Lesion network localization of criminal behavior

R Ryan Darby, Andreas Horn, Fiery Cushman, Michael D Fox, R Ryan Darby, Andreas Horn, Fiery Cushman, Michael D Fox

Abstract

Following brain lesions, previously normal patients sometimes exhibit criminal behavior. Although rare, these cases can lend unique insight into the neurobiological substrate of criminality. Here we present a systematic mapping of lesions with known temporal association to criminal behavior, identifying 17 lesion cases. The lesion sites were spatially heterogeneous, including the medial prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and different locations within the bilateral temporal lobes. No single brain region was damaged in all cases. Because lesion-induced symptoms can come from sites connected to the lesion location and not just the lesion location itself, we also identified brain regions functionally connected to each lesion location. This technique, termed lesion network mapping, has recently identified regions involved in symptom generation across a variety of lesion-induced disorders. All lesions were functionally connected to the same network of brain regions. This criminality-associated connectivity pattern was unique compared with lesions causing four other neuropsychiatric syndromes. This network includes regions involved in morality, value-based decision making, and theory of mind, but not regions involved in cognitive control or empathy. Finally, we replicated our results in a separate cohort of 23 cases in which a temporal relationship between brain lesions and criminal behavior was implied but not definitive. Our results suggest that lesions in criminals occur in different brain locations but localize to a unique resting state network, providing insight into the neurobiology of criminal behavior.

Keywords: criminality; functional connectivity; lesion; morality; sociopathy.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Copyright © 2018 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Lesions temporally associated with criminal behavior. Lesions from 17 patients with acquired criminal behavior, manually traced onto a common brain atlas (MNI).
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Lesion network mapping technique. (A) Lesions traced onto a standardized MNI brain template. (B) Brain regions functionally connected to each lesion location based on a large resting-state functional connectivity database. (C) Overlap in the functional connectivity maps from each lesion identifies brain regions functionally connected to the greatest number of lesion locations.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Lesions temporally associated with criminality are part of a unique connected brain network. (A) Regions functionally connected to 17 lesion locations temporally associated with criminal behavior. (B) Nonparametric voxel-wise lesion-symptom mapping results showing regions in which 17 lesions temporally associated with criminal behavior were significantly more connected (positive in red, negative in blue) compared with the connectivity of 63 lesions causing other syndromes.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
Lesions temporally associated with criminality are functionally connected to morality regions. (A) Regions functionally connected to lesion locations temporally associated with criminal behavior (red) are similar to regions activated by moral decision-making tasks (green; overlap in yellow). (B and C) Lesion locations temporally associated with criminal behavior (gray) are functionally connected to regions activated by moral decision-making tasks identified in meta-analyses using Neurosynth (B) or ALE (C), unlike lesions causing other neurologic syndromes (control syndromes; black). **P < 0.0001.
Fig. 5.
Fig. 5.
Lesions temporally associated with criminality are functionally connected to some, but not all, subcomponents of moral decision making. Connectivity with lesion locations temporally associated with criminal behavior (red) is similar to that in regions activated by some but not other components of moral decision making (green; overlap in yellow). Quantitatively, lesion locations temporally associated with criminal behavior are functionally connected to regions activated by value-based decision making (A) and theory of mind tasks (B), but not with empathy (C) or cognitive control tasks (D). These results were specific to lesions temporally associated with criminal behavior (gray) compared with lesions causing other neurologic syndromes (control syndromes; black). **P < 0.0001.
Fig. 6.
Fig. 6.
Lesions temporally associated with criminality are connected in opposite directions to opponent networks involved in moral decision making. (A) Lesion locations are positively connected (red) with regions activated by decisions to avoid harm in personal moral dilemmas (green; overlap in yellow). (B) Lesion locations are negatively connected (blue) with regions activated by utilitarian decisions in impersonal moral dilemmas (green; overlap in yellow). Results are specific to lesions temporally associated with criminal behavior (gray) compared with lesions causing other neurologic syndromes (black). **P < 0.0001.
Fig. 7.
Fig. 7.
Replication in lesions of uncertain temporal association with criminal behavior. Lesions with a clear temporal association (original cohort) (A) and uncertain temporal association (replication cohort) (B) are connected to the same regions (lesion network overlap, Right; voxel-wise nonparametric comparison with control syndromes, Middle). Lesions in both cohorts were significantly connected to regions involved in morality compared with control lesions (Right). **P < 0.0001.

