School-age effects of the newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program for preterm infants with intrauterine growth restriction: preliminary findings

Gloria McAnulty, Frank H Duffy, Sandra Kosta, Neil I Weisenfeld, Simon K Warfield, Samantha C Butler, Moona Alidoost, Jane Holmes Bernstein, Richard Robertson, David Zurakowski, Heidelise Als, Gloria McAnulty, Frank H Duffy, Sandra Kosta, Neil I Weisenfeld, Simon K Warfield, Samantha C Butler, Moona Alidoost, Jane Holmes Bernstein, Richard Robertson, David Zurakowski, Heidelise Als

Abstract

Background: The experience in the newborn intensive care nursery results in premature infants' neurobehavioral and neurophysiological dysfunction and poorer brain structure. Preterms with severe intrauterine growth restriction are doubly jeopardized given their compromised brains. The Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program improved outcome at early school-age for preterms with appropriate intrauterine growth. It also showed effectiveness to nine months for preterms with intrauterine growth restriction. The current study tested effectiveness into school-age for preterms with intrauterine growth restriction regarding executive function (EF), electrophysiology (EEG) and neurostructure (MRI).

Methods: Twenty-three 9-year-old former growth-restricted preterms, randomized at birth to standard care (14 controls) or to the Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program (9 experimentals) were assessed with standardized measures of cognition, achievement, executive function, electroencephalography, and magnetic resonance imaging. The participating children were comparable to those lost to follow-up, and the controls to the experimentals, in terms of newborn background health and demographics. All outcome measures were corrected for mother's intelligence. Analysis techniques included two-group analysis of variance and stepwise discriminate analysis for the outcome measures, Wilks' lambda and jackknifed classification to ascertain two-group classification success per and across domains; canonical correlation analysis to explore relationships among neuropsychological, electrophysiological and neurostructural domains at school-age, and from the newborn period to school-age.

Results: Controls and experimentals were comparable in age at testing, anthropometric and health parameters, and in cognitive and achievement scores. Experimentals scored better in executive function, spectral coherence, and cerebellar volumes. Furthermore, executive function, spectral coherence and brain structural measures discriminated controls from experimentals. Executive function correlated with coherence and brain structure measures, and with newborn-period neurobehavioral assessment.

Conclusion: The intervention in the intensive care nursery improved executive function as well as spectral coherence between occipital and frontal as well as parietal regions. The experimentals' cerebella were significantly larger than the controls'. These results, while preliminary, point to the possibility of long-term brain improvement even of intrauterine growth compromised preterms if individualized intervention begins with admission to the NICU and extends throughout transition home. Larger sample replications are required in order to confirm these results.

Clinical trial registration: The study is registered as a clinical trial. The trial registration number is NCT00914108.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Consort flow chart.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Standard EEG electrode names and positions. Head in vertex view, nose above, left ear to left. EEG electrodes: Z: Midline: FZ: Midline Frontal; CZ: Midline Central; PZ: Midline Parietal; OZ: Midline Occipital. Even numbers, right hemisphere locations; odd numbers, left hemisphere locations: Fp: Frontopolar; F: Frontal; C: Central; T: Temporal; P: Parietal; O: Occipital. The standard 19, 10–20 electrodes are shown as black circles. An additional subset of 17, 10–10 electrodes are shown as open circles.
Figure 3
Figure 3
3D rendering of parcellation of a 9-year-old child’s brain from MRI. The surface model is depicted on top of a mid-sagittal slice from the T1-weighted MRI. Comparison of regional tissue volumes across subjects can be employed to indicate localized structural differences.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Demonstration of T1-weighted MRI tissue segmentation combined with parcellation of a 9-year-old child’s brain. Axial slice on the left; corresponding combined tissue segmentation/parcellation on the right.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Rey-Osterrieth complex figure. The figure represents sample drawings from two study children, one from the Control group, a 9 year 11 month old born at 29w GA; and one from the Experimental group, a 9 year 6 month old born at 32w GA. The conditions displayed are from left to right: Copy, Immediate Recall and Delayed Recall.
Figure 6
Figure 6
EEG spectral coherence factors at school-age, control (C) (n =12), experimental (E) (n = 9). Head shown in vertex view, nose above, left ear to left. EEG frequency and coherence electrodes shown above head. Arrow color illustrates experimental group coherence; green = decreased, red = increased.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Distribution of right and left cerebellar tissue volumes by group. The figure represents a scatter plot of the control (n = 11) and the experimental (n = 7) groups’ distributions of right and left cerebellar tissue volumes expressed as percentage of total parenchyma.

