Cerebral Correlates of Abnormal Emotion Conflict Processing in Euthymic Bipolar Patients: A Functional MRI Study

Pauline Favre, Mircea Polosan, Cédric Pichat, Thierry Bougerol, Monica Baciu, Pauline Favre, Mircea Polosan, Cédric Pichat, Thierry Bougerol, Monica Baciu

Abstract

Background: Patients with bipolar disorder experience cognitive and emotional impairment that may persist even during the euthymic state of the disease. These persistent symptoms in bipolar patients (BP) may be characterized by disturbances of emotion regulation and related fronto-limbic brain circuitry. The present study aims to investigate the modulation of fronto-limbic activity and connectivity in BP by the processing of emotional conflict.

Methods: Fourteen euthymic BP and 13 matched healthy subjects (HS) underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while performing a word-face emotional Stroop task designed to dissociate the monitoring/generation of emotional conflict from its resolution. Functional connectivity was determined by means of psychophysiological interaction (PPI) approach.

Results: Relative to HS, BP were slower to process incongruent stimuli, reflecting higher amount of behavioral interference during emotional Stroop. Furthermore, BP showed decreased activation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during the monitoring and a lack of bilateral amygdala deactivation during the resolution of the emotional conflict. In addition, during conflict monitoring, BP showed abnormal positive connectivity between the right DLPFC and several regions of the default mode network.

Conclusions: Overall, our results highlighted dysfunctional processing of the emotion conflict in euthymic BP that may be subtended by abnormal activity and connectivity of the DLPFC during the conflict monitoring, which, in turn, leads to failure of amygdala deactivation during the resolution of the conflict. Emotional dysregulation in BP may be underpinned by a lack of top-down cognitive control and a difficulty to focus on the task due to persistent self-oriented attention.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT01821469.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1. Example of two successive trials…
Fig 1. Example of two successive trials presented in word-face emotional Stroop.
Stimuli were either congruent or incongruent according to the valence of facial expression (i.e., joyful or fearful) and the valence of the word written across them (i.e., joie: happy or peur: fear). High conflict resolution trials consisted of incongruent stimuli preceded by incongruent stimuli; Low conflict resolution trials consisted of incongruent stimuli preceded by congruent stimuli; No conflict trials consisted of congruent stimuli preceded by either congruent (NC1) or by incongruent stimuli (NC2) (not shown in the figure). Pictures were extracted from the “Montreal Set of Facial Display of Emotion” (MSFDE) database [30].
Fig 2. Behavioral performances during word-face emotional…
Fig 2. Behavioral performances during word-face emotional Stroop.
Panel A: Illustration of behavioral dissociation between conflict monitoring and conflict resolution. The graph shows the mean response time ± SE according to the congruency of the current trial and the congruency of the previous trial. Panel B: Illustration of the increase emotional interference in euthymic bipolar patients. The graph shows the mean response time ± SE according to the group and the congruency of the current trial. *p<0.05. Abbreviations: LR: Low conflict resolution; HR: High conflict resolution; NC: No conflict.
Fig 3. Results of the within-group analyses…
Fig 3. Results of the within-group analyses at whole-brain level during conflict monitoring in (A) healthy subjects and (B) euthymic bipolar patients.
Identified regions are projected onto 2D anatomical slices in axial, coronal and sagittal orientations (p < 0.001 uncorrected at whole brain level, pFWE < 0.05 after small volume correction).
Fig 4. Results provided by between-group analyses.
Fig 4. Results provided by between-group analyses.
Panel A: Whole-brain comparison in HS vs BP during conflict monitoring (LR > HR) (p < 0.001 uncorrected at whole brain level, pFWE < 0.05 after small volume correction). Identified regions are projected onto 2D anatomical slices in axial, coronal and sagittal orientations. Panel B: Region of interest analysis focused on bilateral amygdala. The graph shows the mean %MR signal intensity variations ± SE according to the group and the amount of the conflict (LR vs HR). *p<0.05. Abbreviations: BP: Bipolar patients; HS: Healthy subjects; LR: Low conflict resolution; HR: High conflict resolution.
Fig 5. Psychophysiological interaction results.
Fig 5. Psychophysiological interaction results.
Panel A: Results provided by “within group” analysis in healthy subjects; Panel B: Results provided by “within group” analysis in bipolar patients; Panel C: Results provided by the “between-group” analysis in Bipolar patients vs. Healthy subjects. Red-scale areas represent regions showing positive connectivity with the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; Blue-scale areas represent regions showing negative connectivity with the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Identified regions are projected onto 2D anatomical slices in axial, coronal and sagittal orientations (p < 0.005 uncorrected at whole brain level, pFWE < 0.05 after small volume correction).

