Right inferior frontal cortex activity correlates with tolcapone responsivity in problem and pathological gamblers

Andrew S Kayser, Taylor Vega, Dawn Weinstein, Jan Peters, Jennifer M Mitchell, Andrew S Kayser, Taylor Vega, Dawn Weinstein, Jan Peters, Jennifer M Mitchell

Abstract

Failures of self-regulation in problem and pathological gambling (PPG) are thought to emerge from failures of top-down control, reflected neurophysiologically in a reduced capacity of prefrontal cortex to influence activity within subcortical structures. In patients with addictions, these impairments have been argued to alter evaluation of reward within dopaminergic neuromodulatory systems. Previously we demonstrated that augmenting dopamine tone in frontal cortex via use of tolcapone, an inhibitor of the dopamine-degrading enzyme catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), reduced delay discounting, a measure of impulsivity, in healthy subjects. To evaluate this potentially translational approach to augmenting prefrontal inhibitory control, here we hypothesized that increasing cortical dopamine tone would reduce delay discounting in PPG subjects in proportion to its ability to augment top-down control. To causally test this hypothesis, we administered the COMT inhibitor tolcapone in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subject study of 17 PPG subjects who performed a delay discounting task while functional MRI images were obtained. In this subject population, we found that greater BOLD activity during the placebo condition within the right inferior frontal cortex (RIFC), a region thought to be important for inhibitory control, correlated with greater declines in impulsivity on tolcapone versus placebo. Intriguingly, connectivity between RIFC and the right striatum, and not the level of activity within RIFC itself, increased on tolcapone versus placebo. Together, these findings support the hypothesis that tolcapone-mediated increases in top-down control may reduce impulsivity in PPG subjects, a finding with potential translational relevance for gambling disorders, and for behavioral addictions in general.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT02772978.

Keywords: Dopamine; Frontostriatal; Gambling; Prefrontal cortex; Tolcapone; Ventral striatum.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Study Flow Diagram. As documented in Materials and methods, 39 subjects were screened, of whom 19 met criteria for study participation and were allocated to the intervention. During participation in the randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subject portion of the study, two additional subjects were excluded: one after he failed a urine toxicology screen at his first MRI visit, and another after she fell asleep during her second fMRI session.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Task Design. A. Each trial of the delay discounting task began with fixation, followed by a cue to the trial type. After a brief jittered delay, subjects were prompted to make a decision (in this case, a “Want” decision). B. Illustrated are the four trial types: Want, Don't Want, Sooner, and Larger (see Materials and methods). The pie chart at right illustrates the relative proportions of each of the trial types.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Behavioral Results. A. Shown are the variance-stabilized impulsive choice ratios for each subject in the placebo (dark gray) and tolcapone (light gray) conditions, ordered by magnitude of the difference within the 17 subjects. Subjects whose ICR values decreased on tolcapone are to the left. B. The change in ICR on tolcapone versus placebo was significantly correlated with subjects' scores on the non-planning subscale of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS).
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Brain-Behavior Correlation. A. Two brain regions demonstrated significant negative correlations between ΔICR and BOLD activity during the Want condition on placebo at a significance level of p 2 = 0.59)). A similar result was seen for the right premotor cortex (data not shown). B. This finding was replicated in the tolcapone condition. The same two brain regions demonstrated significant negative correlations between ΔICR and BOLD activity during Want trials at a significance level of p < 0.05, corrected (left panel); and greater BOLD signal in these regions again covaried with greater declines in ICR on tolcapone versus placebo (right panel, shown for the RIFC region; for reference, the equivalent Pearson's r = − 0.89 (r2 = 0.79)).
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Connectivity-Behavior Correlation. A psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis was performed in which the right inferior frontal cortex (RIFC) served as the seed region and the right striatum as the search volume of interest. Shown is the correlation between connectivity with the RIFC during the Want condition (tolcapone versus placebo) and the change in ICR (tolcapone versus placebo), thresholded at a significance level of p 2 = 0.58)). Specifically, greater increases in RIFC ↔ right putamen connection strength on tolcapone versus placebo correlated with greater declines in ICR on tolcapone versus placebo (left panel). When the search was expanded to the rest of the brain, no other regions of this cluster size or greater were found (data not shown).