References

    1. Als H, Duffy F, McAnulty GB, Rivkin MJ, Vajapeyam S, Mulkern RV, Warfield S, Hüppi PS, Butler S, Conneman N. Early experience alters brain function and structure. Pediatrics. 2004;113(4):846–857. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.4.846.
    1. Peters K, Rosychuk R, Hendson L, Cote J, McPherson C, Tyebkhan J. Improvement of short- and long-term outcomes for very low birth weight infants: The Edmonton NIDCAP trial. Pediatrics. 2009;124:1009–1020. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3808.
    1. Heron M, Sutton P, Xu J, Ventura S, Strobino D, Guyer B. Annual summary of vital statistics: 2007. Pediatrics. 2010;125:4–15. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-2416.
    1. Behrman RE, Stith Butler A, Committee on Understanding Premature Birth and Assuring Healthy Outcomes. Preterm birth: causes, consequences, and prevention. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2007. p. 31.
    1. Sizonenko SV, Borradori-Tolsa C, Hüppi PS. Intrauterine growth restriction: impact on brain development and function. Rev Med Suisse. 2008;4(146):509–514.
    1. Als H. Program guide - newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program (NIDCAP): an education and training program for health care professionals. Boston: Copyright, NIDCAP Federation International; 1986. rev 2011.
    1. Als H, Lawhon G, Brown E, Gibes R, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Blickman JG. Individualized behavioral and environmental care for the very low birth weight preterm infant at high risk for bronchopulmonary dysplasia: neonatal intensive care unit and developmental outcome. Pediatrics. 1986;78:1123–1132.
    1. Als H, Lawhon G, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Gibes-Grossman R, Blickman JG. Individualized developmental care for the very low birthweight preterm infant: medical and neurofunctional effects. J Am Med Assoc. 1994;272:853–858. doi: 10.1001/jama.1994.03520110033025.
    1. Fleisher BF, VandenBerg KA, Constantinou J, Heller C, Benitz WE, Johnson A, Rosenthal A, Stevenson DK. Individualized developmental care for very-low-birth-weight premature infants. Clin Pediatr. 1995;34:523–529. doi: 10.1177/000992289503401003.
    1. Westrup B, Kleberg A, von Eichwald K, Stjernqvist K, Lagercrantz H. A randomized controlled trial to evaluate the effects of the newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program in a swedish setting. Pediatrics. 2000;105(1):66–72. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.1.66.
    1. Kleberg A, Westrup B, Stjernqvist K, Lagercrantz H. Indications of improved cognitive development at one year of age among infants born very prematurely who received care based on the newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program (NIDCAP) Early Hum Dev. 2002;68:83–91. doi: 10.1016/S0378-3782(02)00014-2.
    1. Als H, Gilkerson L, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Buehler DM, VandenBerg KA, Sweet N, Sell E, Parad RB, Ringer SA. A three-center randomized controlled trial of individualized developmental care for very low birth weight preterm infants: medical, neurodevelopmental, parenting and caregiving effects. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2003;24(6):399–408. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200312000-00001.
    1. McAnulty G, Duffy F, Butler S, Parad R, Ringer S, Zurakowski D, Als H. Individualized developmental care for a large sample of very preterm infants: health, neurobehavior and neurophysiology. Acta Paediatr. 2009;98:1920–1926. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2009.01492.x.
    1. Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Fischer CB, Kosta S, Butler SC, Parad RB, Blickman JG, Zurakowski D, Ringer SA. Is the newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program (NIDCAP) effective for preterm infants with intrauterine growth restriction? J Perinatol. 2011;31(2):130–136. doi: 10.1038/jp.2010.81.
    1. Westrup B, Böhm B, Lagercrantz H, Stjernqvist K. Preschool outcome in children born very prematurely and cared for according to the newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program (NIDCAP) Acta Paediatr. 2004;93(4):498–507. doi: 10.1080/08035250410023548.
    1. McAnulty G, Duffy F, Butler S, Bernstein J, Zurakowski D, Als H. Effects of the newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program (NIDCAP) at age 8 years: preliminary data. Clin Pediatr. 2010;49(3):258–270. doi: 10.1177/0009922809335668.
    1. Stephens BE, Vohr BR. Neurodevelopmental outcome of the premature infant. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2009;56(3):631–646. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2009.03.005.