References

    1. Henry C, Van den Bulke D, Bellivier F, Roy I, Swendsen J, M'Baïlara K, et al. Affective lability and affect intensity as core dimensions of bipolar disorders during euthymic period. Psychiatry Res. 2008;159(1):1–6.
    1. M'Bailara K, Demotes-Mainard J, Swendsen J, Mathieu F, Leboyer M, Henry C. Emotional hyper-reactivity in normothymic bipolar patients. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(1):63–9. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00656.x
    1. Martínez-Arán A, Vieta E, Colom F, Torrent C, Sánchez-Moreno J, Reinares M, et al. Cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar patients: implications for clinical and functional outcome. Bipolar Disord. 2004;6(3):224–32.
    1. Manove E, Levy B. Cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: an overview. Postgrad Med. 2010;122(4):7–16. 10.3810/pgm.2010.07.2170
    1. Strakowski SM, Adler CM, Almeida J, Altshuler LL, Blumberg HP, Chang KD, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of bipolar disorder: a consensus model. Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(4):313–25. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01022.x
    1. Phillips ML, Swartz HA. A Critical Appraisal of Neuroimaging Studies of Bipolar Disorder: Toward a New Conceptualization of Underlying Neural Circuitry and a Road Map for Future Research. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;in press.
    1. Chen CH, Suckling J, Lennox BR, Ooi C, Bullmore ET. A quantitative meta-analysis of fMRI studies in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2011;13(1):1–15. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00893.x
    1. Houenou J, Frommberger J, Carde S, Glasbrenner M, Diener C, Leboyer M, et al. Neuroimaging-based markers of bipolar disorder: Evidence from two meta-analyses. J Affect Disord. 2011.
    1. Townsend JD, Altshuler LL. Emotion processing and regulation in bipolar disorder: a review. Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(4):326–39. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01021.x
    1. Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, Drevets WC. A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13(9):833–57.
    1. Whalen PJ, Bush G, McNally RJ, Wilhelm S, McInerney SC, Jenike MA, et al. The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44(12):1219–28.
    1. Blair KS, Smith BW, Mitchell DGV, Morton J, Vythilingam M, Pessoa L, et al. Modulation of emotion by cognition and cognition by emotion. Neuroimage. 2007;35(1):430–40.
    1. Lagopoulos J, Malhi GS. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of emotional Stroop in euthymic bipolar disorder. Neuroreport. 2007;18(15):1583
    1. Malhi GS, Lagopoulos J, Sachdev PS, Ivanovski B, Shnier R. An emotional Stroop functional MRI study of euthymic bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2005;7:58–69.
    1. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–62.
    1. Etkin A, Egner T, Peraza DM, Kandel ER, Hirsch J. Resolving emotional conflict: a role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron. 2006;51(6):871–82.
    1. Favre P, Baciu M, Pichat C, De Pourtalès M-A, Fredembach B, Garçon S, et al. Modulation of fronto-limbic activity by the psychoeducation in euthymic bipolar patients. A functional MRI study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2013;214(3):285–95. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.07.007
    1. Rey G, Desseilles M, Favre S, Dayer A, Piguet C, Aubry J-M, et al. Modulation of brain response to emotional conflict as a function of current mood in bipolar disorder: Preliminary findings from a follow-up state-based fMRI study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2014;in press.
    1. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303(5660):1023–6.
    1. Egner T, Hirsch J. The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task. Neuroimage. 2005;24(2):539–47.
    1. Braver TS. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2012;16(2):106–13. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010
    1. Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev. 2001;108(3):624
    1. Favre P, Baciu M, Pichat C, Bougerol T, Polosan M. fMRI evidence for abnormal resting-state functional connectivity in euthymic bipolar patients. J Affect Disord. 2014;165:182–9. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.054
    1. Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry. 1979;134(4):382–9.
