × 4 grid with green and blue squares ordered in a chessboard formation. Each stimulus lights up for 900 ms and is followed by an inter-stimulus interval of 500 ms. d After the stimulus-sequence is presented the participant responds by mouse-clicking on the squares. To respond correctly the presented stimuli have to be reproduced in a reorganized way: The green stimuli have to be reproduced before the blue stimuli; both in the same order as presented (the numbers in picture d show an example of a correct reorganization). e After a response feedback is presented. (A) After feedback-presentation, the participant can start the next trial by clicking on the arrowhead button
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
a Visual feedback in the Feedback-Only (FO) and monetary conditions b A trial in the game condition. The Megabot stands on the left, the enemies to the right
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Mean performance of children with ADHD and control children on the visual-spatial working memory (WM) task in the Feedback-only (FO), 1 euro, 10 euros, and Game condition
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Mean performance over time of children with ADHD, and control children on the visual-spatial working memory (WM) task in the Feedback-only (FO), 1 euro, 10 euros, and Game condition

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th revised edition (DSM-IV-TR) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.
    1. Barber MA, Milich R, Welsh R. Effects of reinforcement schedule and task difficulty on the performance of attention deficit hyperactivity disordered and control boys. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1996;25:66–76. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2501_8.
    1. Barkley RA. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. A Handbook for diagnosis and treatment. 3. New York: Guilford; 2006.
    1. Carlson CL, Tamm L. Responsiveness of children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder to reward and response cost: differential impact on performance and motivation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:73–83. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.1.73.
    1. Carlson CL, Mann M, Alexander DK. Effects of reward and response cost on the performance and motivation of children with AD/HD. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2000;24:87–98. doi: 10.1023/A:1005455009154.
    1. Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:117–23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011.
    1. Corsi PM. Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Dissertation Abstracts International. 1972;34:819B.
    1. Crone EA, Jennings JR, Van der Molen MW. Sensitivity to interference and response contingencies in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:224–226. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00115.
    1. Demurie E, Roeyers H, Baeyens D, Sonuga-Barke EJS. Common alterations in sensitivity to type but not amount of reward in ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:1164–1173. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02374.x.
    1. Douglas VI, Parry PA. Effects of reward and non-reward on frustration and attention in attention deficit disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1994;22:281–302. doi: 10.1007/BF02168075.
    1. Gladwin TE, Figner B, Crone EA, Wiers RW. Addiction, adolescence, and the integration of control and motivation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;1:364–376. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.06.008.
    1. Greenhill LL. Childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Pharmacological treatments. In: Nathan PE, Gorman J, editors. A guide to treatments that work. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. pp. 42–64.
    1. Geurts H, Luman M, van Meel C. What’s in a game: the effect of social motivation on interference control in boys with ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49:848–857. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01916.x.
    1. Haenlein M, Caul WF. Attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity: a specific hypothesis of reward dysfunction. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1987;26(3):356–362. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198705000-00014.
    1. Huang-Pollock CL, Mikami AY, Pfiffner L, McBurnett K. ADHD subtype differences in motivational responsivity but not inhibitory control: evidence from a reward-based variation of the stop signal paradigm. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2007;36:127–136. doi: 10.1080/15374410701274124.
    1. Iaboni F, Douglas VI, Baker AG. Effects of reward and response costs on inhibition in AD/HD children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1995;104:232–240.
    1. Iaboni F, Douglas VI, Ditto B. Psychophysiological response of AD/HD children to reward and extinction. Psychophysiology. 1997;34:116–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02422.x.
    1. Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlström K, Gillberg CG, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: a randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;44(2):177–186. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010.
    1. Kohls G, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Hyperresponsiveness to social rewards in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2009;5:20. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-20.
    1. Konrad K, Gauggel S, Manz A, Scholl M. Lack of inhibition: a motivational deficit in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and children with traumatic brain injury. Child Neuropsychology. 2000;6:286–296. doi: 10.1076/chin.6.4.286.3145.
    1. Kort W, Compaan EL, Bleichrodt N, Resing WCM, Schittekatte M, Bosmans M, Vermeir G, Verhaeghe P. WISC-III NL Handleiding. (Dutch Manual) Amsterdam: NIP; 2002.
    1. Lawrence V, Houghton S, Tannock R, Douglas G, Durkin K, Whiting K. ADHD outside the laboratory: boys’ executive function performance on tasks in videogame play and on a visit to the zoo. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2002;30:447–462. doi: 10.1023/A:1019812829706.
    1. Luman M, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA. The impact of reinforcement contingencies on ADHD: a review and theoretical appraisal. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25:183–213. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.11.001.
    1. Luman M, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA. Modulation of response timing in ADHD, effects of reinforcement valence and magnitude. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:445–456. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9190-8.
    1. Luman M, Van Meel CS, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA, Geurts HM. Does reward frequency or magnitude drive reinforcement-learning in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Psychiatry Research. 2009;168:222–229. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.08.012.
    1. Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, Tannock R. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;44(4):377–384. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000153228.72591.73.
