The theory behind the age-related positivity effect

Andrew E Reed, Laura L Carstensen, Andrew E Reed, Laura L Carstensen

Abstract

The "positivity effect" refers to an age-related trend that favors positive over negative stimuli in cognitive processing. Relative to their younger counterparts, older people attend to and remember more positive than negative information. Since the effect was initially identified and the conceptual basis articulated (Mather and Carstensen, 2005) scores of independent replications and related findings have appeared in the literature. Over the same period, a number of investigations have failed to observe age differences in the cognitive processing of emotional material. When findings are considered in theoretical context, a reliable pattern of evidence emerges that helps to refine conceptual tenets. In this article we articulate the operational definition and theoretical foundations of the positivity effect and review the empirical evidence based on studies of visual attention, memory, decision making, and neural activation. We conclude with a discussion of future research directions with emphasis on the conditions where a focus on positive information may benefit and/or impair cognitive performance in older people.

Keywords: aging; attention; emotion regulation; memory; motivation; positivity effect.

References

    1. Baumeister R. F., Bratslavsky E., Finkenauer C., Vohs K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5, 323–37010.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323
    1. Blanchard-Fields F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emotion: an adult developmental perspective. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 26–3110.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x
    1. Blanchard-Fields F., Mienaltowski A., Seay R. B. (2007). Age differences in everyday problem-solving effectiveness: older adults select more effective strategies for interpersonal problems. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 62B, P61–P6410.1093/geronb/62.1.P61
    1. Budson A. E., Todman R. W., Chong H., Adams E. H., Kensinger E. A., Krangel T. S., Wright C. I. (2006). False recognition of emotional word lists in aging and Alzheimer disease. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 19, 71–7810.1097/01.wnn.0000213905.49525.d0
    1. Cacioppo J. T., Berntson G. G., Bechara A., Tranel D., Hawkley L. C. (2011). “Could an aging brain contribute to subjective well-being? The value added by a social neuroscience perspective,” in Social Neuroscience: Toward Understanding the Underpinnings of the Social Mind, eds Todorov A., Fiske S. T., Prentice D. A. (New York: Oxford University Press; ), 249–262
    1. Carstensen L. L. (1993). “Motivation for social contact across the life span: a theory of socioemotional selectivity,” in Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: 1992, Developmental Perspectives on Motivation, Vol. 40, ed. Jacobs J. E. (Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; ), 209–254
    1. Carstensen L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science 312, 1913–191510.1126/science.1127488
    1. Carstensen L. L., Isaacowitz D. M., Charles S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. Am. Psychol. 54, 165–18110.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165
    1. Carstensen L. L., Löckenhoff C. E. (2003). “Aging, emotion, and evolution: the bigger picture,” in Emotions Inside Out: 130 Years After Darwin’s the Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, eds Ekman P., Campos J. J., Davidson R. J., Waal F. B. M. D. (New York, NY: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences; ), 152–179
    1. Carstensen L. L., Turan B., Scheibe S., Ram N., Ersner-Hershfield H., Samanez-Larkin G. R., Brooks K. P., Nesselroade J. R. (2011). Emotional experience improves with age: evidence based on over 10 years of experience sampling. Psychol. Aging 26, 21–3310.1037/a0021285
    1. Charles S. T., Carstensen L. L. (2010). Social and emotional aging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 383–40910.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100448
    1. Charles S. T., Mather M., Carstensen L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 132, 310–32410.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310
    1. Chartrand T. L., Bargh J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impression formation and memorization goals: nonconscious goal priming reproduces effects of explicit task instructions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 464–47810.1037/0022-3514.71.3.464
