Enhanced inter-regional coupling of neural responses and repetition suppression provide separate contributions to long-term behavioral priming

Stephen J Gotts, Shawn C Milleville, Alex Martin, Stephen J Gotts, Shawn C Milleville, Alex Martin

Abstract

Stimulus identification commonly improves with repetition over long delays ("repetition priming"), whereas neural activity commonly decreases ("repetition suppression"). Multiple models have been proposed to explain this brain-behavior relationship, predicting alterations in functional and/or effective connectivity (Synchrony and Predictive Coding models), in the latency of neural responses (Facilitation model), and in the relative similarity of neural representations (Sharpening model). Here, we test these predictions with fMRI during overt and covert naming of repeated and novel objects. While we find partial support for predictions of the Facilitation and Sharpening models in the left fusiform gyrus and left frontal cortex, the data were most consistent with the Synchrony model, with increased coupling between right temporoparietal and anterior cingulate cortex for repeated objects that correlated with priming magnitude across participants. Increased coupling and repetition suppression varied independently, each explaining unique variance in priming and requiring modifications of all current models.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT00001360.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1. Neural models of repetition priming.
Fig. 1. Neural models of repetition priming.
Four prominent models of repetition priming and repetition suppression are considered. The Synchrony model (upper left) holds that neural activity becomes more synchronized with repetition, permitting more coordinated propagation of activity at lower overall activity levels. The Predictive Coding model (upper right) holds that top-down causal influences are more strongly negative, leading to repetition suppression in the receiving region, along with a gain enhancement that leads to more rapid onset and offset of responses. The Facilitation model (lower left) claims that neural activity onset and offset are advanced in time, with earlier peak responses and a reduction in overall activity. The Sharpening model (lower right) holds that weakly tuned, poorly responsive cells are the ones driving repetition suppression, reducing downstream support for competing stimulus identities and speeding downstream stimulus-selective responses. Figure reproduced with permission from Gotts et al..
Fig. 2. Overt and covert picture naming…
Fig. 2. Overt and covert picture naming tasks.
Participants were instructed to name pictures out loud (Overt Naming) or silently to themselves, pressing a button to mark the naming time (Covert Naming). Trials were structured in a slow event-related design in order to separate the peak responses of individual trials, and jitter of 8–14 s occurred from trial onset to trial onset. Name responses were marked for correctness and the onset time of the voice/button response was recorded for each trial. For analyses of local activity, an empirical hemodynamic response function was estimated across trials with a separate regressor at each timepoint following stimulus onset. For connectivity analyses, the peak BOLD response was calculated for each trial and in each fMRI voxel by averaging the 2 timepoints (4–6 and 6–8 s) adjacent to the expected peak of the hemodynamic response function, with a single peak value saved for each trial in order to minimize the contribution of the temporal contour of the evoked responses from functional and effective connectivity estimates.
Fig. 3. Regions engaged in object naming…
Fig. 3. Regions engaged in object naming and showing repetition effects.
a Above-baseline responses to Overt and Covert Naming tasks are shown at two levels of significance, one at a minimum level of significance (pooling the tasks, P < 0.05, FDR q < 0.05; shown in orange) and another at a more restrictive level of significance (P < 0.0001, FDR q < 0.00016 in each task individually) for which responses can be said to replicate across tasks (shown in red). b Repetition effects, either repetition suppression (blue colors) or repetition enhancement (orange), are shown at two levels of significance, one at a minimum level of significance (pooling the tasks, P < 0.05, FDR q < 0.05; suppression shown in light blue, enhancement shown in orange) and another at a more restrictive level of significance (P < 0.00001, FDR q < 0.00006 in each task individually). Repetition effects were masked by the less restrictive threshold in (a), as all theories being evaluated make claims about regions engaged at above-baseline levels during the tasks. Statistics in (a) and (b) are based on N = 32 independent participant datasets for Overt Naming and N = 28 independent datasets for Covert Naming. c Repetition suppression was highly correlated (Pearson) with priming magnitude in left frontal cortex in terms of effect size (Cohen’s d) across N = 60 independent participants, combining both Overt and Covert Naming (corrected for multiple comparisons by both Bonferroni and FDR). The left frontal region is the same as the dark blue region showing Repetition Suppression (RS) at the more stringent threshold in (b).
Fig. 4. Repetition priming and Primeability.
