Working Memory Training in Alcohol Use Disorder: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Lotfi Khemiri, Christoffer Brynte, Angela Stunkel, Torkel Klingberg, Nitya Jayaram-Lindström, Lotfi Khemiri, Christoffer Brynte, Angela Stunkel, Torkel Klingberg, Nitya Jayaram-Lindström

Abstract

Background: Alcohol use disorder (AUD) is associated with cognitive deficits such as impaired executive functions, which are hypothesized to contribute to the progression of the disease and worsen treatment outcome. Training of working memory (WM) to improve cognitive functions and thereby reduce alcohol use has been proposed as a novel treatment strategy.

Methods: Patients with AUD (n = 50) who were recruited to an outpatient addiction clinic were randomized to receive 5 weeks of active WM training or control training. Participants had weekly follow-up visits, and all cognitive training sessions were done online at home. Primary outcomes were WM function and change in self-reported heavy drinking. Secondary outcomes were craving, other drinking outcomes, and performance on a range of neuropsychological tasks from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery.

Results: The active training group demonstrated a significantly greater improvement in verbal WM compared with the control group. No statistically significant effect of training was found on the primary drinking outcome, but a trend was observed indicating that WM training reduces the number of drinks per drinking occasion. WM training had no statistically significant effect on any of the other neuropsychological tasks.

Conclusions: Cognitive training can improve WM function in individuals with AUD, suggesting that such interventions are feasible to administer in this patient population. The results do not support an effect of WM training on heavy drinking or transfer effects to other cognitive domains. Future studies should evaluate WM training as an adjunct to evidence-based treatments for AUD to assess potential synergistic effects.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT02113618.

Keywords: Alcohol Use Disorder; Cogmed; Cognitive Training; Working Memory.

© 2018 The Authors. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. on behalf of Research Society on Alcoholism.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
CONSORT (Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials) flowchart illustrating the flow of study participants. AUD, alcohol use disorder; WM, working memory; ITT, intention to treat; PP, per protocol.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Digit Span scores at baseline and test day in participants who completed the study. The total score (A) and backward score (B) were significantly improved in the treatment group compared with controls. No statistically significant difference was found for the forward score (C). Values are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean; *p < 0.05.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Spatial Working Memory task performance in participants who completed the study. The main outcomes were total errors (A), strategy (B), between‐errors (C), and within‐errors (D) at baseline and test day for treatment and control groups. There were no significant differences between treatment groups. Values are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Self‐reported drinking in participants who completed the study using the Timeline Followback interview method at baseline (90 days before study) and test day (during study period). There was no significant effect of treatment on percentage of heavy drinking days (A), drinks per drinking day (B), percent drinking days (C), or drinks per day (D), but a trend (p = 0.069) was observed indicating decreased drinks per drinking day in the treatment group compared with the control group (B). Values are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Self‐reported craving (A) and mood (B) at baseline, weekly visits, and test day for treatment group and control group in participants who completed the study. There were no significant differences between treatment groups. Values are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean. Short‐DAQ, Swedish shortened version of the Desire for Alcohol Questionnaire; MADRS‐S, Montgomery–Asberg Depression Self‐Rating Scale.

