Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits

Robert M G Reinhart, John A Nguyen, Robert M G Reinhart, John A Nguyen

Abstract

Understanding normal brain aging and developing methods to maintain or improve cognition in older adults are major goals of fundamental and translational neuroscience. Here we show a core feature of cognitive decline-working-memory deficits-emerges from disconnected local and long-range circuits instantiated by theta-gamma phase-amplitude coupling in temporal cortex and theta phase synchronization across frontotemporal cortex. We developed a noninvasive stimulation procedure for modulating long-range theta interactions in adults aged 60-76 years. After 25 min of stimulation, frequency-tuned to individual brain network dynamics, we observed a preferential increase in neural synchronization patterns and the return of sender-receiver relationships of information flow within and between frontotemporal regions. The end result was rapid improvement in working-memory performance that outlasted a 50 min post-stimulation period. The results provide insight into the physiological foundations of age-related cognitive impairment and contribute to groundwork for future non-pharmacological interventions targeting aspects of cognitive decline.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1:. Experiment 1, frontotemporal HD-tACS procedure…
Fig. 1:. Experiment 1, frontotemporal HD-tACS procedure and task.
a, The multifocal inphase frontotemporal HD-tACS montage and current-flow model shown on 3D reconstructions of the cortical surface. The location and current intensity value of each stimulating electrode are shown. The left prefrontal cortex and left temporal cortex were targeted, each with three electrodes in a center-surround, source-sink pattern to achieve maximum focality. b, All trials began with fixation followed by a target and then a delay period. In memory blocks, subjects judged whether a later probe was the same or a modified version of the target. In control blocks, subjects judged whether a backward masked grating probe was tilted clockwise or counterclockwise from the vertical and were not required to remember the target. Trial-by-trial adjustments in orientation magnitude of the probe ensured comparable performance across memory and control blocks.
Fig. 2:. Experiment 1, behavioral results.
Fig. 2:. Experiment 1, behavioral results.
a, Box plots of reaction time (RT) from correct trials and accuracy in post-stimulation memory blocks show older adults were slower (t82 = 2.331, p = 0.022, dz = 0.509) and less accurate (t82 = 5.587, p < 0.01, dz = 1.219) at baseline, relative to younger adults. After stimulation, accuracy improved (t41 = 3.738, p = 0.001, dz = 0.577), removing the difference between groups (t82 = 0.939, p = 0.350, dz = 0.205). RT did not differ between conditions (t41 = 0.641, p = 0.527, dz = 0.099). Box plots of accuracy (b) and RT (c) from memory blocks sorted into 9 sequential bins (4 minutes per bin with 4 minutes between each bin from interleaved control blocks) shows significant group differences at every time bin for accuracy (ts82 > 6.824, ps < 0.01, dsz > 1.489) and RT (ts82 > 2.432, ps < 0.018, dsz > 0.531), at baseline. Stimulation improved accuracy at every time bin (ts41 > 4.393, ps < 0.01, dsz > 0.678), relative to sham, except the first (t41 = 0.986, p = 0.330, dz = 0.152). Stimulation briefly sped RT at time bins 3 (t41 = 2.841, p = 0.007, dz = 0.438), 4 (t41 = 2.278, p = 0.028, dz = 0.352), and 6 (t41 = 2.282, p = 0.028, dz = 0.352). Between-group comparisons used independent samples two-tailed t-tests (n = 84). Within-group comparisons used paired sample two-tailed t-tests (n = 42). Box-plot center, median; box limits, lower and upper quartiles; whiskers, lower and upper extreme values. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01.
Fig. 3:. Experiment 1, phase-amplitude coupling results.
Fig. 3:. Experiment 1, phase-amplitude coupling results.
a, Results show a significantly increased phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) cluster in left temporal electrodes (top) and voxels (bottom) for theta (7–9 Hz) phase and gamma (26–34 Hz) amplitude frequencies on memory compared to control blocks in younger adults at baseline and older adults after frontotemporal inphase theta HD-tACS (pcorrected < 0.01, two-sided cluster permutation test, n = 42). No other clusters were identified. b, Comodulograms show group x block and stimulation x block interactions for all combinations of low-frequency phase (2–16 Hz) and high-frequency amplitude (18–120 Hz) from left temporal electrodes showing significant PAC. Crosshairs indicate values where t > −2.6 or −2.4, respectively (pcorrected < 0.01, two-sided permutation test, n = 42). c, Box plots of gamma (26–34 Hz) amplitude binned by 8-Hz phase shown across groups and conditions (n = 42). d, Cross-frequency directionality between 6.5–10.5-Hz phase and 26–34-Hz amplitude from left temporal electrodes for each group and condition (center, mean; shaded region, SEM; n = 42). e, Individual subject Pearson correlations (two-tailed) between accuracy and theta-gamma PAC from left temporal electrodes for each group and condition (n = 42). Box-plot center, median; box limits, lower and upper quartiles; whiskers, lower and upper extreme values; points, outliers.