References

    1. Achterberg M., Peper J.S., van Duijvenvoorde A.C., Mandl R.C., Crone E.A. Frontostriatal white matter integrity predicts development of delay of gratification: a longitudinal study. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:1954–1961.
    1. Apud J.A., Mattay V., Chen J., Kolachana B.S., Callicott J.H., Rasetti R., Alce G., Iudicello J.E., Akbar N., Egan M.F., Goldberg T.E., Weinberger D.R. Tolcapone improves cognition and cortical information processing in normal human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:1011–1020.
    1. Aron A.R., Robbins T.W., Poldrack R.A. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:177–185.
    1. Balodis I.M., Kober H., Worhunsky P.D., Stevens M.C., Pearlson G.D., Potenza M.N. Diminished frontostriatal activity during processing of monetary rewards and losses in pathological gambling. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;71:749–757.
    1. Bickel W.K., Marsch L.A. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. Addiction. 2001;96:73–86.
    1. Bickel W.K., Pitcock J.A., Yi R., Angtuaco E.J. Congruence of BOLD response across intertemporal choice conditions: fictive and real money gains and losses. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:8839–8846.
    1. Bickel W.K., Koffarnus M.N., Moody L., Wilson A.G. The behavioral- and neuro-economic process of temporal discounting: a candidate behavioral marker of addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76(Pt B):518–527.
    1. Chen J., Lipska B.K., Halim N., Ma Q.D., Matsumoto M., Melhem S., Kolachana B.S., Hyde T.M., Herman M.M., Apud J., Egan M.F., Kleinman J.E., Weinberger D.R. Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2004;75:807–821.
    1. Clark L., Stokes P.R., Wu K., Michalczuk R., Benecke A., Watson B.J., Egerton A., Piccini P., Nutt D.J., Bowden-Jones H., Lingford-Hughes A.R. Striatal dopamine D(2)/D(3) receptor binding in pathological gambling is correlated with mood-related impulsivity. NeuroImage. 2012;63:40–46.
    1. Cohen J. second ed. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.
    1. Cools R. Role of dopamine in the motivational and cognitive control of behavior. Neuroscientist. 2008;14:381–395.
    1. Cools R., D'Esposito M. Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;69:e113–e125.
    1. Cools R., Sheridan M., Jacobs E., D'Esposito M. Impulsive personality predicts dopamine-dependent changes in frontostriatal activity during component processes of working memory. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:5506–5514.
    1. Desmond J.E., Glover G.H. Estimating sample size in functional MRI (fMRI) neuroimaging studies: statistical power analyses. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2002;118:115–128.
    1. Ekhtiari H., Faghiri A., Oghabian M.A., Paulus M.P. Functional neuroimaging for addiction medicine: from mechanisms to practical considerations. Prog. Brain Res. 2016;224:129–153.
    1. Eklund A., Nichols T.E., Knutsson H. Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2016;113:7900–7905.
    1. Everitt B.J., Robbins T.W. Drug addiction: updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2016;67:23–50.
    1. Forstmann B.U., Anwander A., Schafer A., Neumann J., Brown S., Wagenmakers E.J., Bogacz R., Turner R. Cortico-striatal connections predict control over speed and accuracy in perceptual decision making. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2010;107:15916–15920.
    1. Freemantle S.J., Vaseva A.V., Ewings K.E., Bee T., Krizan K.A., Kelley M.R., Hattab E.M., Memoli V.A., Black C.C., Spinella M.J., Dmitrovsky E. Repression of cyclin D1 as a target for germ cell tumors. Int. J. Oncol. 2007;30:333–340.
    1. Gogos J.A., Morgan M., Luine V., Santha M., Ogawa S., Pfaff D., Karayiorgou M. Catechol-O-methyltransferase-deficient mice exhibit sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine levels and behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1998;95:9991–9996.
    1. Grant J.E., Odlaug B.L., Chamberlain S.R., Hampshire A., Schreiber L.R., Kim S.W. A proof of concept study of tolcapone for pathological gambling: relationships with COMT genotype and brain activation. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013;23:1587–1596.
    1. Gray J.C., MacKillop J. Genetic basis of delay discounting in frequent gamblers: examination of a priori candidates and exploration of a panel of dopamine-related loci. Brain Behav. 2014;4:812–821.
    1. Hampshire A., Sharp D.J. Contrasting network and modular perspectives on inhibitory control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015;19:445–452.
    1. Heatherton T.F., Wagner D.D. Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:132–139.
    1. Jameson J.L., Longo D.L. Precision medicine–personalized, problematic, and promising. N. Engl. J. Med. 2015;372:2229–2234.
    1. Joutsa J., Voon V., Johansson J., Niemela S., Bergman J., Kaasinen V. Dopaminergic function and intertemporal choice. Transl. Psychiatry. 2015;5
    1. Kang M.J., Rangel A., Camus M., Camerer C.F. Hypothetical and real choice differentially activate common valuation areas. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:461–468.
    1. Kayser A.S., Allen D.C., Navarro-Cebrian A., Mitchell J.M., Fields H.L. Dopamine, corticostriatal connectivity, and intertemporal choice. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:9402–9409.
    1. Kayser A.S., Mitchell J.M., Weinstein D., Frank M.J. Dopamine, locus of control, and the exploration-exploitation tradeoff. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:454–462.