    1. McInerny RJ, Kerns KA. Time reproduction in children with ADHD: motivation matters. Child Neuropsychology. 2003;9:91–108. doi: 10.1076/chin.9.2.91.14506.
    1. Michel JA, Kerns KA, Mateer CA. The effect of reinforcement variables on inhibition in children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychology. 2005;11:295–302. doi: 10.1080/092970490911270.
    1. Nigg JT. What causes ADHD? Understanding what goes wrong and why. New York: Guilford; 2006.
    1. Nigg JT, Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Sonuga-Barke EJS. Cuasal heterogeneity in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1224–1230. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025.
    1. Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA. Effects of reward and response cost on response inhibition in AD/HD, disruptive, anxious, and normal children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998;26:161–174. doi: 10.1023/A:1022650216978.
    1. Oosterlaan J, Scheres A, Antrop I, Roeyers H, Sergeant JA. Vragenlijst voor Gedragsproblemen bij Kinderen (VvGK) (Dutch translation of the DBD Rating Scale) Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2000.
    1. Pelham WE, Fabiano GA. Evidence-based psychosocial treatment for ADHD: an update. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2008;37:184–214. doi: 10.1080/15374410701818681.
    1. Pelham WE, Gnagy EM, Greenslade KE, Milich R. Teacher rating of DSM-III-R symptoms for disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1992;8:259–262.
    1. Prins PJM, Dovis S, Ponsioen AJGB, Ten Brink E, Van der Oord S. Does computerized working memory training with game elements enhance motivation and training efficacy in children with ADHD? Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2011;14(3):115–122. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0206.
    1. Rapport MD, Tucker SB, DuPaul GJ, Merlo M, Stoner G. Hyperactivity and frustration: the influence of control over and size of reward in delay gratification. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1986;14:191–204. doi: 10.1007/BF00915440.
    1. Rapport MD, Chung KM, Shore G, Isaacs P. A conceptual model of child psychopathology: implications for understanding attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and treatment efficacy. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology—Special Edition. 2001;30:48–58. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3001_6.
    1. Sattler JM. Assessment of children: Cognitive applications. 4. San Diego: Author; 2001.
    1. Scheres A, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA. Response inhibition in children with DSM-IV subtypes of AD/HD and related disruptive disorders: the role of reward. Child Neuropsychology. 2001;7:172–189. doi: 10.1076/chin.7.3.172.8746.
    1. Scheres A, Milham MP, Knutson B, Castellanos FX. Ventral striatal hyporesponsiveness during reward anticipation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61:720–724. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.042.
    1. Scheres A, Lee A, Sumiya M. Temporal reward discounting and ADHD: task and symptom specific effects. Journal of Neural Transmission. 2008;115:221–226. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0813-6.
    1. Sergeant JA, Oosterlaan J, Van der Meere J. Information processing and energetic factors in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. In: Quay HC, Hogan AE, editors. Handbook of disruptive behavior disorders. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 1999. pp. 75–104.
    1. Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas CP, Dulcan MK, Schwab-Stone ME. NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:28–38. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014.
    1. Shanahan M, Pennington B, Wilcutt E. Do motivational incentives reduce the inhibition deficit in ADHD? Developmental Neuropsychology. 2008;33:137–159. doi: 10.1080/87565640701884238.
    1. Shaw R, Grayson A, Lewis V. Inhibition, ADHD, and computergames: the inhibitory performance of children with ADHD on computerized tasks and games. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2005;8(4):160–168. doi: 10.1177/1087054705278771.
    1. Shiels K, Hawk LW, Jr, Lysczek CL, Tannock R, Pelham WE, Jr, Spencer SV, Gangloff BP, Waschbusch DA. The effects of incentives on visual-spatial working memory in children with Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:903–913. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9221-0.
    1. Solanto MV. The effects of reinforcement and response—cost on a delayed response task in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1990;31:803–808. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00819.x.
    1. Solanto MV, Wender EH, Bartell SS. Effects of methylphenidate and behavioral contingencies on sustained attention in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: a test of the reward dysfunction hypothesis. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 1997;7(2):123–136. doi: 10.1089/cap.1997.7.123.
    1. Sonuga-Barke EJS. Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD—a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Research. 2002;130:29–36. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00432-6.
    1. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Sergeant JA, Nigg JT, Willcutt EG. Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in ADHD: nosological and diagnostic implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2008;17:367–384. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2007.11.008.
    1. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Bitsakou P, Thompson M. Beyond the dual pathway model: evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibitory control, and delay-related impairments in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010;49:345–355.
    1. Slusarek M, Velling S, Bunk D, Eggers C. Motivational effects of inhibitory control in children with ADHD. Journal of the American Academic of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40(3):355–363. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200103000-00016.
    1. Steinberg L, Albert D, Cauffman E, Banich M, Graham S, Woolard J. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:1764–1778. doi: 10.1037/a0012955.
    1. Ströhle A, Stoy M, Wrase J, Schwarzer S, Schlagenhauf F, Huss M, Hein J, Nedderhut A, Neumann B, Gregor A, Juckel G, Knutson B, Lehmkuhl U, Bauer M, Heinz A. Reward anticipation and outcomes in adult males with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage. 2008;39:966–972. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.044.
    1. Tripp G, Alsop B. Sensitivity to reward frequency in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1999;28:366–375. doi: 10.1207/S15374424jccp280309.
    1. Tripp G, Alsop B. Sensitivity to reward delay in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD) Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:691–698. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00764.
    1. Van der Meere J, Hughes KA, Börger N, Sallee FR. The effect of reward on sustained attention in AD/HD children with and without CD. In: Sergeant JA, editor. European approach to hyperkinetic disorder. Zurich: Fotorotar; 1995. pp. 241–253.
    1. Wechsler D. The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. 4. Baltimore: Williams & Witkins; 1958.

Source: PubMed

3
구독하다