Mooney face stimuli for visual perception research

Caspar M Schwiedrzik, Lucia Melloni, Aaron Schurger, Caspar M Schwiedrzik, Lucia Melloni, Aaron Schurger

Abstract

In 1957, Craig Mooney published a set of human face stimuli to study perceptual closure: the formation of a coherent percept on the basis of minimal visual information. Images of this type, now known as "Mooney faces", are widely used in cognitive psychology and neuroscience because they offer a means of inducing variable perception with constant visuo-spatial characteristics (they are often not perceived as faces if viewed upside down). Mooney's original set of 40 stimuli has been employed in several studies. However, it is often necessary to use a much larger stimulus set. We created a new set of over 500 Mooney faces and tested them on a cohort of human observers. We present the results of our tests here, and make the stimuli freely available via the internet. Our test results can be used to select subsets of the stimuli that are most suited for a given experimental purpose.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1. Face and Mooney face stimuli.
Fig 1. Face and Mooney face stimuli.
(A) This greyscale image is easily perceived as a face although most visual information is covered by shadows. (B) A typical “Mooney” face. (C) An extremely easy “Mooney” face, devoid of cast shadows.
Fig 2. Reaction times.
Fig 2. Reaction times.
Subjects were faster when they perceived a face then when they did not, both in the upright and in the inverted condition (mean difference 29.8 ms, T(17) = 3.86, p = 0.001). They also showed a typical face inversion effect, taking longer to perceive inverted than upright Mooney faces (mean difference 38.9 ms, T(17) = 7.54, p<0.001). No reaction time differences between perceived and non-perceived scrambled images were observed (mean difference 2.1 ms, T(17) = 0.11, p = 0.906). Error bars reflect the standard error of the mean, corrected for between-subject variability [34, 35].
Fig 3. Frequency of stimuli perceived as…
Fig 3. Frequency of stimuli perceived as face, upright or inverted.
Most faces were correctly identified as faces by the majority of subjects when presented upright (green) both in our and in the original Mooney face set, but both stimulus sets also contain difficult stimuli that are only perceived as faces by a few subjects. Face inversion (blue) markedly reduced the number of ‘face’ responses. On the right are examples of easy and difficult Mooney faces from the upright and the inverted conditions, respectively. Original Mooney faces reprinted from [1] under a CC BY license, with permission from the Canadian Psychological Association Inc., original copyright 1957.
Fig 4. Inversion effects.
Fig 4. Inversion effects.
Inversion effects were evident both when considering whether an inverted face was recognized as a face (red) and when considering reaction times (yellow), i.e., longer reaction times to inverted than to upright faces. The latter type of inversion effect was much more frequent than the former. Overall, inversion effects of reaction times showed a median effect of 66 ms in our new stimulus set, and 79 ms in the original Mooney stimulus set (black solid lines). The right shows the stimuli with the most reliable/largest inversion effects for face recognition and reaction times, respectively. Original Mooney faces reprinted from [1] under a CC BY license, with permission from the Canadian Psychological Association Inc., original copyright 1957.