Fig. 4. Repetition priming and Primeability.
a When averaged across participants within each task, priming magnitudes specific to each of the 200 stimuli are highly reliable across task. For the Covert Naming task, these priming magnitudes were assessed in a post-fMRI session during which participants overtly named all stimuli (either pre-exposed as OLD or novel to the fMRI session as NEW). b When responses are averaged across both participants and tasks, a strong relationship was observed between average response time (RT) to an object when NEW and the subsequent priming magnitude observed. Items were subjected to a median split based on the RT when NEW (using the group-average normative data) to classify objects as either Strong Primeable (slow RT) or Weak Primeable (fast RT), permitting a within-participant measure of “Primeability”. Statistics in (a) and (b) are based on N = 200 independent stimuli. c When grouping objects by Primeability (either Strong or Weak), priming effect sizes for each participant were indeed greater for Strong compared to Weak Primeable objects in the Overt Naming task (left panel); this is expected since these responses contributed to the original calculation of Primeability. However, this same relationship was also found for the Covert Naming button-press response times during fMRI, which were not used in calculating Primeability (right panel). The middle horizontal line in each box plot represents the median (50th %ile), the horizontal lines just above and below the median represent the 25th and 75th %iles, the top and bottom horizontal lines represent the minimum and maximum values, and the boundaries of the horizontal notches inside the 25th and 75th %iles depict the 95% confidence limits of the median. Individual datapoints are plotted as open circles. Statistics in (c) are based on N = 32 independent participant datasets for Overt Naming and N = 28 independent datasets for Covert Naming. For related content in SI, see Supplementary Fig. 1.
Fig. 5. Across- and within-participant relationships between…
Fig. 5. Across- and within-participant relationships between repetition suppression and priming.
a Correlations (Pearson) across participants between Repetition Supression (RS) magnitude (NEW–OLD) and priming effect size (Cohen’s d) reveal a significant relationship in left frontal cortex and non-significant trends in left and right fusiform regions (left-most plot same as Fig. 3c). b Separating trials into Strong and Weak Primeable conditions show that significant within-participant relationships between RS and priming are found in left frontal, left fusiform, and ACC regions (greater RS in the Strong Primeable condition by paired t-tests). The middle horizontal line in each box plot represents the median (50th %ile), the horizontal lines just below and above the median represent the 25th and 75th %iles, the bottom and top horizontal lines represent the minimum and maximum values, and the boundaries of the horizontal notches inside the 25th and 75th %iles depict the 95% confidence limits of the median. Individual datapoints are plotted as open circles. Statistics (paired t-tests) are based on N = 60 independent participants (combining Overt and Covert Naming), and multiple comparisons were corrected by FDR (q < 0.05).
Fig. 6. Functional connectivity shows interaction between…
Fig. 6. Functional connectivity shows interaction between Primeability and Repetition.
a A right temporoparietal (R TP) region exhibited an interaction in whole-brain connectedness between Repetition and Primeability, similar to that seen in the behavioral priming results (P < 0.001, corrected to P < 0.025). When used as a seed, R TP jointly exhibited this interaction with the anterior cingulate (ACC), right putamen, right STG, and the right fusiform gyrus. b Regions showing a Repetition × Primeability interaction were combined with regions showing repetition suppression in both Overt and Covert Naming Tasks. c Region-by-region functional connectivity interactions of Repetition × Primeability are shown for all 7 regions (left panel). FDR-corrected effects are indicated by black boxes (P < 0.01, q < 0.05). Region-by-region comparisons of OLD versus NEW functional connectivity are shown for Strong and Weak Primeable conditions separately in the right panels. Warm colors (red) indicate OLD > NEW and cool colors (blue) indicate NEW > OLD. FDR-corrected effects (P < 0.0286, q < 0.05) were calculated among all region-by-region combinations showing a significant Repetition × Primeability interaction, indicated by black squares. Statistics are based on N = 60 independent participants, organized into a factorial design with Task (Overt, Covert Naming) as a between-participant variable. For related content in SI, see Supplementary Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 1.