    1. Imdad A, Yakoob MY, Siddiqui S, Bhutta ZA. Screening and triage of intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR) in general population and high risk pregnancies: a systematic review with a focus on reduction of IUGR related stillbirths. BMC Publ Health. 2011;11(Suppl 3):S1. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-S3-S1.
    1. Low JA, Handley-Derry MH, Burke SO, Peters RD, Pater EA, Killen HL, Derrick EJ. Association of intrauterine fetal growth retardation and learning deficits at age 9 to 11 years. Am J Obstet Gyn. 1992;167(6):1499–1505.
    1. Geva R, Eshel R, Leitner Y, Valevski AF, Harel S. Neuropsychological outcomes of children with intrauterine growth restriction: A 9-year prospective study. Pediatrics. 2006;118(1):91–100. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2343.
    1. Leitner Y, Fattal-Valevski A, Geva R, Eshel R, Toledano-Alhadef H, Rotstein M, Bassan H, Radianu B, Bitchonsky O, Jaffa A. Neurodevelopmental outcome of children with intrauterine growth retardation: a longitudinal, 10-year prospective study. J Child Neurol. 2007;22(5):580–587. doi: 10.1177/0883073807302605.
    1. Guellec I, Lapillonne A, Renolleau S, Charlaluk M, Roze J, Marret S, Vieux R, Monique K, Ancel P. Group. ES. Neurologic outcomes at school age in very preterm infants born with severe or mild growth restriction. Pediatrics. 2011;127(4):e883–e891. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-2442. Epub 2011 Mar 7.
    1. Martin JA, Hamilton BE, Sutton PD, Ventura SJ, Menacker F, Kirmeyer S. Division of Vital Statistics. Births: Final Data for 2004. National Vital Statistics Reports. 2005;55(1)
    1. Anderson PJ, Doyle LW. Group TVICS. Executive functioning in school-aged children who were born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight in the 1990s. Pediatrics. 2004;114:50–57. doi: 10.1542/peds.114.1.50.
    1. Taylor H, Klein N, Drotar D, Schluchter M, Hack M. Consequences and risks of <1000-g birth weight for neuropsychological skills, achievement, and adaptive functioning. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27:459–470. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200612000-00002.
    1. Petrou S, Mehta Z, Hockley C, Cook-Mozaffari P, Henderson J, Goldacre M. The impact of preterm birth on hospital inpatient admissions and costs during the first 5 years of life. Pediatrics. 2003;112(6):1290–1297. doi: 10.1542/peds.112.6.1290.
    1. Duffy FH, Als H, McAnulty GB. Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for gestational age effects in healthy preterm and fullterm infants studied 2 weeks after expected due date. Child Dev. 1990;61:1271–1286. doi: 10.2307/1130893.
    1. Sizonenko S, Borradori-Tolsa C, Vauthay D, Lodygensky G, Lazeyras F, Huppi P. Impact of intrauterine growth restriction and glucocorticoids on brain development: Insights using advanced magnetic resonance imaging. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2006;254-255:163–171.
    1. Lodygensky G, Seghier M, Warfield S, Tolsa C, Sizonenko S, Lazeyras F, Huppi P. Intrauterine growth restriction affects the preterm infant’s hippocampus. Pediatr Res. 2008;63(4):438–443. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318165c005.
    1. Als H. Toward a synactive theory of development: promise for the assessment of infant individuality. Inf Mental Health J. 1982;3:229–243. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(198224)3:4<229::AID-IMHJ2280030405>;2-H.
    1. Als H. Manual for the naturalistic observation of the newborn (preterm and fullterm) Boston: The Children’s Hospital; 1984. vol. Revision.
    1. Als H. In: Developmental interventions in the neonatal intensive care nursery. Goldson E, editor. New York: Oxford University Press; 1999. Reading the premature infant; pp. 18–85.
    1. Gilkerson L, Als H. Role of reflective process in the implementation of developmentally supportive care in the newborn intensive care unit. Inf Young Child. 1995;7:20–28. doi: 10.1097/00001163-199504000-00005.
    1. Ballard JL. A simplified assessment of gestational age. Pediatr Res. 1977;11:374.
    1. Gairdner D, Pearson J. A growth chart for premature and other infants. Arch Dis Child. 1971;46:783–787. doi: 10.1136/adc.46.250.783.
    1. Als H. Children’s Hospital family interview: early experience 8 year Old interview. Boston: Children’s Hospital; 1990. Revised 2007.
    1. Casey MB, Cohen M, Schuerholz LJ, Singer HS, Denckla MB. Language-based cognitive functioning in parents of offspring with ADHD comorbid for tourette syndrome or learning disabilities. Dev Neuropsychol. 2000;17(1):85–111. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1701_06.
