The detrimental impacts of negative age stereotypes on the episodic memory of older adults: does social participation moderate the effects?

Stephen C Y Chan, Alma M L Au, Simon M K Lai, Stephen C Y Chan, Alma M L Au, Simon M K Lai

Abstract

Background: Older adults' cognitive abilities can be impaired through priming of negative age stereotypes. However, it is unclear whether the effects of negative priming can be extended to episodic memory, which is believed to be the most age-sensitive type among the long-term memory systems, in Asian populations. Social participation has recently emerged as a potential protective factor for maintaining the cognitive function of older adults. The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of negative age stereotype priming on episodic memory and the moderating role of social participation in the priming effect.

Methods: A total of 105 community-dwelling older adults residing in Hong Kong were randomly allocated to two experimental conditions. Participants were primed either with negative age stereotype words (n = 53) or neutral words (n = 52) using an implicit priming task. Episodic memory performance was assessed using the Hong Kong List Learning Task (HKLLT), which includes total learning, two delayed recalls and a recognition task. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to assess group differences in the priming task and memory performance, while a series of moderation analyses were performed to examine the moderating effects of social participation.

Results: The group that received negative age stereotype priming performed significantly worse than the group that received neutral words in their episodic memory test. Additional analyses showed that socially active individuals might be less prone to the effects of negative age stereotypes for the recognition task only.

Conclusions: Older adults who are more socially active might be more immune to the effects of negative age stereotype priming on episodic memory. These results provide initial support for the hypothesis that social participation might act as an effective strategy to ward against negative age stereotype priming.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov: NCT04202120 (first posted December 17, 2019), (Retrospectively registered).

Keywords: Age stereotypes; Episodic memory; Older adults; Social participation.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
An example of the flow of the masked priming for one typical trial
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Research flow of the present study
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Moderation effect of social participation between experimental group and reaction time to positive emotional words
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Moderation effect of social participation between experimental group and recognition trial performance