Fig. 7. Effective connectivity increases between R…
Fig. 7. Effective connectivity increases between R TP cortex and ACC correlate with priming magnitude.
a Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to estimate effective connectivity among the 7 regions. The optimal 10-parameter SEM model is shown, with arrows indicating causal directionality and connections exhibiting a significant Repetition × Primeability interaction (P < 0.0063, q < 0.05) shown with thick red arrows (non-significant interactions shown with black arrows). b Region-by-region comparisons of SEM parameters for OLD versus NEW objects are shown separately for the Strong and Weak Primeable conditions. For directionality, sending ROIs are listed along the x-axes and receiving ROIs are listed along the y-axes. FDR-corrected OLD/NEW comparisons (P < 0.0091, q < 0.05) were assessed among connections exhibiting a Repetition × Primeability interaction, indicated with black squares. c The connection from the R TP ROI to ACC ROI shows a Repetition × Primeability interaction that is consistent with the Synchrony model (with increased coupling for OLD objects in the Strong Primeable condition). The middle horizontal line in each box plot represents the median (50th %ile), the horizontal lines just below and above the median represent the 25th and 75th %iles, the bottom and top horizontal lines represent the minimum and maximum values, and the boundaries of the horizontal notches inside the 25th and 75th %iles depict the 95% confidence limits of the median. Individual datapoints are plotted as open circles. d The R TP to ACC connection further exhibited a correlation across participants with observed priming magnitude, assessed by effect size (Cohen’s d). This correlation appeared to be driven by the Strong Primeable condition (rightmost panel), with priming effect size in the Strong Primeable condition correlated with the difference between OLD and NEW SEM parameters in the Strong Primeable condition. Statistics are based on N = 60 independent participants, organized into a factorial design with Task (Overt, Covert Naming) as a between-participant variable (Primeability and Repetition are both within-participant variables). For related content in SI, see Supplementary Fig. 3.
Fig. 8. MVPA tests of the Sharpening…
Fig. 8. MVPA tests of the Sharpening model.
a Spatial correlations of peak responses on each individual trial were calculated across all trials per condition type using the full extent of the four Repetition Suppression (RS) clusters (see Supplementary Table 1), retaining the median inter-item correlation per participant. A significant Repetition × Primeability interaction was observed for the Left Frontal and ACC ROIs (corrected by FDR, q < 0.05). The middle horizontal line in each box plot represents the median (50th %ile), the horizontal lines just below and above the median represent the 25th and 75th %iles, the bottom and top horizontal lines represent the minimum and maximum values, and the boundaries of the horizontal notches inside the 25th and 75th %iles depict the 95% confidence limits of the median. Individual datapoints are plotted as open circles. b Strong covariation of the spatial correlations with average beta coefficients and estimated signal-to-noise ratio eliminated most of the differences between conditions after adjustment for these variables, leaving only a significant Repetition × Primeability interaction in the Left Frontal ROI. Bar plots are used along with standard error of the mean (SE) estimates, since these adjusted means and model-residual error are not defined on the individual participants and only on the full LME model. Statistics are based on N = 60 independent participants, organized into a factorial design with Task (Overt, Covert Naming) as a between-participant variable (Primeability and Repetition are within-participant variables). For related content in SI, see Supplementary Fig. 4.
Fig. 9. Assessing activity timing predictions of…
Fig. 9. Assessing activity timing predictions of the Facilitation and Predictive Coding models.
A hemodynamic response function model (gamma variate) was fit to the beta coefficients at each timepoint (TR) for each participant and experimental condition, permitting estimates of the peak time (tpeak) (see graphic at the bottom). Group-average response functions are shown with dashed lines for each condition, and group-average estimates of peak times are shown with vertical dotted lines (mean data on the actual measured beta coefficients are shown with solid lines). Main effects of Repetition on peak time were observed in the Left Fusiform and Left Frontal ROIs (OLD peaks earlier than NEW peaks), but there were no significant interactions between Repetition and Primeability on peak time. Statistics are based on N = 60 independent participants, organized into a factorial design with Task (Overt, Covert Naming) as a between-participant variable. A color key for the experimental conditions is shown at the bottom right (OLD, Strong Primeable = red; OLD, Weak Primeable = orange; NEW, Strong Primeable = gray; NEW, Weak Primeable = black).

References

    1. Cave CB, Squire LR. Intact and long-lasting repetition priming in amnesia. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1992;18:509–520. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.509.
    1. Tulving E, Schacter DL. Priming and human memory systems. Science. 1990;247:301–306. doi: 10.1126/science.2296719.
    1. Cave CB. Very long-lasting priming in picture naming. Psychol. Sci. 1997;8:322–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00446.x.
    1. Mitchell DB. Nonconscious priming after 17 years: invulnerable implicit memory? Psychol. Sci. 2006;17:925–929. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01805.x.
    1. Wiggs CL, Weisberg J, Martin A. Repetition priming across the adult lifespan—the long and short of it. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2006;13:308–325. doi: 10.1080/138255890968718.