    1. Whitley E, Gale CR, Deary IJ, Kivimaki M, Batty GD. Association of maternal and paternal IQ with offspring conduct, emotional, and attention problem scores: transgenerational evidence from the 1958 british birth cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(10):1032–1038. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.111.
    1. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman brief intelligence test. 2. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2004.
    1. Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty G, Butler S, Lightbody L, Kosta S, Weisenfeld N, Robertson R, Parad R, Ringer SA. NIDCAP improves brain function and structure in preterm infants with severe intrauterine growth restriction. J Perinatol. 2012;32(10):797–803. doi: 10.1038/jp.2011.201.
    1. Castillo M. Selective vulnerability and the cerebellum in neonates. Am J Neuroradiol. 2007;28:20–21.
    1. Limperopoulos C, Bassan H, Gauvreau K, Robertson RL, Sullivan NR, Benson CB, Avery L, Stewart J, Soul JS, Ringer SA. Does cerebellar injury in premature infants contribute to the high prevalence of long-term cognitive, learning, and behavioral disability in survivors? Pediatrics. 2007;120(3):584–593. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-1041.
    1. Parker J, Mitchell A, Kalpakidou A, Walshe M, Jung HY, Nosarti C, Santosh P, Rifkin L, Wyatt J, Murray RM. Cerebellar growth and behavioural & neuropsychological outcome in preterm adolescents. Brain. 2008;131(5):1344–1351.
    1. Volpe JJ. Cerebellum of the premature infant: rapidly developing, vulnerable, clinically important. J Child Neurol. 2009;24(9):1085–1104. doi: 10.1177/0883073809338067.
    1. Volpe JJ. Brain injury in premature infants: a complex amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:110–124. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70294-1.
    1. Tam EW, Chau V, Ferriero DM, Barkovich AJ, Poskitt KJ, Studholme C, Fok ED, Grunau RE, Glidden DV, Miller SP. Preterm cerebellar growth impairment after postnatal exposure to glucocorticoids. Sci Transl Med. 2011;3(105):105ra105. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3002884.
    1. Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain. 1998;121(4):561–579. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.4.561.
    1. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman assessment battery for children (KABC-II) 2. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2003.
    1. Woodcock RW, McGrew KS, Mather N. Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing; 2001.
    1. Osterrieth PA. Le test de copie d’une figure complexe. Arch Psychologie. 1944;30:206–356.
    1. Beebe DW, Ris MD, Brown TM, Dietrich KN. Executive functioning and memory for the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test among community adolescents. Appl Neuropsychol. 2004;11(2):91–98. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1102_4.
    1. Somerville J, Tremont G, Stern RA. The boston qualitative scoring system as a measure of executive functioning in Rey-osterrieth complex figure performance. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22(5):613–621. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200010)22:5;1-9;FT613.
    1. Bernstein JH, Waber DP. Developmental scoring system for the Rey-osterrieth complex figure. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1996.
    1. Waber DP, McCormick MC, Workman-Daniels K. Neurobehavioral outcomes in very low birthweight, low birthweight, and normal birthweight children with and without medical complications. Galveston, Texas: ; 1992. (Abstract presented at the international neuropsychological society 21st annual meeting).
    1. Waber D, McCormick M. Late neurophysiological outcomes in preterm infants of normal IQ: selective vulnerability of the visual system. J Pediat Psychol. 1995;20:721–735. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/20.6.721.
    1. Waber DP. In: Handbook of Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Usage: Clinical and Research Applications. Kaplan E, Knight J, editor. Lutz, Fl: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2003. Parsing children’s productions of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: What Develops?
    1. Holmes Bernstein J, Waber DP. Executive function in education: from theory to practice. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. Executive capacities from a developmental perspective.
    1. Srinvasan R, Winter WR, Ding J, Nunez PL. EEG and MEG coherence: measures of functional connectivity at distinct spatial scales of neocortical dynamics. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;166(1):41–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.06.026.