References

    1. Anderson DA. The aggregate burden of crime. J Law Econ. 1999;42:611–642.
    1. Glenn AL, Raine A. Neurocriminology: Implications for the punishment, prediction and prevention of criminal behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15:54–63.
    1. Blake PY, Pincus JH, Buckner C. Neurologic abnormalities in murderers. Neurology. 1995;45:1641–1647.
    1. Anderson NE, Kiehl KA. The psychopath magnetized: Insights from brain imaging. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16:52–60.
    1. Bufkin JL, Luttrell VR. Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent behavior: Current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2005;6:176–191.
    1. Adolphs R. Human lesion studies in the 21st century. Neuron. 2016;90:1151–1153.
    1. Denno DW. The myth of the double-edged sword : An empirical study of neuroscience evidence in criminal cases. Boston Coll Law Rev. 2015;56:493–552.
    1. Saver JL, Damasio AR. Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage. Neuropsychologia. 1991;29:1241–1249.
    1. Blumer D, Benson DF. Personality changes with frontal lobe lesions. In: Benson DF, Blumer D, editors. Psychiatric Aspects of Neurological Disease. Grune & Stratton; New York: 1975. pp. 151–170.
    1. Anderson SW, Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:1032–1037.
    1. Anderson SW, Damasio H, Tranel D. Neuropsychological impairments associated with lesions caused by tumor or stroke. Arch Neurol. 1990;47:397–405.
    1. Taber-Thomas BC, et al. Arrested development: Early prefrontal lesions impair the maturation of moral judgement. Brain. 2014;137:1254–1261.
    1. Pardini M, et al. Prefrontal cortex lesions and MAO-A modulate aggression in penetrating traumatic brain injury. Neurology. 2011;76:1038–1045.
    1. Anderson SW, Barrash J, Bechara A, Tranel D. Impairments of emotion and real-world complex behavior following childhood- or adult-onset damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12:224–235.
    1. Batts S. Brain lesions and their implications in criminal responsibility. Behav Sci Law. 2009;27:261–272.
    1. Damasio H, Grabowski T, Frank R, Galaburda AM, Damasio AR. The return of Phineas Gage: Clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science. 1994;264:1102–1105.
    1. Mendez MF. The neurobiology of moral behavior: Review and neuropsychiatric implications. CNS Spectr. 2009;14:608–620.
    1. Braun CMJ, Léveillé C, Guimond A. An orbitofrontostriatopallidal pathway for morality: Evidence from postlesion antisocial and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2008;13:296–337.
    1. Fumagalli M, Priori A. Functional and clinical neuroanatomy of morality. Brain. 2012;135:2006–2021.
    1. Monakow C. Die Lokalisation im Grosshirn: und der Abbau der Funktion durch kortikale Herde. von J.F. Bergmann; Wiesbaden, Germany: 1914.
    1. Boes AD, et al. Network localization of neurological symptoms from focal brain lesions. Brain. 2015;138:3061–3075.
    1. Sutterer MJ, et al. Canceled connections: Lesion-derived network mapping helps explain differences in performance on a complex decision-making task. Cortex. 2016;78:31–43.
    1. Darby RR, Laganiere S, Pascual-Leone A, Prasad S, Fox MD. Finding the imposter: Brain connectivity of lesions causing delusional misidentifications. Brain. 2017;140:497–507.
    1. Fischer DB, et al. A human brain network derived from coma-causing brainstem lesions. Neurology. 2016;87:2427–2434.
    1. Laganiere S, Boes AD, Fox MD. Network localization of hemichorea-hemiballismus. Neurology. 2016;86:2187–2195.
    1. Buckholtz JW. Social norms, self-control, and the value of antisocial behavior. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;3:122–129.
    1. Baron-Cohen S. The Science of Evil: On Empathy and the Origins of Cruelty. Basic Books; New York: 2013.
    1. Horn A, et al. Connectivity predicts deep brain stimulation outcome in Parkinson disease. Ann Neurol. 2017;82:67–78.
    1. Darby RR, Fox MD. Reply. Capgras syndrome: Neuroanatomical assessment of brain MRI findings in an adolescent patient. Brain. 2017;140:e44.