    1. Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol. Rev. 1992;99:195–231. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.195.
    1. Desimone R. Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1996;93:13494–13499. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13494.
    1. Henson RN. Neuroimaging studies of priming. Prog. Neurobiol. 2003;70:53–81. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(03)00086-8.
    1. Schacter DL, Buckner RL. Priming and the brain. Neuron. 1998;20:185–195. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80448-1.
    1. Wiggs CL, Martin A. Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1998;8:227–233. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80144-X.
    1. Li L, Miller EK, Desimone R. The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1993;69:1918–1929. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.69.6.1918.
    1. van Turennout M, Bielamowicz L, Martin A. Modulation of neural activity during object naming: effects of time and practice. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:381–391. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.4.381.
    1. Gotts SJ. Incremental learning of perceptual and conceptual representations and the puzzle of neural repetition suppression. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2016;23:1055–1071. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0855-y.
    1. McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O’Reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 1995;102:419–457. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419.
    1. Norman KA, Newman EL, Detre GJ, Polyn SM. How inhibitory oscillations can train neural networks and punish competitors. Neural Comput. 2006;18:1577–1610. doi: 10.1162/neco.2006.18.7.1577.
    1. Gotts SJ, Chow CC, Martin A. Repetition priming and repetition suppression: a case for enhanced efficiency through neural synchronization. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012;3:227–237. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2012.670617.
    1. Ghuman AS, Bar M, Dobbins IG, Schnyer DM. The effects of priming on frontal-temporal communication. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:8405–8409. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710674105.
    1. Gilbert JR, Gotts SJ, Carver FW, Martin A. Object repetition leads to local increases in the temporal coordination of neural responses. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010;4:30.
    1. Friston KJ. A theory of cortical responses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2005;360:815–836. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622.
    1. Friston KJ, Kiebel SJ. Predictive coding under the free-energy principle. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009;364:1211–1221. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0300.
    1. Friston KJ. Predictive coding, precision and synchrony. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012;3:238–239. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2012.691277.
    1. James TW, Gauthier I. Repetition-induced changes in BOLD response reflect accumulation of neural activity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2006;27:37–45. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20165.
    1. James TW, Humphreys GK, Gati JS, Menon RS, Goodale MA. The effects of visual object priming on brain activation before and after recognition. Curr. Biol. 2000;10:1017–1024. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00655-2.
    1. Ewbank MP, Henson RN. Explaining away repetition effects via predictive coding. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012;3:239–240. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2012.689960.
    1. Gotts SJ, Milleville SC, Martin A. Object identification leads to a conceptual broadening of object representations in lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2015;76:62–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.041.
    1. van Turennout M, Ellmore T, Martin A. Long-lasting cortical plasticity in the object naming system. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3:1329–1334. doi: 10.1038/81873.
    1. Gilmore AW, Kalinowski SE, Milleville SC, Gotts SJ, Martin A. Identifying task-general effects of stimulus familiarity in the parietal memory network. Neuropsychologia. 2019;124:31–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.12.023.
    1. Kan IP, Thompson-Schill SL. Effect of name agreement on prefrontal activity during overt and covert picture naming. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2004;4:43–57. doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.1.43.
    1. Dobbins IG, Schnyer DM, Verfaellie M, Schacter DL. Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature. 2004;428:316–319. doi: 10.1038/nature02400.
    1. Maccotta L, Buckner RL. Evidence for neural effects of repetition that directly correlate with behavioral priming. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004;16:1625–1632. doi: 10.1162/0898929042568451.
    1. Horner AJ, Henson RN. Priming, response learning and repetition suppression. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1979–1991. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.018.
    1. Lebreton M, Bavard S, Daunizeau J, Palminteri S. Assessing inter-individual differences with task-related functional neuroimaging. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2019;3:897–905. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0681-8.
    1. Bainbridge WA. The memorability of people: intrinsic memorability across transformations of a person’s face. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2017;43:706–716. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000339.
    1. Gotts SJ, et al. Fractionation of social brain circuits in autism spectrum disorders. Brain. 2012;135:2711–2725. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws160.
    1. Saad ZS, et al. Correcting brain-wide correlation differences in resting-state FMRI. Brain Connect. 2013;3:339–352. doi: 10.1089/brain.2013.0156.
    1. Reid AT, et al. Advancing functional connectivity research from association to causation. Nat. Neurosci. 2019;22:1751–1760. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0510-4.