    1. Comblain C., D’Argembeau A., Van der Linden M., Aldenhoff L. (2004). The effect of ageing on the recollection of emotional and neutral pictures. Memory 12, 673–68410.1080/09658210344000477
    1. Ebner N. C., Johnson M. K., Fischer H. (2012). Neural mechanisms of reading facial emotions in young and older adults. Front. Psychol. 3:223.10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00223
    1. English T. (2012). Aging, Emotion, and Health-Related Decisions: the Role of Health Status. Berkeley: Department of Psychology, University of California
    1. Fernandes M., Ross M., Wiegand M., Schryer E. (2008). Are the memories of older adults positively biased? Psychol. Aging 23, 297–30610.1037/0882-7974.23.2.297
    1. Fleming K., Kim S. H., Doo M., Maguire G., Potkin S. G. (2003). Memory for emotional stimuli in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 18, 340–34210.1177/153331750301800604
    1. Fredrickson B. L., Carstensen L. L. (1990). Choosing social partners: how old age and anticipated endings make people more selective. Psychol. Aging 5, 335–34710.1037/0882-7974.5.3.335
    1. Fung H. H., Carstensen L. L. (2004). Motivational changes in response to blocked goals and foreshortened time: testing alternatives for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychol. Aging 19, 68–7810.1037/0882-7974.19.1.68
    1. Fung H. H., Carstensen L. L. (2006). Goals change when life’s fragility is primed: lessons learned from older adults, the September 11th attacks and SARS. Soc. Cogn. 24, 248–27810.1521/soco.2006.24.3.248
    1. Fung H. H., Carstensen L. L., Lutz A. M. (1999). Influence of time on social preferences: implications for life-span development. Psychol. Aging 14, 595–60410.1037/0882-7974.14.4.595
    1. Fung H. H., Lu A. Y., Goren D., Isaacowitz D. M., Wadlinger H. A., Wilson H. R. (2008). Age-related positivity enhancement is not universal: older Chinese look away from positive stimuli. Psychol. Aging 23, 440–44610.1037/0882-7974.23.2.440
    1. Gallo D. A., Foster K. T., Johnson E. L. (2009). Elevated false recollection of emotional pictures in young and older adults. Psychol. Aging 24, 981–98810.1037/a0017545
    1. Grühn D., Smith J., Baltes P. B. (2005). No aging bias favoring memory for positive material: evidence from a heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally toned words. Psychol. Aging 20, 579–58810.1037/0882-7974.20.4.579
    1. Hajcak G., Moser J. S., Simons R. F. (2006). Attending to affect: appraisal strategies modulate the electrocortical response to arousing pictures. Emotion 6, 517–52210.1037/1528-3542.6.3.517
    1. Hawkes K. (2003). Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 15, 380–40010.1002/ajhb.10156
    1. Hess T. M., Queen T. L., Patterson T. R. (2012). To deliberate or not to deliberate: interactions between age, task characteristics, and cognitive activity on decision making. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 25, 29–4010.1002/bdm.711
    1. Hot P., Klein-Koerkamp Y., Baciu M. (2012). Preserved and impaired emotional memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Psychol. 3:331.10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00331
    1. Isaacowitz D. M., Allard E. S., Murphy N. A., Schlangel M. (2009a). The time course of age-related preferences toward positive and negative stimuli. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 64B, 188–19210.1093/geronb/gbn036
    1. Isaacowitz D. M., Toner K., Neupert S. D. (2009b). Use of gaze for real-time mood regulation: effects of age and attentional functioning. Psychol. Aging 24, 989–99410.1037/a0017706
    1. Isaacowitz D. M., Blanchard-Fields F. (2012). Linking process and outcome in the study of emotion and aging. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 3–1710.1177/1745691611424750
    1. Isaacowitz D. M., Toner K., Goren D., Wilson H. R. (2008). Looking while unhappy mood-congruent gaze in young adults, positive gaze in older adults. Psychol. Sci. 19, 848–85310.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02167.x
    1. Isaacowitz D. M., Wadlinger H. A., Goren D., Wilson H. R. (2006a). Is there an age-related positivity effect in visual attention? A comparison of two methodologies. Emotion 6, 511–51610.1037/1528-3542.6.3.511
    1. Isaacowitz D. M., Wadlinger H. A., Goren D., Wilson H. R. (2006b). Selective preference in visual fixation away from negative images in old age? An eye-tracking study. Psychol. Aging 21, 40–4810.1037/0882-7974.21.1.40
    1. Kennedy Q., Mather M., Carstensen L. L. (2004). The role of motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical memory. Psychol. Sci. 15, 208–21410.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503011.x