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th ed., text rev. American Psychiatric Association, Washington DC.
    1. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th ed. American Psychiatric Association, Arlington, VA.
    1. Anton RF, Moak DH, Latham P (1995) The Obsessive Compulsive Drinking Scale: a self‐rated instrument for the quantification of thoughts about alcohol and drinking behavior. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 19:92–99.
    1. Au J, Sheehan E, Tsai N, Duncan GJ, Buschkuehl M, Jaeggi SM (2015) Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta‐analysis. Psychon Bull Rev 22:366–377.
    1. Bäckman L, Nyberg L, Soveri A, Johansson J, Andersson M, Dahlin E, Neely AS, Virta J, Laine M, Rinne JO (2011) Effects of working‐memory training on striatal dopamine release. Science 333:718.
    1. Baddeley A (2003) Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:829–839.
    1. Bickel WK, Moody L, Quisenberry A (2014) Computerized working‐memory training as a candidate adjunctive treatment for addiction. Alcohol Res Curr Rev 36:123–126.
    1. Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, Hill PF, Baxter C (2011) Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol Psychiatry 69:260–265.
    1. Bigorra A, Garolera M, Guijarro S, Hervás A (2016) Long‐term far‐transfer effects of working memory training in children with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 25:853–867.
    1. Bjork JM, Hommer DW, Grant SJ, Danube C (2004) Impulsivity in abstinent alcohol‐dependent patients: relation to control subjects and type 1‐/type 2‐like traits. Alcohol 34:133–150.
    1. Boileau I, Assaad JM, Pihl RO, Benkelfat C, Leyton M, Diksic M, Tremblay RE, Dagher A (2003) Alcohol promotes dopamine release in the human nucleus accumbens. Synapse 49:226–231.
    1. Brehmer Y, Westerberg H, Bellander M, Fürth D, Karlsson S, Bäckman L (2009) Working memory plasticity modulated by dopamine transporter genotype. Neurosci Lett 467:117–120.
    1. Brooks SJ, Wiemerslage L, Burch KH, Maiorana SA, Cocolas E, Schiöth HB, Kamaloodien K, Stein DJ (2017) The impact of cognitive training in substance use disorder: the effect of working memory training on impulse control in methamphetamine users. Psychopharmacology 234:1911–1921.
    1. Chanraud S, Martelli C, Delain F, Kostogianni N, Douaud G, Aubin H‐J, Reynaud M, Martinot J‐L (2007) Brain morphometry and cognitive performance in detoxified alcohol‐dependents with preserved psychosocial functioning. Neuropsychopharmacol Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol 32:429–438.
    1. Conklin HM, Ogg RJ, Ashford JM, Scoggins MA, Zou P, Clark KN, Martin‐Elbahesh K, Hardy KK, Merchant TE, Jeha S, Huang L, Zhang H (2015) Computerized cognitive training for amelioration of cognitive late effects among childhood cancer survivors: a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Oncol Off J Am Soc Clin Oncol 33:3894–3902.
    1. Constantinidis C, Klingberg T (2016) The neuroscience of working memory capacity and training. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:438–449.
    1. Coull JT, Middleton HC, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ (1995) Clonidine and diazepam have differential effects on tests of attention and learning. Psychopharmacology 120:322–332.
    1. Dahlin E, Nyberg L, Bäckman L, Neely AS (2008) Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: immediate training gains, transfer, and long‐term maintenance. Psychol Aging 23:720–730.
    1. Diamond A (2013) Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol 64:135–168.
    1. Di Chiara G, Imperato A (1988) Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85:5274–5278.
    1. Dick DM, Smith G, Olausson P, Mitchell SH, Leeman RF, O'Malley SS, Sher K (2010) Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders. Addict Biol 15:217–226.
    1. Finn PR, Justus A, Mazas C, Steinmetz JE (1999) Working memory, executive processes and the effects of alcohol on Go/No‐Go learning: testing a model of behavioral regulation and impulsivity. Psychopharmacology 146:465–472.
    1. Finn PR, Mazas CA, Justus AN, Steinmetz J (2002) Early‐onset alcoholism with conduct disorder: go/no go learning deficits, working memory capacity, and personality. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 26:186–206.