    1. Chen G, et al. Vector autoregression, structural equation modeling, and their synthesis in neuroimaging data analysis. Comput. Biol. Med. 2011;41:1142–1155. doi: 10.1016/j.compbiomed.2011.09.004.
    1. Haxby JV, et al. Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science. 2001;293:2425–2430. doi: 10.1126/science.1063736.
    1. Ramirez FM. Orientation encoding and viewpoint invariance in face recognition: inferring neural properties from large-scale signals. Neuroscientist. 2018;24:582–608. doi: 10.1177/1073858418769554.
    1. Ramirez FM, Merriam EP. Forward models of repetition suppression depend critically on assumptions of noise and granularity. Nat. Commun. 2020;11:4732. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-18315-w.
    1. Tong F, Harrison SA, Dewey JA, Kamitani Y. Relationship between BOLD amplitude and pattern classification of orientation-selective activity in the human visual cortex. NeuroImage. 2012;63:1212–1222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.005.
    1. Ramot M, Walsh C, Martin A. Multifaceted integration: memory for faces is subserved by widespread connections between visual, memory, auditory, and social networks. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:4976–4985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0217-19.2019.
    1. Korzeniewska A, et al. Changes in human brain dynamics during behavioral priming and repetition suppression. Prog. Neurobiol. 2020;18:101788. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2020.101788.
    1. Alink A, Abdulrahman H, Henson RN. Forward models demonstrate that repetition suppression is best modelled by local neural scaling. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:3854. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05957-0.
    1. De Baene W, Vogels R. Effects of adaptation on the stimulus selectivity of macaque inferior temporal spiking activity and local field potentials. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:2145–2165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp277.
    1. McMahon DB, Olson CR. Repetition suppression in monkey inferotemporal cortex: relation to behavioral priming. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:3532–3543. doi: 10.1152/jn.01042.2006.
    1. Weiner KS, Sayres R, Vinberg J, Grill-Spector K. fMRI-adaptation and category selectivity in human ventral temporal cortex: regional differences across time scales. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;103:3349–3365. doi: 10.1152/jn.01108.2009.
    1. Mattar MG, Olkkonen M, Epstein RA, Aguirre GK. Adaptation decorrelates shape representations. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:3812. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06278-y.
    1. Boettiger CA, D’Esposito M. Frontal networks for learning and executing arbitrary stimulus-response associations. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:2723–2732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3697-04.2005.
    1. Packard MG, Knowlton BJ. Learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;25:563–593. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142937.
    1. Henson RN, Eckstein D, Waszak F, Frings C, Horner AJ. Stimulus-response bindings in priming. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:376–384. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.004.
    1. Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Moscovitch M. Cognitive contributions of the ventral parietal cortex: an integrative theoretical account. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012;16:338–352. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.008.
    1. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2011;34:569–599. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731.
    1. Smith JF, Pillai A, Chen K, Horwitz B. Effective connectivity modeling for fMRI: six issues and possible solutions using linear dynamical systems. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2012;5:104. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00104.
    1. Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W. Dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage. 2003;19:1273–1302. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00202-7.
    1. Persichetti AS, Avery JA, Huber L, Merriam EP, Martin A. Layer-specific contributions to imagined and executed hand movements in human primary motor cortex. Curr. Biol. 2020;30:1721–1725. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.046.
    1. Balota DA, et al. The English lexicon project. Behav. Res. Methods. 2007;39:445–459. doi: 10.3758/BF03193014.
    1. Bandettini PA, Cox RW. Event-related fMRI contrast when using constant interstimulus interval: theory and experiment. Magn. Reson. Med. 2000;43:540–548. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1522-2594(200004)43:4<540::AID-MRM8>;2-R.
    1. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018.
    1. Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014.
    1. Jo HJ, Saad ZS, Simmons WK, Milbury LA, Cox RW. Mapping sources of correlation in resting state FMRI, with artifact detection and removal. NeuroImage. 2010;52:571–582. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.246.
    1. Fischl B, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:341–355. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X.
    1. Glover GH, Li TQ, Ress D. Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magn. Reson. Med. 2000;44:162–167. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200007)44:1<162::AID-MRM23>;2-E.
    1. Birn RM, Smith MA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA. The respiration response function: the temporal dynamics of fMRI signal fluctuations related to changes in respiration. NeuroImage. 2008;40:644–654. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.059.
    1. Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage. 2007;37:90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042.
    1. Stoddard J, et al. Aberrant intrinsic functional connectivity within and between corticostriatal and temporal–parietal networks in adults and youth with bipolar disorder. Psychol. Med. 2016;46:1509–1522. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716000143.
