Parental presence and holding in the neonatal intensive care unit and associations with early neurobehavior

L C Reynolds, M M Duncan, G C Smith, A Mathur, J Neil, T Inder, R G Pineda, L C Reynolds, M M Duncan, G C Smith, A Mathur, J Neil, T Inder, R G Pineda

Abstract

Objective: To investigate the effects of parental presence and infant holding in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) on neurobehavior at term equivalent.

Study design: Prospective cohort enrolled 81 infants born 30 weeks gestation. Nurses tracked parent visitation, holding and skin-to-skin care throughout the NICU hospitalization. At term, the NICU Network Neurobehavioral Scale was administered. Associations between visitation, holding and early neurobehavior were determined using linear and logistic regression.

Result: The mean hours per week of parent visitation was 21.33±20.88 (median=13.90; interquartile range 10.10 to 23.60). Infants were held an average of 2.29±1.47 days per week (median=2.00; interquartile range 1.20 to 3.10). Over the hospital stay, visitation hours decreased (P=0.01), while holding frequencies increased (P<0.001). More visitation was associated with better quality of movement (P=0.02), less arousal (P=0.01), less excitability (P=0.03), more lethargy (P=0.01) and more hypotonia (P<0.01). More holding was associated with improved quality of movement (P<0.01), less stress (P<0.01), less arousal (P=0.04) and less excitability (P<0.01).

Conclusion: Infants of caregivers who were visited and held more often in the NICU had differences in early neurobehavior by term equivalent, which supports the need for and importance of early parenting in the NICU.

Conflict of interest statement

There are no conflicts of interest to report.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Patterns of Visitation and Holding Across the Length of Stay Note: TE=Term equivalent age. The first time point in each plot represents the first two weeks of life; the second time point in each plot represents weeks three and four of life; the third time point represents week five through term-equivalent age; and the final time point represents a summary of what occurred from birth through term-equivalent.
Figure 1
Figure 1
Patterns of Visitation and Holding Across the Length of Stay Note: TE=Term equivalent age. The first time point in each plot represents the first two weeks of life; the second time point in each plot represents weeks three and four of life; the third time point represents week five through term-equivalent age; and the final time point represents a summary of what occurred from birth through term-equivalent.

References

    1. Anderson P, Doyle LW. Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;289(24):3264–72.
    1. World Health Organization. Classification, Assessment, Surveys and Terminology Team. World Health Organization; 2001. Classification A, Surveys,, Team T. ICIDH-2: International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health: Final Draft, Full Version.
    1. Lindberg B, Öhrling K. Experiences of having a prematurely born infant from the perspective of mothers in northern Sweden. International journal of circumpolar health. 2008;67(5)
    1. Fanaroff AKJ, Klaus M. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: the predictive value of maternal visiting patterns. Pediatrics. 1972;49:287–90.
    1. Latva R, Lehtonen L, Salmelin R, Tamminen T. Visits by the family to the neonatal intensive care unit. Acta Paediatrica. 2006;96(2):215–20.
    1. Franck LS, Spencer C. Parent visiting and participation in infant caregiving activities in a neonatal unit. Birth. 2003;30(1):31–5.
    1. Reid M, Lloyd D, Campbell G, Murray K, Porter M. Scottish neonatal intensive care units; a study of staff and parental attitudes. Health bulletin. 1995;53(5):314.
    1. Rosenfield AG. Visiting in the intensive care nursery. Child Development. 1980:939–41.
    1. Zeskind PS, Iacino R. Effects of maternal visitation to preterm infants in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit. Child Development. 1984:1887–93.
    1. Latva R, Lehtonen L, Salmelin RK, Tamminen T. Visiting less than every day: a marker for later behavioral problems in Finnish preterm infants. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2004;158(12):1153.
    1. Örtenstrand A, Westrup B, Broström EB, Sarman I, Åkerström S, Brune T, et al. The Stockholm Neonatal Family Centered Care Study: effects on length of stay and infant morbidity. Pediatrics. 2010;125(2):e278–e85.
    1. Newnham C, Inder T, Milgrom J. Measuring preterm cumulative stressors within the NICU: The neonatal infant stressor scale. Early human development. 2009;85(9):549–55.
    1. Lester BM, Tronick EZ, LaGasse L, Seifer R, Bauer CR, Shankaran S, et al. The maternal lifestyle study: effects of substance exposure during pregnancy on neurodevelopmental outcome in 1-month-old infants. Pediatrics. 2002;110(6):1182–92.
    1. Cockburn F, Cooke R. The CRIB (Clinical Risk Index For Babies) score: A tool for assessing initial neonatal risk. Lancet. 1993;342(8865):193–8.
    1. Barrington KJ. The adverse neuro-developmental effects of postnatal steroids in the preterm infant: a systematic review of RCTs. BMC pediatrics. 2001;1(1):1.
    1. Franz AR, Pohlandt F, Bode H, Mihatsch WA, Sander S, Kron M, et al. Intrauterine, early neonatal, and postdischarge growth and neurodevelopmental outcome at 5.4 years in extremely preterm infants after intensive neonatal nutritional support. Pediatrics. 2009;123(1):e101–e9.
    1. Miller SP, Ferriero DM, Leonard C, Piecuch R, Glidden DV, Partridge JC, et al. Early brain injury in premature newborns detected with magnetic resonance imaging is associated with adverse early neurodevelopmental outcome. The Journal of pediatrics. 2005;147(5):609–16.
    1. Gonya J, Nelin LD. Factors associated with maternal visitation and participation in skin-to-skin care in an all referral level IIIc NICU. Acta Paediatrica. 2012
    1. Whitelaw A, Sleath K. Myth of the marsupial mother: home care of very low birth weight babies in Bogota, Colombia. Lancet. 1985;1(8439):1206.
    1. Aucott S, Donohue PK, Atkins E, Allen MC. Neurodevelopmental care in the NICU. Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews. 2002;8(4):298–308.
    1. Boukydis CFZ, Bigsby R, Lester BM. Clinical use of the neonatal intensive care unit network neurobehavioral scale. Pediatrics. 2004;113(Supplement 2):679–89.
    1. Hoare BJ, Imms C. Upper-limb injections of botulinum toxin-A in children with cerebral palsy: a critical review of the literature and clinical implications for occupational therapists. The American journal of occupational therapy. 2004;58(4):389–97.
    1. Johnson RE, Jones HE, Jasinski DR, Svikis DS, Haug NA, Jansson LM, et al. Buprenorphine treatment of pregnant opioid-dependent women: maternal and neonatal outcomes. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2001;63(1):97–103.
    1. Liu J, Bann C, Lester B, Tronick E, Das A, Lagasse L, et al. Neonatal neurobehavior predicts medical and behavioral outcome. Pediatrics. 2010;125(1):e90–e8.
    1. Tronick EZ, Olson K, Rosenberg R, Bohne L, Lu J, Lester BM. Normative neurobehavioral performance of healthy infants on the neonatal intensive care unit network neurobehavioral scale. Pediatrics. 2004;113(Supplement 2):676–8.

Source: PubMed

3
S'abonner