Fig. 4:. Experiment 1, phase synchronization results.
Fig. 4:. Experiment 1, phase synchronization results.
a, A significant cluster in prefrontal cortex showed increased phase-locked value (PLV) with temporal cortex seed region in the theta band for younger adults at baseline and for older adults after stimulation (pcorrected < 0.01, two-sided cluster permutation test, n = 42). For theta PLV, the between-group difference at baseline (t82 = 6.450, p < 0.01, dz= 0.995) and the between-condition difference within older adults (t41 = 8.238, p < 0.01, dz= 1.271) were significant. b, A significant cluster in lateral occipital cortex showed increased PLV with temporal cortex seed region in the gamma band for each group and condition (pcorrected < 0.01, two-sided cluster permutation test, n = 42). For gamma PLV, there were no significant group (t82 = 0.898, p = 0.374, dz= 0.139) or condition differences (t41 = 1.012, p = 0.317, dz= 0.156). No other clusters were found. Between-group comparisons used independent two-tailed t-tests (n = 84). Within-group comparisons used paired sample two-tailed t-tests (n = 42). Box-plot center, median; box limits, lower and upper quartiles; whiskers, lower and upper extreme values; points, outliers.
Fig. 5:. Experiment 2, single-region HD-tACS procedures.
Fig. 5:. Experiment 2, single-region HD-tACS procedures.
The unifocal frontal (a) and unifocal temporal (b) HD-tACS montages and current-flow models shown on 3D reconstructions of the cortical surface. The location and current intensity value of each stimulating electrode are shown. The frontal montage targeted left prefrontal cortex. The temporal montage targeted left temporal cortex. Each stimulation site used three electrodes in a center-surround, source-sink pattern to achieve maximum focality.
Fig. 6:. Experiment 2, behavioral results.
Fig. 6:. Experiment 2, behavioral results.
Box plots of reaction time (RT) from correct trials and accuracy of older adults from post-stimulation memory blocks shown across the stimulation conditions of Experiment 2. Relative to sham, frontotemporal inphase theta-tuned stimulation exerted a preferential improvement in task accuracy (t27 = 3.101, p = 0.004, dz = 0.586) without changing RT (t27 = 1.278, p = 0.212, dz = 0.242). Nontuned stimulation had no significant impact on accuracy (t27 = 1.211, p = 0.236, dz = 0.229), or RT (t27 = 0.406, p = 0.688, dz = 0.077), relative to sham. Frontal-alone and temporal-alone stimulation had no significant effect on RT (frontal, t27 = 0.182, p = 0.857, dz = 0.034; temporal, t27 = 0.085, p = 0.933, dz = 0.016) or accuracy (frontal, t27 = 0.323, p = 0.749, dz = 0.061; temporal, t27 = 0.162, p = 0.873, dz = 0.031). Paired sample two-tailed t-tests (n = 28). Box-plot center, median; box limits, lower and upper quartiles; whiskers, lower and upper extreme values. ** p < 0.01.

References

    1. Bishop NA, Lu T & Yankner BA Neural mechanisms of ageing and cognitive decline. Nature 464, 529–535 (2010).
    1. Hebert LE, Weuve J, Scherr PA & Evans DA Alzheimer disease in the United States (2010–2050) estimated using the 2010 Census. Neurology 80, 1778–1783 (2013).
    1. Hayutin AM Global demographic shifts create challenges and opportunities. PREA Quart, 46–53 (2007).
    1. Park DC & Reuter-Lorenz PA The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 60, 173–196 (2009).
    1. Grady C Trends in neurocognitive aging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13, 491–505 (2012).
    1. Hedden T & Gabrieli JD Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5, 87–96 (2004).
    1. Tomasi D & Volkow ND Aging and functional brain networks. Molecular Psychiatry 17, 549–558 (2012).
    1. Andrews-Hanna JR, et al. Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 56, 924–935 (2007).
    1. Davis SW, et al. Assessing the effects of age on long white matter tracts using diffusion tensor tractography. NeuroImage 46, 530–541 (2009).