    1. Lins OG, Picton TW, Berg P, Scherg M. Ocular artifacts in recording EEGs and event-related potentials. II: source dipoles and source components. Brain Topogr. 1993;6(1):65–78. doi: 10.1007/BF01234128.
    1. Berg P, Scherg M. Dipole modeling of eye activity and its application to the removal of eye artifacts from EEG and MEG. Clin Phys Physiol Meas. 1991;12(Suppl A):49–54.
    1. van Drongelen W. Signal Processing for Neuroscientists: An Introduction to the Analysis of Physiological Signals. Volume 5. Oxford: Elsevier; 2011.
    1. Semlitsch HV, Anderer P, Schuster P, Presslich O. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology. 1986;23(6):695–703. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00696.x.
    1. Duffy FH, Jones K, Bartels P, McAnulty G, Albert M. Unrestricted principal components analysis of brain electrical activity: Issues of data dimensionality, artifact, and utility. Brain Topogr. 1992;4(4):291–307. doi: 10.1007/BF01135567.
    1. Foley DH. Consideration of sample and feature size. IEEE Trans Inform Theory. 1972;IT-18:618–626.
    1. Bartels PH. Numerical evaluation of cytologic data III. Selection of features for discrimination. Anal Quant Cytol. 1979;1:153–159.
    1. Schmahmann JD. Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;16:367–378. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.3.367.
    1. Weisenfeld N, Warfield SK. Automatic segmentation of newborn brain MRI. NeuroImage. 2009;47:564–572. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.068.
    1. Weisenfeld NI, Warfield SK. Learning likelihoods for labeling (L3): a general multi-classifier segmentation algorithm. Med Image Comput Comput Assist Interv. 2011;14(Pt 3):322–329.
    1. Dixon WJ. BMDP statistical software manual. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1988.
    1. Aickin M, Gensler H. Adjusting for multiple testing when reporting research results: the bonferroni vs holm methods. Am J Pub Health. 1996;86(5):726–728. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.86.5.726.
    1. Siegel S. Non-parametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1956.
    1. Cohen J. Statistical power for analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press; 1969.
    1. Rao CR. Advanced statistical methods in biometric research. New York: Hafner Press; 1974.
    1. Lachenbruch PA. Discriminant analysis. New York: Hafner Press; 1975.
    1. Lachenbruch P, Mickey RM. Estimation of error rates in discriminant analysis. Technometrics. 1968;10:1–11. doi: 10.1080/00401706.1968.10490530.
    1. Als H, Lester BM, Tronick EZ, Brazelton TB. In: Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics. Volume 1. Fitzgerald HE, Lester BM, Yogman MW, editor. New York: Plenum Press; 1982. Manual for the assessment of preterm infants’ behavior (APIB) pp. 65–132.
    1. Davis A, Pierson E, Finch W. A canonical correlation analysis of intelligence and executive functioning. Appl Neuropsychol. 2011;18(1):61–68. doi: 10.1080/09084282.2010.523392.
    1. Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Schabus M, Doppelmayr M. Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. Int J Psychophysiol. 2005;57(2):97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.018.
    1. Brain Development Cooperative Group. Total and regional brain volumes in a population-based normative sample from 4 to 18 years: the NIH MRI study of normal brain development. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(1):1–12.
    1. Fair DA, Cohen AL, Power JD, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Functional brain networks develop from a “local to ditributed” organization. PLoS Comput Biol. 2009;5(5):e1000381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381.
    1. D’Esposito M, Aguirre GK, Zarahan E, Ballard D, Shin RK, Lease J. Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. Cognit Brain Res. 1998;7:1–13. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(98)00004-4.
    1. D’Esposito M, Postle BR, Ballard D, Lease J. Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Cognit. 1999;41:66–86. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1999.1096.
    1. Postle BR, Stern CE, Rosen BR, Corkin S. An fMRI investigation of cortical contributions to spatial and nonspatial visual working memory. NeuroImage. 2000;11(5):409–423. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0570.
    1. Song J, Jiang Y. Visual working memory for simple and complex features: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2006;30:963–972. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.006.