References

    1. Mooney CM. Age in the development of closure ability in children. Can J Psychol. 1957;11(4):219–26. doi: .
    1. Ramachandran VS, Armel C, Foster C, Stoddard R. Object recognition can drive motion perception. Nature. 1998;395(6705):852–3. doi: .
    1. Ludmer R, Dudai Y, Rubin N. Uncovering camouflage: amygdala activation predicts long-term memory of induced perceptual insight. Neuron. 2011;69(5):1002–14. doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3281502.
    1. George N, Jemel B, Fiori N, Chaby L, Renault B. Electrophysiological correlates of facial decision: insights from upright and upside-down Mooney-face perception. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;24(3):663–73. doi: .
    1. Kanwisher N, Tong F, Nakayama K. The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition. 1998;68(1):B1–11. doi: .
    1. Rodriguez E, George N, Lachaux JP, Martinerie J, Renault B, Varela FJ. Perception's shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity. Nature. 1999;397(6718):430–3. doi: .
    1. Hsieh PJ, Vul E, Kanwisher N. Recognition alters the spatial pattern of FMRI activation in early retinotopic cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(3):1501–7. doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3257064.
    1. Hegde J, Kersten D. A link between visual disambiguation and visual memory. J Neurosci. 2010;30(45):15124–33. doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3040725.
    1. Perrett DI, Smith PA, Potter DD, Mistlin AJ, Head AS, Milner AD, et al. Neurones responsive to faces in the temporal cortex: studies of functional organization, sensitivity to identity and relation to perception. Hum Neurobiol. 1984;3(4):197–208. .
    1. Tovee MJ, Rolls ET, Ramachandran VS. Rapid visual learning in neurones of the primate temporal visual cortex. Neuroreport. 1996;7(15–17):2757–60. doi: .
    1. Taubert J, Parr LA. The perception of two-tone Mooney faces in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Cogn Neurosci. 2012;3(1):21–8. doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3377185.
    1. Moeller S, Crapse T, Chang L, Tsao DY. The effect of face patch microstimulation on perception of faces and objects. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20(5):743–52. doi: .
    1. Imamoglu F, Kahnt T, Koch C, Haynes JD. Changes in functional connectivity support conscious object recognition. Neuroimage. 2012;63(4):1909–17. doi: .
    1. Brodski-Guerniero A, Paasch GF, Wollstadt P, Ozdemir I, Lizier JT, Wibral M. Information-theoretic evidence for predictive coding in the face-processing system. J Neurosci. 2017;37(34):8273–83. doi: .
    1. Dolan RJ, Fink GR, Rolls E, Booth M, Holmes A, Frackowiak RS, et al. How the brain learns to see objects and faces in an impoverished context. Nature. 1997;389(6651):596–9. doi: .
    1. Wasserstein J, Barr WB, Zappulla R, Rock D. Facial closure: interrelationship with facial discrimination, other closure tests, and subjective contour illusions. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(2):158–63. doi: .
    1. Busigny T, Joubert S, Felician O, Ceccaldi M, Rossion B. Holistic perception of the individual face is specific and necessary: evidence from an extensive case study of acquired prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(14):4057–92. doi: .
    1. Uhlhaas PJ, Linden DE, Singer W, Haenschel C, Lindner M, Maurer K, et al. Dysfunctional long-range coordination of neural activity during Gestalt perception in schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2006;26(31):8168–75. doi: .
    1. Sun L, Grutzner C, Bolte S, Wibral M, Tozman T, Schlitt S, et al. Impaired gamma-band activity during perceptual organization in adults with autism spectrum disorders: evidence for dysfunctional network activity in frontal-posterior cortices. J Neurosci. 2012;32(28):9563–73. doi: .
    1. Grutzner C, Wibral M, Sun L, Rivolta D, Singer W, Maurer K, et al. Deficits in high- (>60 Hz) gamma-band oscillations during visual processing in schizophrenia. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:88 doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3607810.
    1. Rivolta D, Castellanos NP, Stawowsky C, Helbling S, Wibral M, Grutzner C, et al. Source-reconstruction of event-related fields reveals hyperfunction and hypofunction of cortical circuits in antipsychotic-naive, first-episode schizophrenia patients during Mooney face processing. J Neurosci. 2014;34(17):5909–17. doi: .
    1. Mooney CM. Closure with negative after-images under flickering light. Can J Psychol. 1956;10(4):191–9. doi: .
    1. Jemel B, Pisani M, Calabria M, Crommelinck M, Bruyer R. Is the N170 for faces cognitively penetrable? Evidence from repetition priming of Mooney faces of familiar and unfamiliar persons. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003;17(2):431–46. doi: .
    1. McKeeff TJ, Tong F. The timing of perceptual decisions for ambiguous face stimuli in the human ventral visual cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(3):669–78. doi: .
    1. Rossion B, Dricot L, Goebel R, Busigny T. Holistic face categorization in higher order visual areas of the normal and prosopagnosic brain: toward a non-hierarchical view of face perception. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;4:225 doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3025660.
    1. Schwiedrzik CM, Melloni L, Schurger A. Mooney face stimuli for visual perception research. Figshare [Internet]. 2018. Available from: .
    1. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. doi: .
    1. Foreman N. Correlates of performance on the Gollin and Mooney tests of visual closure. J Gen Psychol. 1991;118(1):13–20. doi: .
    1. Verhallen RJ, Bosten JM, Goodbourn PT, Bargary G, Lawrance-Owen AJ, Mollon JD. An online version of the Mooney Face Test: phenotypic and genetic associations. Neuropsychologia. 2014;63:19–25. doi: .
    1. Hautus MJ. Corrections for extreme proportions and their biasing effects on estimated values of d'. Behav Res Meth Instrum Comput. 1995;27(1):46–51. doi:
    1. Fleiss JL. Measuring nominal scale agreement among many raters. Psychol Bull. 1971;76(5):378–82. doi:
    1. Cronbach LJ. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika. 1951;16(3):297–334. doi:
    1. Crouzet SM, Kirchner H, Thorpe SJ. Fast saccades toward faces: face detection in just 100 ms. Journal of vision. 2010;10(4):16 1–7. Epub 2010/05/15. doi: .
    1. Cousineau D. Confidence intervals in within-subjects designs: a simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method. Tutor Quant Methods Psychol. 2005;1(1):42–5. doi:
    1. Morey RD. Confidence intervals from normalized data: a correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutor Quant Methods Psychol. 2008;4(2):61–4. doi:
    1. Mooney CM. Closure as affected by viewing time and multiple visual fixations. Can J Psychol. 1957;11(1):21–8. doi: .
    1. Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33(1):159–74. doi: .
    1. Leo I, Simion F. Newborn's Mooney-face perception. Infancy. 2009;14(6):641–53. doi:
    1. Uhlhaas PJ, Roux F, Singer W, Haenschel C, Sireteanu R, Rodriguez E. The development of neural synchrony reflects late maturation and restructuring of functional networks in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(24):9866–71. doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2687997.
    1. Carbon CC, Gruter M, Gruter T. Age-dependent face detection and face categorization performance. PLoS One. 2013;8(10):e79164 doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3792936.
    1. Yoon JM, Witthoft N, Winawer J, Frank MC, Everett DL, Gibson E. Cultural differences in perceptual reorganization in US and Piraha adults. PLoS One. 2014;9(11):e110225 doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4238998.
    1. McKone E. Isolating the special component of face recognition: peripheral identification and a Mooney face. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004;30(1):181–97. doi: .
    1. Muth C, Carbon CC. The aesthetic aha: on the pleasure of having insights into Gestalt. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2013;144(1):25–30. doi: .
    1. Kemelmacher-Shlizerman I, Basri R, Nadler B. 3D shape reconstruction of Mooney faces. IEEE Comput Soc Conf Comput Vis Pattern Recogn. 2008. doi:

Source: PubMed

3
구독하다