    1. O’Sullivan M, et al. Evidence for cortical “disconnection” as a mechanism of age-related cognitive decline. Neurology 57, 632–638 (2001).
    1. Siegel M, Donner TH & Engel AK Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13, 121–134 (2012).
    1. Roux F & Uhlhaas PJ Working memory and neural oscillations: alpha-gamma versus theta-gamma codes for distinct WM information? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18, 16–25 (2014).
    1. Helfrich RF & Knight RT Oscillatory dynamics of prefrontal cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20, 916–930 (2016).
    1. Fell J & Axmacher N The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 12, 105–118 (2011).
    1. Sarnthein J, Petsche H, Rappelsberger P, Shaw G & von Stein A Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 95, 7092–7096 (1998).
    1. Axmacher N, et al. Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107, 3228–3233 (2010).
    1. Fries P Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron 88, 220–235 (2015).
    1. Daume J, Gruber T, Engel AK & Friese U Phase-amplitude coupling and long-range phase synchronization reveal frontotemporal interactions during visual working memory. Journal of Neuroscience 37, 313–322 (2017).
    1. Sreenivasan KK, Curtis CE & D’Esposito M Revisiting the role of persistent neural activity during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18, 82–89 (2014).
    1. Lara AH & Wallis JD Executive control processes underlying multiitem working memory. Nature Neuroscience 17, 876–883 (2014).
    1. Reinhart RMG Disruption and rescue of interareal theta phase coupling and adaptive behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 114, 11542–11547 (2017).
    1. Helfrich RF, et al. Selective modulation of interhemispheric functional connectivity by HD-tACS shapes perception. Public Library of Sciences Biology 12, e1002031 (2014).
    1. Polanía R, Nitsche MA, Korman C, Batsikadze G & Paulus W The importance of timing in segregated theta phase-coupling for cognitive performance. Current Biology 14, 1314–1318 (2012).
    1. Wolinski N, Cooper NR, Sauseng P & Romei V The speed of parietal theta frequency drives visuospatial working memory capacity. Public LIbrary of Sciences Biology 16, e2005348 (2018).
    1. Alekseichuk I, Turi Z, Amador de Lara G, Antal A & Paulus W Spatial working memory in humans depends on theta and high gamma synchronization in the prefrontal cortex. Current Biology 26, 1513–1521 (2016).
    1. Salthouse TA The aging of working memory. Neuropsychology 8, 535–543 (1994).
    1. Voytek B, et al. Age-related changes in 1/f neural electrophysiological noise. Journal of Neuroscience 35, 13257–13265 (2015).
    1. Cole SR & Voytek B Brain oscillations and the importance of waveform shape. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21, 137–149 (2017).
    1. Scheffer-Teixeira R & Tort AB On cross-frequency phase-phase coupling between theta and gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. Elife 5, 423–435 (2016).
    1. Daume J, Graetz S, Gruber T, Engel AK & Friese U Cognitive control during audiovisual working memory engages frontotemporal theta band interactions. Scientific Reports 7, 12585 (2018).
    1. Ranganath C & D’Esposito M Medial temporal lobe activity associated with active maintenance of novel information. Neuron 31, 865–873 (2001).
    1. Nichols EA, Kao YC, Verfaellie M & Gabrieli JD Hippocampus, working memory and long-term memory for faces: evidence from fMRI and global amnesia for involvement of the medial temporal lobes. Hippocampus 16, 604–616 (2006).
    1. Axmacher N, et al. Sustained neural activity patterns during working memory in the human medial temporal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 7807–7816 (2007).
    1. Axmacher N, Schmitz DP, Wagner T, Elger CE & Fell J Interactions between medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and inferior temporal regions during visual working memory: a combined intracranial EEG and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience 28, 7304–7312 (2008).
    1. Lueschow A, Miller EK & Desimone R Inferior temporal mechanisms for invariant object recognition. Cerebral Cortex 5, 523–531 (1994).
    1. Woloszyn L & Sheinberg DL Neural dynamics in inferior temporal cortex during a visual working memory task. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 5494–5507 (2009).
    1. Powell HW, et al. Material-specific lateralization of memory encoding in the medial temporal lobe: blocked versus event-related design. Neuroimage 27, 231–239 (2005).
    1. Henson R A mini-review of fMRI studies of human medial temporal lobe activity associated with recognition memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 58, 340–360 (2005).
    1. Ali MM, Sellers KK & Frohlich F Transcranial alternating current stimulation modulates large-scale cortical network activity by network resonance. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 11262–11275 (2013).