    1. Eickhoff SB, et al. Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: A random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2907–2926.
    1. Bzdok D, et al. Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Struct Funct. 2012;217:783–796.
    1. Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat Methods. 2011;8:665–670.
    1. Greene JD, Nystrom LE, Engell AD, Darley JM, Cohen JD. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron. 2004;44:389–400.
    1. Cushman F. Action, outcome, and value: A dual-system framework for morality. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2013;17:273–292.
    1. Young L, Cushman F, Hauser M, Saxe R. The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:8235–8240.
    1. Darby RR, Edersheim J, Price BH. What patients with behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia can teach us about moral responsibility. AJOB Neurosci. 2016;7:193–201.
    1. Fox MD, et al. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102:9673–9678.
    1. Greene JD, Sommerville RB, Nystrom LE, Darley JM, Cohen JD. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science. 2001;293:2105–2108.
    1. Koenigs M, et al. Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature. 2007;446:908–911.
    1. Koenigs M, Kruepke M, Zeier J, Newman JP. Utilitarian moral judgment in psychopathy. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012;7:708–714.
    1. Mendez MF, Anderson E, Shapira JS. An investigation of moral judgement in frontotemporal dementia. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18:193–197.
    1. Koenigs M, Tranel D. Irrational economic decision-making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: Evidence from the ultimatum game. J Neurosci. 2007;27:951–956.
    1. Koenigs M, Kruepke M, Newman JP. Economic decision-making in psychopathy: A comparison with ventromedial prefrontal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:2198–2204.
    1. O’Callaghan C, et al. Fair play: Social norm compliance failures in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2016;139:204–216.
    1. Feng C, Luo Y-J, Krueger F. Neural signatures of fairness-related normative decision making in the ultimatum game: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:591–602.
    1. Mendez MF, Chen AK, Shapira JS, Miller BL. Acquired sociopathy and frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2005;20:99–104.
    1. Liljegren M, et al. Criminal behavior in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72:295–300.
    1. Diehl-Schmid J, Perneczky R, Koch J, Nedopil N, Kurz A. Guilty by suspicion? Criminal behavior in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2013;26:73–77.
    1. Ermer E, Cope LM, Nyalakanti PK, Calhoun VD, Kiehl KA. Aberrant paralimbic gray matter in criminal psychopathy. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121:649–658.
    1. de Oliveira-Souza R, et al. Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain: Fronto-temporo-limbic grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage. 2008;40:1202–1213.
    1. Motzkin JC, Newman JP, Kiehl KA, Koenigs M. Reduced prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy. J Neurosci. 2011;31:17348–17357.
    1. Fazel S, Lichtenstein P, Grann M, Långström N. Risk of violent crime in individuals with epilepsy and traumatic brain injury: A 35-year Swedish population study. PLoS Med. 2011;8:e1001150.
    1. Timonen M, et al. The association of preceding traumatic brain injury with mental disorders, alcoholism and criminality: The northern Finland 1966 birth cohort study. Psychiatry Res. 2002;113:217–226.
    1. Grafman J, et al. Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: A report of the Vietnam head injury study. Neurology. 1996;46:1231–1238.
    1. Yeo BT, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–1165.
    1. Grefkes C, Fink GR. Connectivity-based approaches in stroke and recovery of function. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13:206–216.

Source: PubMed

3
구독하다