    1. Pellet J, Bobon D, Mormont I, Lang F, Massardier A. Etude princeps de validation française de la MADRS, sous échelle de dépression de la CPRS Compte rendu du Congrès de Psychiatrie et de Neurologie de langue Française. Paris, Masson; 1981.
    1. Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry. 1978;133(5):429–35.
    1. Favre S, Aubry JM, Gex-Fabry M, Ragama-Pardos E, McQuillan A, Bertschy G. Translation and validation of a French version of the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS). L'Encephale. 2003;29(6):499
    1. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders: SCID-I, Patients Edition (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002.
    1. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1976.
    1. Beaupré MG, Hess U. Cross-cultural emotion recognition among Canadian ethnic groups. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2005;36(3):355–70.
    1. Friston KJ, Zarahn E, Josephs O, Henson RNA, Dale AM. Stochastic designs in event-related fMRI. Neuroimage. 1999;10(5):607–19.
    1. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15(1):273–89.
    1. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline JP, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp. 1995;2(4):189–210.
    1. Friston KJ, Fletcher P, Josephs O, Holmes A, Rugg MD, Turner R. Event-related fMRI: characterizing differential responses. Neuroimage. 1998;7(1):30–40.
    1. Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Kraft RA, Burdette JH. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage. 2003;19(3):1233
    1. Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 1997;6(3):218–29.
    1. Gitelman DR, Penny WD, Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Modeling regional and psychophysiologic interactions in fMRI: the importance of hemodynamic deconvolution. Neuroimage. 2003;19(1):200–7.
    1. Robinson LJ, Thompson JM, Gallagher P, Goswami U, Young AH, Ferrier IN, et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2006;93(1):105–15.
    1. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J Affect Disord. 2009;113(1):1–20.
    1. Pompei F, Jogia J, Tatarelli R, Girardi P, Rubia K, Kumari V, et al. Familial and disease specific abnormalities in the neural correlates of the Stroop Task in Bipolar Disorder. Neuroimage. 2011;56(3):1677–84. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.052
    1. Kronhaus DM, Lawrence NS, Williams AM, Frangou S, Brammer MJ, Williams SCR, et al. Stroop performance in bipolar disorder: further evidence for abnormalities in the ventral prefrontal cortex. Bipolar Disord. 2006;8(1):28–39.
    1. Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23(10):475–83.
    1. Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex- an Update:Time is of the essence. Neuron. 2001;30(2):319–33.
    1. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2005;9(5):242–9.
    1. Walter H, von Kalckreuth A, Schardt D, Stephan A, Goschke T, Erk S. The temporal dynamics of voluntary emotion regulation. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(8):e6726–e. 10.1371/journal.pone.0006726
    1. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(2):676–82.
    1. Raichle ME, Snyder AZ. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage. 2007;37(4):1083–90.
    1. Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2001;98(7):4259–64.
    1. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124(1):1–38.
    1. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):215–29.
    1. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102(27):9673–8.
    1. Lemogne C, le Bastard G, Mayberg H, Volle E, Bergouignan L, Lehéricy S, et al. In search of the depressive self: extended medial prefrontal network during self-referential processing in major depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009;4(3):305–12. 10.1093/scan/nsp008
    1. Nejad AB, Fossati P, Lemogne C. Self-referential processing, rumination, and cortical midline structures in major depression. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:666 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00666
    1. Mayberg HS, Liotti M, Brannan SK, McGinnis S, Mahurin RK, Jerabek PA, et al. Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(5):675–82.
    1. Greicius MD, Flores BH, Menon V, Glover GH, Solvason HB, Kenna H, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(5):429–37.

Source: PubMed

3
구독하다