    1. Davis SW, et al. Visual and semantic representations predict subsequent memory in perceptual and conceptual memory tests. Cereb. Cortex. 2021;31:974–992. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa269.
    1. Genovese C, Lazar NA, Nichols T. Thesholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. NeuroImage. 2002;15:870–878. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1037.
    1. Cox RW, Chen G, Glen DR, Reynolds RC, Taylor PA. FMRI clustering in AFNI: false-positive rates redux. Brain Connect. 2017;7:152–171. doi: 10.1089/brain.2016.0475.
    1. Eklund A, Nichols T, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:7900–7905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113.
    1. Berman RA, et al. Disrupted sensorimotor and social–cognitive networks underlie symptoms in childhood-onset schizophrenia. Brain. 2016;139:276–291. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv306.
    1. Jasmin K, et al. Overt social interaction and resting state in young adult males with autism: core and contextual neural features. Brain. 2019;142:808–822. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz003.
    1. Cole MW, Pathak S, Schneider W. Identifying the brain’s most globally connected regions. NeuroImage. 2010;49:3132–3148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.001.
    1. Salomon R, et al. Global functional connectivity deficits in schizophrenia depend on behavioral state. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:12972–12981. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2987-11.2011.
    1. Meoded A, Morrissette AR, Schanz O, Gotts SJ, Floeter MK. Cerebro- cerebellar connectivity is increased in primary lateral sclerosis. NeuroImage Clin. 2015;7:288–296. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.12.009.
    1. Smith REW, et al. Sex differences in resting-state functional connectivity of the cerebellum in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019;13:104. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00104.
    1. Watson CE, Gotts SJ, Martin A, Buxbaum LJ. Bilateral functional connectivity at rest predicts apraxic symptoms after left hemisphere stroke. NeuroImage Clin. 2019;21:101526. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.08.033.
    1. Song S, Gotts SJ, Dayan E, Cohen LG. Practice structure improves unconscious transitional memories by increasing synchrony in a premotor network. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015;27:1503–1512. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00796.
    1. Steel A, et al. Shifts in connectivity during procedural learning after motor cortex stimulation: a combined transcranial magnetic stimulation/functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Cortex. 2016;74:134–148. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.10.004.
    1. Chen G, Saad ZS, Britton JC, Pine DS, Cox RW. Linear mixed-effects modeling approach to FMRI group analysis. NeuroImage. 2013;73:176–190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.047.
    1. Gotts SJ, et al. The perils of global signal regression for group comparisons: a case study of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:356. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00356.
    1. Gotts SJ, Gilmore AW, Martin A. Brain networks, dimensionality, and global signal averaging in resting-state fMRI: hierarchical network structure results in low-dimensional spatiotemporal dynamics. NeuroImage. 2020;205:116289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116289.
    1. Friston KJ. Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1994;2:56–78. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020107.
    1. Friston KJ. Functional and effective connectivity: a review. Brain Connect. 2011;1:13–36. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0008.
    1. McIntosh AR, Gonzalez-Lima F. Structural equation modeling and its application to network analysis in functional brain imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1994;2:2–22. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020104.
    1. Price LR, Laird AR, Fox PT, Ingham RJ. Modeling dynamic functional neuroimaging data using structural equation modeling. Struct. Equ. Modeling. 2009;16:147–162. doi: 10.1080/10705510802561402.
    1. Akaike, H. (1973). Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In 2nd Int. Symp. Information Theory (eds. Petrov, B. N. & Csáki, F.) 267–281 (Springer, 1973).
    1. Misaki M, Kim Y, Bandettini PA, Kriegeskorte N. Comparison of multivariate classifiers and response normalizations for pattern-information fMRI. NeuroImage. 2010;53:103–118. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.051.
    1. Norman KA, Polyn SM, Detre GJ, Haxby JV. Beyond mind reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006;10:424–430. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.07.005.
    1. Op de Beeck HP, Torfs K, Wagemans J. Perceived shape similarity among unfamiliar objects and the organization of the human object vision pathway. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:10111–10123. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2511-08.2008.
    1. Spearman C. The proof and measurement of association between two things. Am. J. Psychol. 1904;15:72–101. doi: 10.2307/1412159.
    1. Yeo BT, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011.
    1. Gotts, S. J. Mechanisms Underlying Enhanced Processing Efficiency in Neural Systems (Carnegie Mellon University, 2003).

Source: PubMed

3
구독하다