Neural mechanisms underlying motivation of mental versus physical effort

Liane Schmidt, Maël Lebreton, Marie-Laure Cléry-Melin, Jean Daunizeau, Mathias Pessiglione, Liane Schmidt, Maël Lebreton, Marie-Laure Cléry-Melin, Jean Daunizeau, Mathias Pessiglione

Abstract

Mental and physical efforts, such as paying attention and lifting weights, have been shown to involve different brain systems. These cognitive and motor systems, respectively, include cortical networks (prefronto-parietal and precentral regions) as well as subregions of the dorsal basal ganglia (caudate and putamen). Both systems appeared sensitive to incentive motivation: their activity increases when we work for higher rewards. Another brain system, including the ventral prefrontal cortex and the ventral basal ganglia, has been implicated in encoding expected rewards. How this motivational system drives the cognitive and motor systems remains poorly understood. More specifically, it is unclear whether cognitive and motor systems can be driven by a common motivational center or if they are driven by distinct, dedicated motivational modules. To address this issue, we used functional MRI to scan healthy participants while performing a task in which incentive motivation, cognitive, and motor demands were varied independently. We reasoned that a common motivational node should (1) represent the reward expected from effort exertion, (2) correlate with the performance attained, and (3) switch effective connectivity between cognitive and motor regions depending on task demand. The ventral striatum fulfilled all three criteria and therefore qualified as a common motivational node capable of driving both cognitive and motor regions of the dorsal striatum. Thus, we suggest that the interaction between a common motivational system and the different task-specific systems underpinning behavioral performance might occur within the basal ganglia.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Behavioral task and results.
Figure 1. Behavioral task and results.
(A) Example of task trial. Successive screenshots displayed are shown from left to right with durations in ms. Every trial started with a central fixation cross. Then the monetary incentive (0.01, 0.1, or 1€) was displayed as a coin image and effort was triggered by the onset of a graduated line representing a ladder. The goal was to move the white cursor up as high as possible, each step representing 10% of the money at stake. To reach the next step participants had to squeeze the handgrip on the side of the numerically greater figure in the white box. In congruent pairs this figure was also the greater physically (font size), whereas it was physically smaller in incongruent pairs. The motor demand was manipulated by changing the amount of force needed to reach the next step (30% versus 60% of the maximal force in easy versus hard trials). The cognitive demand was manipulated by changing the proportion of congruent pairs (100% versus 50% in easy versus hard trials). At the end of every trial the cumulative total of monetary earnings was displayed on the screen. (B) Performance across experimental conditions. Performance is expressed as the percentage of the monetary incentive reached (i.e., of steps completed on the ladder). Bars represent the average performance ± inter-participant standard error for the three monetary incentives (0.01, 0.1, and 1€) and the four effort conditions (m, easy motor effort; M, hard motor effort; c, easy cognitive effort; C, hard cognitive effort). * Significant difference (two-tailed paired t test, p<0.05); ns, non-significant.
Figure 2. Neural correlates of main experimental…
Figure 2. Neural correlates of main experimental factors and effects (reward level, cognitive and motor demands, and performance level).