    1. Kensinger E. A. (2004). Remembering emotional experiences: the contribution of valence and arousal. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 231–30710.1515/REVNEURO.2004.15.4.231
    1. Kensinger E. A. (2008). Age differences in memory for arousing and nonarousing emotional words. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 63B, P13–P1810.1093/geronb/63.1.P13
    1. Kensinger E. A., Brierley B., Medford N., Growdon J. H., Corkin S. (2002). Effects of normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease on emotional memory. Emotion 2, 118–13410.1037/1528-3542.2.2.118
    1. Kensinger E. A., O’Brien J. L., Swanberg K., Garoff-Eaton R. J., Schacter D. L. (2007). The effects of emotional content on reality-monitoring performance in young and older adults. Psychol. Aging 22, 752–76410.1037/0882-7974.22.4.752
    1. Kim S., Healey M. K., Goldstein D., Hasher L., Wiprzycka U. J. (2008). Age differences in choice satisfaction: a positivity effect in decision making. Psychol. Aging 23, 33–3810.1037/0882-7974.23.1.33
    1. Kisley M. A., Wood S., Burrows C. L. (2007). Looking at the sunny side of life: age-related change in an event-related potential measure of the negativity bias. Psychol. Sci. 18, 838–84310.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01988.x
    1. Knight M., Seymour T. L., Gaunt J. T., Baker C., Nesmith K., Mather M. (2007). Aging and goal-directed emotional attention: distraction reverses emotional biases. Emotion 7, 705–71410.1037/1528-3542.7.4.705
    1. Ko S. G., Lee T. H., Yoon H. Y., Kwon J. H., Mather M. (2011). How does context affect assessments of facial emotion? The role of culture and age. Psychol. Aging 26, 48–5910.1037/a0020222
    1. Kwon Y., Scheibe S., Samanez-Larkin G. R., Tsai J. L., Carstensen L. L. (2009). Replicating the positivity effect in picture memory in Koreans: evidence for cross-cultural generalizability. Psychol. Aging 24, 748–75410.1037/a0016054
    1. Labouvie-Vief G., Grühn D., Studer J. (2010). “Dynamic integration of emotion and cognition: equilibrium regulation in development and aging,” in The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Vol. 2, eds Lerner R. M., Lamb M. E., Freund A. M. (Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), 79–115
    1. Leclerc C. M., Kensinger E. A. (2011). Neural processing of emotional pictures and words: a comparison of young and older adults. Dev. Neuropsychol. 36, 519–53810.1080/87565641.2010.549864
    1. Leigland L. A., Schulz L. E., Janowsky J. S. (2004). Age related changes in emotional memory. Neurobiol. Aging 25, 1117–112410.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.10.015
    1. Löckenhoff C. E., Carstensen L. L. (2007). Aging, emotion, and health-related decision strategies: motivational manipulations can reduce age differences. Psychol. Aging 22, 134–14610.1037/0882-7974.22.1.134
    1. Löckenhoff C. E., Carstensen L. L. (2008). Decision strategies in health care choices for self and others: older but not younger adults make adjustments for the age of the decision target. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 63, P106–P10910.1093/geronb/63.2.P106
    1. Mather M. (2006). “Why memories may become more positive as people age,” in Memory and Emotion: Interdisciplinary Perspectives, eds Uttl B., Ohta N., Siegenthaler A. L. (Malden: Blackwell Publishing; ), 135–158
    1. Mather M., Canli T., English T., Whitfield S., Wais P., Ochsner K., Carstensen L. L. (2004). Amygdala responses to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Psychol. Sci. 15, 259–26310.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00662.x
    1. Mather M., Carstensen L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychol. Sci. 14, 409–41510.1111/1467-9280.01455
    1. Mather M., Carstensen L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 496–50210.1016/j.tics.2005.07.010
    1. Mather M., Johnson M. K. (2000). Choice-supportive source monitoring: do our decisions seem better to us as we age? Psychol. Aging 15, 596–60610.1037/0882-7974.15.4.596
    1. Mather M., Knight M. (2005). Goal-directed memory: the role of cognitive control in older adults’ emotional memory. Psychol. Aging 20, 554–57010.1037/0882-7974.20.4.554
    1. Mather M., Knight M., McCaffrey M. (2005). The allure of the alignable: younger and older adults’ false memories of choice features. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 134, 38–5110.1037/0096-3445.134.1.38
    1. Mather M., Knight M. R. (2006). Angry faces get noticed quickly: threat detection is not impaired among older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 61B, P54–P5710.1093/geronb/61.1.P54