    1. Herrmann CS, Rach S, Neuling T & Strüber D Transcranial alternating current stimulation: a review of the underlying mechanisms and modulation of cognitive processes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7 (2013).
    1. Vossen A, Gross J & Thut G Alpha power increase after transcranial alternating current stimulation at alpha frequency (α-tACS) reflects plastic changes rather than entrainment. Brain Stimulation 8, 499–508 (2015).
    1. Zaehle T, Rach S & Herrmann CS Transcranial alternating current stimulation enhances individual alpha activity in human EEG. Public Library of Sciences ONE 5, e13766 (2010).
    1. Bergmann TO & Born J Phase-amplitude coupling: A general mechanism for memory processing and synaptic plasticity? Neuron 97, 10–13 (2018).
    1. Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H & Desimone R High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science 324, 1207–1210 (2009).
    1. Wang XJ Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiological Reviews 90, 1195–1268 (2010).
    1. Violante IR, et al. Externally induced frontoparietal synchronization modulates network dynamics and enhances working memory performance. Elife 6, e22001 (2017).
    1. Folstein M, Folstein SE & McHugh PR “Mini-mental state.” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12, 189–198 (1975).
    1. Beck AT, Steer RA & Brown GK Manual for the beck depression inventory (The Psychological Association, San Antonio, TX, 1996).
    1. Sabia S, et al. Impact of smoking on cognitive decline in early old age: the Whitehall II cohort study. Archives in General Psychiatry 69, 627–635 (2012).
    1. Grundey J, Amu R, Batsikadze G, Paulus W & Nitsche MA Diverging effects of nicotine on motor learning performance: improvement in deprived smokers and attenuation in non-smokers. Addictive Behaviors 74, 90–97 (2017).
    1. Grundey J, et al. Double dissociation of working memory and attentional processes in smokers and non-smokers with and without nicotine. Psychopharmacology 232, 2491–2501 (2015).
    1. Noury N, Hipp JF & Siegel M Physiological processes non-linearly affect electrophysiological recordings during transcranial electric stimulation. Neuroimage 140, 99–109 (2016).
    1. Noury N & Siegel M Analyzing EEG and MEG signals recorded during tES, a reply. Neuroimage 167, 53–61 (2017).
    1. Konkle T, Brady TF, Alvarez GA & Oliva A Conceptual distinctiveness supports detailed visual long-term memory for real-world objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 139, 558–578 (2010).
    1. Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA & Oliva A Visual long-term memory has a massive storage capacity for object details. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105, 14325–14329 (2008).
    1. Park JY, Jhung K, Lee J & S.K., A. Theta-gamma coupling during a working memory task as compared to a simple vigilance task. Neuroscience Letters 532, 39–43 (2013).
    1. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E & Schoffelen JM FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience 2011, 1–9 (2011).
    1. Nguyen J, Deng Y & Reinhart RMG Brain-state determines learning improvements after transcranial alternating-current stimulation to fontal cortex. Brain Stimulation 11, 723–726 (2018).
    1. Kanai R, Chaieb L, Antal A, Walsh V & Paulus W Frequency-dependent electrical stimulation of the visual cortex. Current Biology 18, 1839–1843 (2008).
    1. Schutter DJ & Hortensius R Retinal origin of phosphenes to transcranial alternating current stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology 121, 1080–1084 (2010).
    1. Paulus W On the difficulties of separating retinal from cortical origins of phosphenes when using transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Clinical Neurophysiology 121, 987–991 (2010).
    1. Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J & Varela FJ Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Human Brain Mapping 8, 194–208 (1999).
    1. Reinhart RMG, Zhu J, Park S & Woodman GF Medial-frontal stimulation enhances learning in schizophrenia by restoring prediction-error signaling. Journal of Neuroscience 35, 12232–12240 (2015).
    1. Reinhart RMG & Woodman GF Enhancing long-term memory with stimulation tunes visual attention in one trial. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 112, 625–630 (2015).
    1. Reinhart RMG & Woodman GF Causal control of medial-frontal cortex governs electrophysiological and behavioral indices of performance monitoring and learning. Journal of Neuroscience 34, 4214–4227 (2014).
    1. Reinhart RMG, Zhu J, Park S & Woodman GF Synchronizing theta oscillations with direct-current stimulation strengthens adaptive control in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 112, 9448–9453 (2015).
    1. Reinhart RMG, Cosman JD, Fukuda K & Woodman GF Using transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS) to understand cognitive processing. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics 79, 3–23 (2017).