    1. Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, Wearing H (2004) The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Dev Psychol 40:177–190.
    1. Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, de Beurs E, van den Brink W (2006) Neurocognitive functions in pathological gambling: a comparison with alcohol dependence, Tourette syndrome and normal controls. Addict Abingdon Engl 101:534–547.
    1. Green CT, Long DL, Green D, Iosif A‐M, Dixon JF, Miller MR, Fassbender C, Schweitzer JB (2012) Will working memory training generalize to improve off‐task behavior in children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Neurother J Am Soc Exp Neurother 9:639–648.
    1. Heinz A, Siessmeier T, Wrase J, Buchholz HG, Gründer G, Kumakura Y, Cumming P, Schreckenberger M, Smolka MN, Rösch F, Mann K, Bartenstein P (2005) Correlation of alcohol craving with striatal dopamine synthesis capacity and D2/3 receptor availability: a combined [18F]DOPA and [18F]DMFP PET study in detoxified alcoholic patients. Am J Psychiatry 162:1515–1520.
    1. Hinson JM, Jameson TL, Whitney P (2003) Impulsive decision making and working memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 29:298–306.
    1. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD (2012) Executive functions and self‐regulation. Trends Cogn Sci 16:174–180.
    1. Houben K, Wiers RW, Jansen A (2011) Getting a grip on drinking behavior: training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychol Sci 22:968–975.
    1. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ (2008) Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:6829–6833.
    1. Jurado MB, Rosselli M (2007) The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol Rev 17:213–233.
    1. Khemiri L, Jayaram‐Lindström N, Hammarberg A (2017) Psychometric evaluation of a Swedish version of the Shortened Desires for Alcohol Questionnaire (Shortened‐DAQ). J Subst Abuse Treat 79:61–66.
    1. Khurana A, Romer D, Betancourt LM, Brodsky NL, Giannetta JM, Hurt H (2013) Working memory ability predicts trajectories of early alcohol use in adolescents: the mediational role of impulsivity. Addict Abingdon Engl 108:506–515.
    1. Kirby KN, Maraković NN (1996) Delay‐discounting probabilistic rewards: rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychon Bull Rev 3:100–104.
    1. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK (1999) Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non‐drug‐using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen 128:78–87.
    1. Klingberg T (2010) Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed) 14:317–324.
    1. Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlström K, Gillberg CG, Forssberg H, Westerberg H (2005) Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD–a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44:177–186.
    1. Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H (2002) Training of working memory in children with ADHD. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 24:781–791.
    1. Lawrence AJ, Luty J, Bogdan NA, Sahakian BJ, Clark L (2009a) Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology 207:163–172.
    1. Lawrence AJ, Luty J, Bogdan NA, Sahakian BJ, Clark L (2009b) Problem gamblers share deficits in impulsive decision‐making with alcohol‐dependent individuals. Addict Abingdon Engl 104:1006–1015.
    1. Le Berre A‐P, Fama R, Sullivan EV (2017) Executive functions, memory, and social cognitive deficits and recovery in chronic alcoholism: a critical review to inform future research. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 41:1432–1443.
    1. Le Berre A‐P, Vabret F, Cauvin C, Pinon K, Allain P, Pitel A‐L, Eustache F, Beaunieux H (2012) Cognitive barriers to readiness to change in alcohol‐dependent patients. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 36:1542–1549.
    1. Li S‐C, Schmiedek F, Huxhold O, Röcke C, Smith J, Lindenberger U (2008) Working memory plasticity in old age: practice gain, transfer, and maintenance. Psychol Aging 23:731–742.
    1. Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA (1984) On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10:276–291.
    1. Love A, James D, Willner P (1998) A comparison of two alcohol craving questionnaires. Addict Abingdon Engl 93:1091–1102.
    1. Martelli C, Petillion A, Brunet‐Lecomte M, Miranda Marcos R, Chanraud S, Amirouche A, Letierce A, Kostogianni N, Lemaitre H, Aubin H‐J, Blecha L, Reynaud M, Martinot J‐L, Benyamina A (2017) Neuropsychological impairment in detoxified alcohol‐dependent subjects with preserved psychosocial functioning. Front Psychiatry 8:193.