References

    1. Popham LE, Hess TM. Theories of age stereotyping and views of aging. In: Pachana NA (ed) Encyclopedia of geropsychology. Singapore: Springer; 2015. pp. 1–10.
    1. Hummert M, Garstka T, Shaner J, Strahm S. Stereotypes of the elderly held by young, middle-aged, and elderly adults. J Gerontol. 1994;49(5):P240–P249.
    1. Jamieson J, Harkins S. Distinguishing between the effects of stereotype priming and stereotype threat on math performance. Group Processes Intergroup Relations. 2011;15(3):291–304.
    1. Hess T, Hinson J, Hodges E. Moderators of and mechanisms underlying stereotype threat effects on older Adults’ memory performance. Exp Aging Res. 2009;35(2):153–177.
    1. Levy B. Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;71(6):1092–1107.
    1. Levy B, Ryall A, Pilver C, Sheridan P, Wei J, Hausdorff J. Influence of African American elders’ age stereotypes on their cardiovascular response to stress. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2007;21(1):85–93.
    1. Hausdorff J, Levy B, Wei J. The power of ageism on physical function of older persons: reversibility of age-related gait changes. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47(11):1346–1349.
    1. Levy B. Stereotype embodiment. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;18(6):332–336.
    1. Levy B, Slade M, Kasl S. Longitudinal benefit of positive self-perceptions of aging on functional health. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57(5):P409–P417.
    1. Wurm S, Tesch-Römer C, Tomasik M. Longitudinal findings on aging-related cognitions, control beliefs, and health in later life. J Gerontol. 2007;62(3):P156–P164.
    1. Mock S, Eibach R. Aging attitudes moderate the effect of subjective age on psychological well-being: evidence from a 10-year longitudinal study. Psychol Aging. 2011;26(4):979–986.
    1. Sarkisian C, Hays R, Mangione C. Do older adults expect to age successfully? The association between expectations regarding aging and beliefs regarding healthcare seeking among older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50(11):1837–1843.
    1. Levy B, Slade M, Kunkel S, Kasl S. Longevity increased by positive self-perceptions of aging. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83(2):261–270.
    1. Horton S, Baker J, Pearce G, Deakin J. On the malleability of performance. J Appl Gerontol. 2008;27(4):446–465.
    1. Meisner B. A meta-analysis of positive and negative age stereotype priming effects on behavior among older adults. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2011;67B(1):13–17.
    1. Hess T, Hinson J, Statham J. Explicit and implicit stereotype activation effects on memory: do age and awareness moderate the impact of priming? Psychol Aging. 2004;19(3):495–505.
    1. Levy B, Leifheit-Limson E. The stereotype-matching effect: greater influence on functioning when age stereotypes correspond to outcomes. Psychol Aging. 2009;24(1):230–233.
    1. Chan AS. Hong Kong list learning test. 2nd Edn. Hong Kong: Department of Psychological and Integrative Neuropsychological Rehabilitation Center. 2006.
    1. Vauclair C, Hanke K, Huang L, Abrams D. Are Asian cultures really less ageist than Western ones? It depends on the questions asked. Int J Psychol. 2016;52(2):136–144.
    1. Bai X, Chow N. Chinese Elders’ self-image and their perceived peer-image: possibility of self-enhancement bias. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2013;77(1):1–16.
    1. Bourassa K, Memel M, Woolverton C, Sbarra D. Social participation predicts cognitive functioning in aging adults over time: comparisons with physical health, depression, and physical activity. Aging Ment Health. 2015;21(2):133–146.
    1. Berkman L, Glass T, Brissette I, Seeman T. From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Soc Sci Med. 2000;51(6):843–857.
    1. Moser C, Spagnoli J, Santos-Eggimann B. Self-perception of aging and vulnerability to adverse outcomes at the age of 65-70 years. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2011;66B(6):675–680.
    1. Sánchez Palacios C, Trianes Torres M, Blanca MM. Negative aging stereotypes and their relation with psychosocial variables in the elderly population. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2009;48(3):385–390.
    1. Chiu S, Yu S. An excess of culture: the myth of shared care in the Chinese community in Britain. Ageing Soc. 2001;21(6):681–699.
    1. Stein R, Blanchard-Fields F, Hertzog C. The effects of age-stereotype priming on the memory performance of older adults. Exp Aging Res. 2002;28(2):169–181.
    1. Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [E-Prime 2.0]. (2012). Retrieved from .
    1. Fazio R, Sanbonmatsu D, Powell M, Kardes F. On the automatic activation of attitudes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;50(2):229–238.
    1. Wong A, Xiong Y, Kwan P, Chan A, Lam W, Wang K, Chu W, Nyenhuis D, Nasreddine Z, Wong L, Mok V. The validity, reliability and clinical utility of the Hong Kong Montreal cognitive assessment (HK-MoCA) in patients with cerebral small vessel disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;28(1):81–87.
    1. Yeung M, Sze S, Woo J, Kwok T, Shum D, Yu R, Chan A. Reduced frontal activations at high working memory load in mild cognitive impairment: near-infrared spectroscopy. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2016;42(5–6):278–296.
    1. Levasseur M, Richard L, Gauvin L, Raymond É. Inventory and analysis of definitions of social participation found in the aging literature: proposed taxonomy of social activities. Soc Sci Med. 2010;71(12):2141–2149.
    1. Claesson L, Blomstrand J, Eklund K, Eriksson K, Dahlin-Ivanoff S. Comparison of visual acuity charts identifying visual impairment among older people outside the eye clinic. Disabil Rehabil. 2012;35(16):1394–1400.
    1. Hayes A. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. 2. New York: Guilford Publications; 2018.
    1. Yeung P, Wong L, Chan C, Leung J, Yung C. A validation study of the Hong Kong version of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (HK-MoCA) in Chinese older adults in Hong Kong. Hong Kong Med J. 2014;20:504.
    1. Au A, Chan A, Chiu H. Verbal learning in Alzheimer’s dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9(3):363–375.
    1. Schmader T, Johns M, Forbes C. An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychol Rev. 2008;115(2):336–356.
    1. Lee K, Lee H. Priming effects of age stereotypes on memory of older adults in Korea. Asian J Soc Psychol. 2018;22(1):39–46.
    1. Cabeza R, Kapur S, Craik FIM, Mcintosh AR, Houle S, Tulving E. Functional Neuroanatomy of recall and recognition: a PET study of episodic memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9(2):254–265.
    1. Armstrong B, Gallant SN, Li L, Patel K, Wong BI. Stereotype Threat Effects on Older Adults’ Episodic and Working Memory: A Meta-Analysis. The Gerontologist. 2017;57(suppl_2):S193.
    1. Adams SK, Leibbrant S, Moon H. A critical review of the literature on social and leisure activity and wellbeing in later life. Ageing Soc. 2010;31(4):683–712.
    1. Pan X, Chee K. The power of weak ties in preserving cognitive function: a longitudinal study of older Chinese adults. Aging Ment Health. 2020;24(7):1046–53.
    1. Okun MA, Stock WA, Haring MJ, Witter RA. The social activity/subjective well-being relation. Res Aging. 1984;6(1):45–65.
    1. Zhang Z, Zhang J. Social participation and subjective well-being among retirees in China. Soc Indic Res. 2014;123(1):143–160.

Source: PubMed

3
구독하다