    1. Todd JJ, Han SW, Harrison S, Marois R. The neural correlates of visual working memory encoding: a time-resolved fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1527–1536. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.040.
    1. Schmahmann JD, Caplan D. Cognition, emotion and the cerebellum. Brain. 2006;129(2):290–292.
    1. Tavano A, Grasso R, Gagliardi C, Triulzi F, Bresolin A, Fabbro F, Borgatti R. Disorders of cognitive and affective development in cerebellar malformations. Brain. 2007;130(10):2646–2660. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm201.
    1. Krienen FM, Buckner RL. Segregated fronto-cerebellar circuits revealed by functional connectivity. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(10):2485–2497. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp135.
    1. Schmahmann JD, MacMore J, Vangel M. Cerebellar stroke without motor deficit: clinical evidence for motor and non-motor domains within the human cerebellum. J Neurosci. 2009;162:852–861. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.06.023.
    1. Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. Cortex. 2010;46(7):831–844. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.008.
    1. Stoodley CJ, Valera EM, Schmahmann JD. An fMRI case study of functional topography in the human cerebellum. Behav Neurol. 2010;23(1):65–79.
    1. Schmahmann JD. The role of the cerebellum in cognition and emotion: personal reflections since 1982 on the dysmetria of thought hypothesis, and its historical evolution from theory to therapy. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010;20(3):236–260. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9142-x.
    1. Kelly RM, Strick PL. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J Neurosci. 2003;23(23):8432–84444.
    1. Chheda M, Sherman J, Schmahmann JD. Neurologic, psychiatric and cognitive manifestations in cerebellar agenesis. Neurology. 2002;58(suppl 3):356–370.
    1. Bellebaum C, Daum I. Cerebellar involvement in executive control. Biomed and Life Sci. 2007;6(3):184–192.
    1. Balsters JH, Cussans E, Diedrichsen J, Phillips KA, Preuss TM, Rilling JK, Ramnani N. Evolution of the cerebellar cortex: the selective expansion of prefrontal-projecting cerebellar lobules. NeuroImage. 2010;49(3):2045–2052. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.045.
    1. Total and regional brain volumes in a population-based normative sample from 4 to 18 years: the NIH MRI study of normal brain development. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(1):1–12.
    1. Hashimoto K, Shimizu T, Shimoya K, Kanzaki T, Clapp JF, Murata Y. Fetal cerebellum: US appearance with advancing gestational Age. Radiology. 2001;221(1):70–74. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2211001583.
    1. Meany MJ, Mitchell JB, Aitken DH, Bhatnager S. The effects of neonatal handling on the development of the adrenocortical response to stress: Implications for neuropathology and cognitive deficits in later life. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1991;16(1–3):85–103.
    1. Bhutta AT, Anand KJS. Vulnerability of the developing brain neuronal mechanisms. Clin Perinatol. 2002;29(3):357–372. doi: 10.1016/S0095-5108(02)00011-8.
    1. Kolber B, Boyle M, Wieczorek L, Kelley C, Onwuzurike C, Nettles S, Vogt S, Muglia L. Transient early-life forebrain corticotropin-releasing hormone elevation causes long-lasting anxiogenic and despair-like changes in mice. J Neurosci. 2010;30(7):2571–2581. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4470-09.2010.
    1. Brummelte S, Grunau RE, Chau V, Poskitt KJ, Brant R, Vinall J, Gover A, Synnes AR, Miller SP. Procedural pain and brain development in premature newborns. Ann Neurol. 2012;71(3):385–396. doi: 10.1002/ana.22267.
    1. Bourgeois JP. Synaptogenesis, heterochrony and epigenesis in the mammalian neocortex. Acta Paediatr Suppl. 1997;422:27–33.
    1. Liu D, Diorio J, Day JC, Francis DD, Meaney M. Maternal care, hippocampal synaptogenesis and cognitive development in rats. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(8):799–806. doi: 10.1038/77702.
    1. Shore R. Rethinking the brain: New insights into early development. New York: Families and Work Institute; 1997.
    1. Gunnar MR. Quality of care and the buffering of stress physiology: its potential in protecting the developing human brain. Prev Med. 1998;27(2):208–211. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1998.0276.
    1. Perry BD, Pollard RA, Blakley TL, Baker WL, Vigilante D. Childhood trauma, the neurobiology of adaptation, and “use dependent” development of the brain: how “states” become “traits”. Infant Ment Health J. 1995;259:271–291.

Source: PubMed

3
订阅