    1. Reinhart RMG, Xiao W, McClenahan L & Woodman GF Electrical stimulation of visual cortex can immediately improve spatial vision. Current Biology 25, 1867–1872 (2016).
    1. Poreisz C, Boros K, Antal A & Paulus W Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients. Brain Research Bulletin 72, 208–214 (2007).
    1. Gandiga P, Hummel F & Cohen L Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): A tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology 117, 845–850 (2006).
    1. Jung TP, et al. Imaging brain dynamics using independent component analysis. The Proceedings of the Institution of Electrical and Electronic Engineers 89, 1107–1122 (2001).
    1. Delorme A & Makeig S EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of singel-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 134, 9–21 (2004).
    1. Berens P CircStat: a MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics. Journal of Statistical Software 31, 21 (2009).
    1. van Driel J, Cox R & Cohen MX Phase-clustering bias in phase–amplitude cross-frequency coupling and its removal. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 254, 60–72 (2015).
    1. van Veen B, van Drongelen W, Yuchtman M & Suzuki A Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constraint minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Translations on Biomedical Engineering 44, 867–880 (1997).
    1. Hipp JF, Engel AK & Siegel M Oscillatory synchronization in large-scale cortical networks predicts perception. Neuron 69, 387–396 (2011).
    1. Sekihara K & Nagarajan SS Adaptive spatial filters for electromagnetic brain imaging (Springer, 2008).
    1. Darvas F, Pantazis D, Kucukaltun-Yildirim E & Leahy RM Mapping human brain function with MEG and EEG: methods and validation. NeuroImage 23, S289–S299 (2004).
    1. Dannhauer M, Lanfer B, Wolters CH & Knösche TR Modeling of the human skull in EEG source analysis. Human Brain Mapping 32 (2011).
    1. Wolters CH, et al. Influence of tissue conductivity anisotropy on EEG/MEG field and return current computation in a realistic head model: a simulation and visualization study using high-resolution finite element modeling. NeuroImage 30, 813–826 (2006).
    1. Cho JH, Vorwerk J, Wolters CH & Knösche TR Influence of the head model on EEG and MEG source connectivity analyses. Neuroimage 110, 60–77 (2015).
    1. Buchner H, et al. Inverse localization of electric dipole current sources in finite element models of the human head. Electroencephalography Clinical Neurophysiology 102, 267–278 (1997).
    1. Wolters CH, Grasedyck L & Hackbusch W Efficient computation of lead field bases and influence matrix for the FEM-based EEG and MEG inverse problem. Inverse Problems 20, 1099–1116 (2004).
    1. Lew S, Wolters CH, Dierkes T, Röer C & Macleod RS Accuracy and run-time comparison for different potential approaches and iterative solvers in finite element method based EEG source analysis. Applied Numerical Mathematics 59, 1970–1988 (2009).
    1. Singh AK, Okamoto M, Dan H, Jurcak V & Dan I Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI. Neuroimage 27, 842–851 (2005).
    1. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289 (2002).
    1. Lancaster JL, et al. Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Human Brain Mapping 10, 120–131 (2000).
    1. Jiang H, Bahramisharif A, van Gerven MA & Jensen O Measuring directionality between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. Neuroimage 118, 359–367 (2015).
    1. Gross J, et al. Dynamic imaging of coherent sources: Studying neural interactions in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 98, 694–699 (2001).
    1. Nolte G, et al. Robustly estimating the flow direction of information in complex physical systems. Physical Review Letters 100, 234101 (2008).
    1. Klimesch W EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews 29, 169–195 (1999).
    1. Klimesch W Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, 606–617 (2012).
    1. Maris E & Oostenveld R Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 164, 177–190 (2007).
    1. Nolte G, et al. Identifying true brain interaction from EEG data using the imaginary part of coherency. Clinical Neurophysiology 115, 2292–2307 (2004).
    1. Storey JD A direct approach to false discovery rates. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B 64, 479–498 (2002).
    1. Gazzaley A, Cooney JW, Rissman J & D’Esposito M Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging. Nature Neuroscience 8, 1298–1300 (2005).
    1. McNab F, et al. Age-related changes in working memory and the ability to ignore distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112, 6515–6518 (2015).
    1. Chadick JZ, Zanto TP & Gazzaley A Structural and functional differences in medial prefrontal cortex underlie distractibility and suppression deficits in ageing. Nature Communications 5, 4223 (2014).

Source: PubMed

3
订阅