(A) Statistical parametric maps (SPM) show the main parametric modulation effects obtained with GLM1. (B) SPM shows the conjunction between expected reward and performance level modulation effects. Voxels displayed in grey-black on the glass brains survived a threshold of p<0.05 after voxel-wise correction for multiple comparisons (family-wise error, FWE). Frontal slices were taken at the maxima of interest indicated by blue lines on glass brains and superimposed on the average structural scan. Voxels displayed in yellow on slices survived a threshold of p<0.001 (uncorrected) after cluster-wise FWE correction for multiple comparisons. The [x y z] coordinates of maxima refer to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.
Figure 3. Neural correlates of performance effects…
Figure 3. Neural correlates of performance effects depending on effort type (motor or cognitive demand).
(A) SPM show activations obtained with GLM2 for the contrast between high and low incentives, the correlation with performance levels, and the conjunction between these two effects. (B) SPM shows regions parametrically modulated by performance level during both motor and cognitive effort (conjunction of mC and Mc conditions). (C) Graphs show regression coefficients (betas) obtained in the ventral striatum (VS) for the parametric modulation by performance levels during effort exertion in mC and Mc conditions, separately. Bars represent mean ± inter-participant standard errors. Dotted line indicates non-significant (NS) difference (two-tailed paired t test, p>0.05). (D) Scatter plots illustrate the inter-participant correlation between behavioral and neural incentive effects (1€ versus 1c). Solid line represents significant correlation (p<0.05).
Figure 4. Psychophysiological interaction (PPI) between ventral…
Figure 4. Psychophysiological interaction (PPI) between ventral striatum activity and task demand.
SPMs show voxels in which activity was significantly correlated with left VS signal when the motor demand was high (top) and when the cognitive demand was high (bottom). Significant voxels (p<0.05, whole-brain voxel-wise FWE correction) are displayed in grey-black on glass brains and in orange-yellow on axial slices. Slices were taken at the peaks located in the left caudate for cognitive effort and in the left putamen for motor effort. Activations are superimposed on the average structural brain scan. The [x y z] coordinates refer to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.
Figure 5. Dynamic causal modeling (DCM) analysis…
Figure 5. Dynamic causal modeling (DCM) analysis optimizing the connectivity structure of the striatal network.
In all illustrated models, the driving inputs were boxcar functions over effort exertion periods (Eff) for the caudate (Cd) and putamen (Pt) and over incentive display plus effort exertion modulated by expected reward (Rew) for the ventral striatum (VS). From left to right, connections were systematically added up to a fully connected network. From top to bottom: the locus of parametric modulation by cognitive (Cog) and motor (Mot) task demand was varied. Graphs illustrate the result of a Bayesian model selection (BMS) procedure used to find the most likely model.
Figure 6. Dynamic causal modeling (DCM) analysis…
Figure 6. Dynamic causal modeling (DCM) analysis confronting our best model to alternative hypotheses.
Model a is the winner of the previous Bayesian model selection (i.e., model B1 in Figure 5). Model b reversed the direction of links impacted by task demand modulatory, now going from caudate and putamen to the VS. Model c changed the loci of task demand modulatory effects, now affecting directly VS activity instead of connectivity. VS, ventral striatum; Cd, caudate nucleus; Pt, putamen; Rew, expected reward; Eff, effort exertion; Cog, cognitive demand; Mot, motor demand. Graphs illustrate the result of a Bayesian model selection (BMS) procedure, showing the exceedance probability of each model.