    1. Matthews DB, Silvers JR (2004) The use of acute ethanol administration as a tool to investigate multiple memory systems. Neurobiol Learn Mem 82:299–308.
    1. McNab F, Varrone A, Farde L, Jucaite A, Bystritsky P, Forssberg H, Klingberg T (2009) Changes in cortical dopamine D1 receptor binding associated with cognitive training. Science 323:800–802.
    1. Melby‐Lervåg M, Redick TS, Hulme C (2016) Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “Far Transfer”: evidence from a meta‐analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci 11:512–534.
    1. Narendran R, Mason NS, Paris J, Himes ML, Douaihy AB, Frankle WG (2014) Decreased prefrontal cortical dopamine transmission in alcoholism. Am J Psychiatry 171:881–888.
    1. Noël X, Van der Linden M, Brevers D, Campanella S, Hanak C, Kornreich C, Verbanck P (2012) The contribution of executive functions deficits to impaired episodic memory in individuals with alcoholism. Psychiatry Res 198:116–122.
    1. Nowakowska‐Domagała K, Jabłkowska‐Górecka K, Mokros Ł, Koprowicz J, Pietras T (2017) Differences in the verbal fluency, working memory and executive functions in alcoholics: short‐term vs. long‐term abstainers. Psychiatry Res 249:1–8.
    1. Owen AM, Downes JJ, Sahakian BJ, Polkey CE, Robbins TW (1990) Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 28:1021–1034.
    1. Owen AM, Hampshire A, Grahn JA, Stenton R, Dajani S, Burns AS, Howard RJ, Ballard CG (2010) Putting brain training to the test. Nature 465:775–778.
    1. Petry NM (2001) Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology 154:243–250.
    1. Quisenberry AJ, Snider SE, Bickel WK (2016) The return of rate dependence. Behav Anal (Wash DC) 16:215–220.
    1. Rass O, Schacht RL, Buckheit K, Johnson MW, Strain EC, Mintzer MZ (2015) A randomized controlled trial of the effects of working memory training in methadone maintenance patients. Drug Alcohol Depend 156:38–46.
    1. Rogers RD, Owen AM, Middleton HC, Williams EJ, Pickard JD, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (1999) Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 19:9029–9038.
    1. Shallice T (1982) Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 298:199–209.
    1. Snider SE, Deshpande HU, Lisinski JM, Koffarnus MN, LaConte SM, Bickel WK (2018) Working memory training improves alcohol users' episodic future thinking: a rate‐dependent analysis. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 3:160–167.
    1. Snider SE, Quisenberry AJ, Bickel WK (2016) Order in the absence of an effect: identifying rate‐dependent relationships. Behav Processes 127:18–24.
    1. Sobell LC, Sobell MB (1992) Timeline follow‐back: a technique for assessing self‐reported alcohol consumption, in Measuring Alcohol Consumption: psychosocial and Biological Methods (Litten RZ, Allen JP. eds), pp 41–72. Humana Press, Totowa, NJ.
    1. Sofuoglu M (2010) Cognitive enhancement as a pharmacotherapy target for stimulant addiction. Addict Abingdon Engl 105:38–48.
    1. Spencer‐Smith M, Klingberg T (2015) Benefits of a working memory training program for inattention in daily life: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. PLoS One 10:e0119522.
    1. Stavro K, Pelletier J, Potvin S (2013) Widespread and sustained cognitive deficits in alcoholism: a meta‐analysis. Addict Biol 18:203–213.
    1. Svanborg P, Asberg M (2001) A comparison between the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and the self‐rating version of the Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS). J Affect Disord 64:203–216.
    1. Verdejo‐Garcia A (2016) Cognitive training for substance use disorders: neuroscientific mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 68:270–281.
    1. Verdejo‐García A, Lawrence AJ, Clark L (2008) Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance‐use disorders: review of findings from high‐risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:777–810.
    1. Westerberg H, Jacobaeus H, Hirvikoski T, Clevberger P, Ostensson M‐L, Bartfai A, Klingberg T (2007) Computerized working memory training after stroke—a pilot study. Brain Inj 21:21–29.
    1. de Wit H (2009) Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addict Biol 14:22–31.

Source: PubMed

3
订阅