    1. Linnet J., Moller A., Peterson E., Gjedde A., Doudet D. Inverse association between dopaminergic neurotransmission and Iowa Gambling Task performance in pathological gamblers and healthy controls. Scand. J. Psychol. 2011;52:28–34.
    1. Mazur J.E. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In: Mazur J.E., Nevin J.A., Rachlin H., editors. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: the Effect of Delay and Intervening Events on Reinforcement Value. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1987. pp. 55–73.
    1. McLaren D.G., Ries M.L., Xu G., Johnson S.C. A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. NeuroImage. 2012;61:1277–1286.
    1. Mitchell J.M., Fields H.L., D'Esposito M., Boettiger C.A. Impulsive responding in alcoholics. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2005;29:2158–2169.
    1. Mohammadi B., Hammer A., Miedl S.F., Wiswede D., Marco-Pallares J., Herrmann M., Munte T.F. Intertemporal choice behavior is constrained by brain structure in healthy participants and pathological gamblers. Brain Struct. Funct. 2015
    1. Morean M.E., DeMartini K.S., Leeman R.F., Pearlson G.D., Anticevic A., Krishnan-Sarin S., Krystal J.H., O'Malley S.S. Psychometrically improved, abbreviated versions of three classic measures of impulsivity and self-control. Psychol. Assess. 2014;26:1003–1020.
    1. Patton J.H., Stanford M.S., Barratt E.S. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995;51:768–774.
    1. Pine A., Shiner T., Seymour B., Dolan R.J. Dopamine, time, and impulsivity in humans. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:8888–8896.
    1. Potenza M.N. The neural bases of cognitive processes in gambling disorder. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:429–438.
    1. Reise S.P., Moore T.M., Sabb F.W., Brown A.K., London E.D. The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11: reassessment of its structure in a community sample. Psychol. Assess. 2013;25:631–642.
    1. Saez I., Zhu L., Set E., Kayser A., Hsu M. Dopamine modulates egalitarian behavior in humans. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:912–919.
    1. Schmaal L., Joos L., Koeleman M., Veltman D.J., van den Brink W., Goudriaan A.E. Effects of modafinil on neural correlates of response inhibition in alcohol-dependent patients. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013;73:211–218.
    1. Sheehan D.V., Lecrubier Y., Sheehan K.H., Amorim P., Janavs J., Weiller E., Hergueta T., Baker R., Dunbar G.C. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 1998;59(Suppl. 20):22–33. (quiz 34–57)
    1. Spinella M. Normative data and a short form of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Int. J. Neurosci. 2007;117:359–368.
    1. Tabibnia G., Monterosso J.R., Baicy K., Aron A.R., Poldrack R.A., Chakrapani S., Lee B., London E.D. Different forms of self-control share a neurocognitive substrate. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:4805–4810.
    1. Thirion B., Pinel P., Meriaux S., Roche A., Dehaene S., Poline J.B. Analysis of a large fMRI cohort: statistical and methodological issues for group analyses. NeuroImage. 2007;35:105–120.
    1. Tunbridge E.M., Bannerman D.M., Sharp T., Harrison P.J. Catechol-o-methyltransferase inhibition improves set-shifting performance and elevates stimulated dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:5331–5335.
    1. Tzourio-Mazoyer N., Landeau B., Papathanassiou D., Crivello F., Etard O., Delcroix N., Mazoyer B., Joliot M. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15:273–289.
    1. van Holst R.J., Chase H.W., Clark L. Striatal connectivity changes following gambling wins and near-misses: associations with gambling severity. Neuroimaging Clin. N. Am. 2014;5:232–239.
    1. Volkow N.D., Baler R.D. NOW vs LATER brain circuits: implications for obesity and addiction. Trends Neurosci. 2015;38:345–352.
    1. Voon V., Fernagut P.O., Wickens J., Baunez C., Rodriguez M., Pavon N., Juncos J.L., Obeso J.A., Bezard E. Chronic dopaminergic stimulation in Parkinson's disease: from dyskinesias to impulse control disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:1140–1149.
    1. Voytek B., Kayser A.S., Badre D., Fegen D., Chang E.F., Crone N.E., Parvizi J., Knight R.T., D'Esposito M. Oscillatory dynamics coordinating human frontal networks in support of goal maintenance. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1318–1324.
    1. Whelan R., Conrod P.J., Poline J.B., Lourdusamy A., Banaschewski T., Barker G.J., Bellgrove M.A., Buchel C., Byrne M., Cummins T.D., Fauth-Buhler M., Flor H., Gallinat J., Heinz A., Ittermann B., Mann K., Martinot J.L., Lalor E.C., Lathrop M., Loth E., Nees F., Paus T., Rietschel M., Smolka M.N., Spanagel R., Stephens D.N., Struve M., Thyreau B., Vollstaedt-Klein S., Robbins T.W., Schumann G., Garavan H., Consortium I. Adolescent impulsivity phenotypes characterized by distinct brain networks. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:920–925.
    1. Wiehler A., Peters J. Reward-based decision making in pathological gambling: the roles of risk and delay. Neurosci. Res. 2015;90:3–14.
    1. Zack M., Poulos C.X. Parallel roles for dopamine in pathological gambling and psychostimulant addiction. Curr. Drug Abuse Rev. 2009;2:11–25.
    1. Zandbelt B.B., Bloemendaal M., Hoogendam J.M., Kahn R.S., Vink M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and functional MRI reveal cortical and subcortical interactions during stop-signal response inhibition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013;25:157–174.

Source: PubMed

3
订阅