References

    1. Berridge K. C. Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiol Behav. 2004;81:179–209.
    1. Small D. M, Gitelman D, Simmons K, Bloise S. M, Parrish T, et al. Monetary incentives enhance processing in brain regions mediating top-down control of attention. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:1855–1865.
    1. Kouneiher F, Charron S, Koechlin E. Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:939–945.
    1. Krawczyk D. C, Gazzaley A, D'Esposito M. Reward modulation of prefrontal and visual association cortex during an incentive working memory task. Brain Res. 2007;1141:168–177.
    1. Pochon J. B, Levy R, Fossati P, Lehericy S, Poline J. B, et al. The neural system that bridges reward and cognition in humans: an fMRI study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:5669–5674.
    1. Locke H. S, Braver T. S. Motivational influences on cognitive control: behavior, brain activation, and individual differences. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2008;8:99–112.
    1. Spraker M. B, Yu H, Corcos D. M, Vaillancourt D. E. Role of individual basal ganglia nuclei in force amplitude generation. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:821–834.
    1. Pleger B, Blankenburg F, Ruff C. C, Driver J, Dolan R. J. Reward facilitates tactile judgments and modulates hemodynamic responses in human primary somatosensory cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:8161–8168.
    1. Keisker B, Hepp-Reymond M. C, Blickenstorfer A, Meyer M, Kollias S. S. Differential force scaling of fine-graded power grip force in the sensorimotor network. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2453–2465.
    1. Pessiglione M, Schmidt L, Draganski B, Kalisch R, Lau H, et al. How the brain translates money into force: a neuroimaging study of subliminal motivation. Science. 2007;316:904–906.
    1. Talelli P, Ewas A, Waddingham W, Rothwell J. C, Ward N. S. Neural correlates of age-related changes in cortical neurophysiology. Neuroimage. 2008;40:1772–1781.
    1. Daw N. D, Doya K. The computational neurobiology of learning and reward. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:199–204.
    1. Rangel A, Camerer C, Montague P. R. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:545–556.
    1. Knutson B, Cooper J. C. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction. Curr Opin Neurol. 2005;18:411–417.
    1. O'Doherty J. P, Hampton A, Kim H. Model-based fMRI and its application to reward learning and decision making. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1104:35–53.
    1. McClure S. M, York M. K, Montague P. R. The neural substrates of reward processing in humans: the modern role of FMRI. Neuroscientist. 2004;10:260–268.
    1. Rushworth M. F, Behrens T. E. Choice, uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:389–397.
    1. Alexander G. E, DeLong M. R, Strick P. L. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986;9:357–381.
    1. Brown R. G, Pluck G. Negative symptoms: the ‘pathology’ of motivation and goal-directed behaviour. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:412–417.
    1. Haber S. N. The primate basal ganglia: parallel and integrative networks. J Chem Neuroanat. 2003;26:317–330.
    1. Lehericy S, Ducros M, Van de Moortele P. F, Francois C, Thivard L, et al. Diffusion tensor fiber tracking shows distinct corticostriatal circuits in humans. Ann Neurol. 2004;55:522–529.
    1. Schmidt L, Clery-Melin M. L, Lafargue G, Valabregue R, Fossati P, et al. Get aroused and be stronger: emotional facilitation of physical effort in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2009;29:9450–9457.
    1. Kaufmann L, Koppelstaetter F, Delazer M, Siedentopf C, Rhomberg P, et al. Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2005;25:888–898.
    1. Liu X, Wang H, Corbly C. R, Zhang J, Joseph J. E. The involvement of the inferior parietal cortex in the numerical Stroop effect and the distance effect in a two-digit number comparison task. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1518–1530.
    1. Tang J, Critchley H. D, Glaser D. E, Dolan R. J, Butterworth B. Imaging informational conflict: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of numerical stroop. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:2049–2062.
    1. Knutson B, Adams C. M, Fong G. W, Hommer D. Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci. 2001;21:RC159.
    1. O'Doherty J, Dayan P, Schultz J, Deichmann R, Friston K, et al. Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science. 2004;304:452–454.
    1. Yacubian J, Glascher J, Schroeder K, Sommer T, Braus D. F, et al. Dissociable systems for gain- and loss-related value predictions and errors of prediction in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2006;26:9530–9537.
    1. Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandin G, Dolan R. J, Frith C. D. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature. 2006;442:1042–1045.
    1. Elliott R, Friston K. J, Dolan R. J. Dissociable neural responses in human reward systems. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6159–6165.
    1. Gan J. O, Walton M. E, Phillips P. E. Dissociable cost and benefit encoding of future rewards by mesolimbic dopamine. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:25–27.
    1. Phillips P. E, Walton M. E, Jhou T. C. Calculating utility: preclinical evidence for cost-benefit analysis by mesolimbic dopamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191:483–495.