    1. Mikels J. A., Larkin G. R., Reuter-Lorenz P. A., Carstensen L. L. (2005). Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychol. Aging 20, 542–55310.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542
    1. Mikels J. A., Reed A. E. (2009). Monetary losses do not loom large in later life: age differences in the framing effect. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 64B, 457–46010.1093/geronb/gbp021
    1. Moskowitz G. B. (2002). Preconscious effects of temporary goals on attention. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38, 397–40410.1016/S0022-1031(02)00001-X
    1. Murphy N. A., Isaacowitz D. M. (2008). Preferences for emotional information in older and younger adults: a meta-analysis of memory and attention tasks. Psychol. Aging 23, 263–28610.1037/0882-7974.23.2.263
    1. Neisser U. (1979). “The control of information pickup in selective looking,” in Perception and its Development: A Tribute to Eleanor J. Gibson, ed. Pick A. D. (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; ), 201–219
    1. Öhman A., Lundqvist D., Esteves F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 381–39610.1037/0022-3514.80.3.381
    1. Peters E., Dieckmann N. F., Weller J. (2011). “Age differences in complex decision making,” in Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, 7th Edn, eds Schaie K. W., Willis S. L. (San Diego: Academic Press; ), 133–15110.1016/B978-0-12-380882-0.00009-7
    1. Piguet O., Connally E., Krendl A. C., Huot J. R., Corkin S. (2008). False memory in aging: effects of emotional valence on word recognition accuracy. Psychol. Aging 23, 307–31410.1037/0882-7974.23.2.307
    1. Ready R. E., Weinberger M. I., Jones K. M. (2007). How happy have you felt lately? Two diary studies of emotion recall in older and younger adults. Cogn. Emot. 21, 728–75710.1080/02699930600948269
    1. Rozin P., Royzman E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Person. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 5, 296–32010.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2
    1. Samanez-Larkin G. R., Carstensen L. L. (2011). “Socioemotional functioning and the aging brain,” in The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience, eds Decety J., Cacioppo J. T. (New York: Oxford University Press; ), 507–521
    1. Samanez-Larkin G. R., Gibbs S. E. B., Khanna K., Nielsen L., Carstensen L. L., Knutson B. (2007). Anticipation of monetary gain but not loss in healthy older adults. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 787–79110.1038/nn0907-1222b
    1. Schlagman S., Schulz J., Kvavilashvili L. (2006). A content analysis of involuntary autobiographical memories: examining the positivity effect in old age. Memory 14, 161–17510.1080/09658210544000024
    1. Schupp H. T., Cuthbert B. N., Bradley M. M., Cacioppo J. T., Ito T., Lang P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology 37, 257–26110.1016/S0167-8760(00)00108-2
    1. Shadel D. (2012). Outsmarting the Scam Artists. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
    1. Shamaskin A. M., Mikels J. A., Reed A. E. (2010). Getting the message across: age differences in the positive and negative framing of health care messages. Psychol. Aging 25, 746–75110.1037/a0018431
    1. Spaniol J., Voss A., Grady C. L. (2008). Aging and emotional memory: cognitive mechanisms underlying the positivity effect. Psychol. Aging 23, 859–87210.1037/a0014218
    1. St. Jacques P., Dolcos F., Cabeza R. (2010). Effects of aging on functional connectivity of the amygdala during negative evaluation: a network analysis of fMRI data. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 315–32710.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.03.012
    1. Vuilleumier P. (2002). Facial expression and selective attention. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 15, 291–30010.1097/00001504-200205000-00011
    1. Weller J. A., Levin I. P., Denburg N. L. (2011). Trajectory of risky decision making for potential gains and losses from ages 5 to 85. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 24, 331–34410.1002/bdm.677
    1. Williams L. M., Brown K. J., Palmer D., Liddell B. J., Kemp A. H., Olivieri G., Gordon E. (2006). The mellow years? Neural basis of improving emotional stability over age. J. Neurosci. 26, 6422–643010.1523/JNEUROSCI.1016-06.2006
    1. Wurm L. H. (2011). Decreasing complexity of affective space in older adults lower on cognitive control: affective effects in a nonaffective task and with nonaffective stimuli. Psychol. Aging 26, 716–73010.1037/a0022513

Source: PubMed

3
订阅