    1. Walton M. E, Kennerley S. W, Bannerman D. M, Phillips P. E, Rushworth M. F. Weighing up the benefits of work: behavioral and neural analyses of effort-related decision making. Neural Netw. 2006;19:1302–1314.
    1. Botvinick M. M, Huffstetler S, McGuire J. T. Effort discounting in human nucleus accumbens. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2009;9:16–27.
    1. Croxson P. L, Walton M. E, O'Reilly J. X, Behrens T. E, Rushworth M. F. Effort-based cost-benefit valuation and the human brain. J Neurosci. 2009;29:4531–4541.
    1. Brehm J. W, Self E. A. The intensity of motivation. Annu Rev Psychol. 1989;40:109–131.
    1. Yelnik J. Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. Mov Disord. 2002;17(Suppl 3):S15–S21.
    1. Palminteri S, Boraud T, Lafargue G, Dubois B, Pessiglione M. Brain hemispheres selectively track the expected value of contralateral options. J Neurosci. 2009;29:13465–13472.
    1. Lebreton M, Jorge S, Michel V, Thirion B, Pessiglione M. An automatic valuation system in the human brain: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuron. 2009;64:431–439.
    1. Hare T. A, Camerer C. F, Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science. 2009;324:646–648.
    1. Plassmann H, O'Doherty J, Rangel A. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. J Neurosci. 2007;27:9984–9988.
    1. Hampton A. N, Bossaerts P, O'Doherty J. P. The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in abstract state-based inference during decision making in humans. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8360–8367.
    1. Mogenson G. J, Jones D. L, Yim C. Y. From motivation to action: functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Prog Neurobiol. 1980;14:69–97.
    1. Apicella P, Ljungberg T, Scarnati E, Schultz W. Responses to reward in monkey dorsal and ventral striatum. Exp Brain Res. 1991;85:491–500.
    1. Cromwell H. C, Schultz W. Effects of expectations for different reward magnitudes on neuronal activity in primate striatum. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89:2823–2838.
    1. Lauwereyns J, Watanabe K, Coe B, Hikosaka O. A neural correlate of response bias in monkey caudate nucleus. Nature. 2002;418:413–417.
    1. Samejima K, Ueda Y, Doya K, Kimura M. Representation of action-specific reward values in the striatum. Science. 2005;310:1337–1340.
    1. Pasquereau B, Nadjar A, Arkadir D, Bezard E, Goillandeau M, et al. Shaping of motor responses by incentive values through the basal ganglia. J Neurosci. 2007;27:1176–1183.
    1. Draganski B, Kherif F, Kloppel S, Cook P. A, Alexander D. C, et al. Evidence for segregated and integrative connectivity patterns in the human basal ganglia. J Neurosci. 2008;28:7143–7152.
    1. Grabli D, McCairn K, Hirsch E. C, Agid Y, Feger J, et al. Behavioural disorders induced by external globus pallidus dysfunction in primates: I. Behavioural study. Brain. 2004;127:2039–2054.
    1. Worbe Y, Baup N, Grabli D, Chaigneau M, Mounayar S, et al. Behavioral and movement disorders induced by local inhibitory dysfunction in primate striatum. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:1844–1856.
    1. Baron M. S, Wichmann T, Ma D, DeLong M. R. Effects of transient focal inactivation of the basal ganglia in parkinsonian primates. J Neurosci. 2002;22:592–599.
    1. Mallet L, Schupbach M, N′Diaye K, Remy P, Bardinet E, et al. Stimulation of subterritories of the subthalamic nucleus reveals its role in the integration of the emotional and motor aspects of behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:10661–10666.
    1. Friston K. Causal modelling and brain connectivity in functional magnetic resonance imaging. PLoS Biol. 2009;7:e33. doi: .
    1. Laplane D, Dubois B. Auto-activation deficit: a basal ganglia related syndrome. Mov Disord. 2001;16:810–814.
    1. Schmidt L, d'Arc B. F, Lafargue G, Galanaud D, Czernecki V, et al. Disconnecting force from money: effects of basal ganglia damage on incentive motivation. Brain. 2008;131:1303–1310.
    1. Gershman S. J, Pesaran B, Daw N. D. Human reinforcement learning subdivides structured action spaces by learning effector-specific values. J Neurosci. 2009;29:13524–13531.
    1. Wunderlich K, Rangel A, O'Doherty J. P. Neural computations underlying action-based decision making in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:17199–17204.
    1. Deichmann R, Gottfried J. A, Hutton C, Turner R. Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2003;19:430–441.
    1. Weiskopf N, Hutton C, Josephs O, Deichmann R. Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: a whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T. Neuroimage. 2006;33:493–504.
    1. Gitelman D. R, Penny W. D, Ashburner J, Friston K. J. Modeling regional and psychophysiologic interactions in fMRI: the importance of hemodynamic deconvolution. Neuroimage. 2003;19:200–207.
    1. Friston K. J, Buechel C, Fink G. R, Morris J, Rolls E, et al. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 1997;6:218–229.
    1. den Ouden H. E, Daunizeau J, Roiser J, Friston K. J, Stephan K. E. Striatal prediction error modulates cortical coupling. J Neurosci. 2009;30:3210–3219.
    1. Stephan K. E, Penny W. D, Daunizeau J, Moran R. J, Friston K. J. Bayesian model selection for group studies. Neuroimage. 2009;46:1004–1017.

Source: PubMed

3
구독하다