A Behavioral Measure of Costly Helping: Replicating and Extending the Association with Callous Unemotional Traits in Male Adolescents

Joseph T Sakai, Manish S Dalwani, Susan K Mikulich-Gilbertson, Shannon K McWilliams, Kristen M Raymond, Thomas J Crowley, Joseph T Sakai, Manish S Dalwani, Susan K Mikulich-Gilbertson, Shannon K McWilliams, Kristen M Raymond, Thomas J Crowley

Abstract

Background: Some conduct-disordered youths have high levels of callous unemotional traits and meet the DSM-5's "with limited prosocial emotions" (LPE) specifier. These youths often do aggressive, self-benefitting acts that cost others. We previously developed a task, the AlAn's game, which asks participants to repeatedly decide whether to accept or reject offers in which they will receive money but a planned charity donation will be reduced. In our prior work, more "costly helping" (i.e., rejecting the offered money and protecting the donation) was associated with lower callous unemotional traits. Here we extend that prior work in a larger sample of adolescent male patients with serious conduct problems and controls, and test whether this association is mediated specifically by a Moral Elevation response (i.e., a positive emotional response to another's act of virtue).

Methods: The adolescent male participants were: 45 patients (23 with LPE) and 26 controls, who underwent an extensive phenotypic assessment including a measure of Moral Elevation. About 1 week later participants played the AlAn's game.

Results: All AlAn's game outcomes demonstrated significant group effects: (1) money taken for self (p = 0.02); (2) money left in the charitable donation (p = 0.03); and, (3) costly helping (p = 0.047). Controls took the least money and did the most costly helping, while patients with LPE took the most money and did the least costly helping. Groups also significantly differed in post-stimulus Moral Elevation scores (p = 0.005). Exploratory analyses supported that the relationship between callous unemotional traits and costly helping on the AlAn's game may be mediated in part by differences in Moral Elevation.

Conclusions: The AlAn's game provides a standardized behavioral measure associated with callous unemotional traits. Adolescents with high levels of callous unemotional traits engage in fewer costly helping behaviors, and those differences may be related to blunting of positive emotional responses.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: T. Crowley recently served on the National Advisory Council of the National Institute on Drug Abuse, and on a Task Force of the American Psychiatric Association for drafting the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Edition 5. Dr. Sakai received reimbursement in 2012 for completing a policy review for the WellPoint Office of Medical Policy & Technology Assessment (OMPTA), WellPoint, Inc., Thousand Oaks, CA. He also served as a board member of the ARTS (Addiction Research and Treatment Services) Foundation. The other authors declare no competing interests. Dr. Sakai's and Crowley's competing interests do not alter the authors' adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.

Figures

Fig 1. Examples of the different trial…
Fig 1. Examples of the different trial types in the AlAn’s game.
Panel A shows an Active Trial where the participant will receive 64 cents and the Red Cross donation will be reduced by 2 cents. Participants are asked to accept or reject this offer. Panel B shows an Attentional Control Trial where the participant will lose 2 cents and the Red Cross donation will be reduced by 8 cents. We term this kind of Attention Control trials, “Logically-Reject” Trials. Panel C shows an Attention Control Trial where the participant will gain 32 cents and the Red Cross donation won’t change. We term these “Logically-Accept” Trials. Panel D shows a Calculation Trial where the You number (+8) is not bigger than the Red Cross number +16). Note: The circle remains red for 5 seconds, allowing participants to view the trial content. Then the circle turns green and subjects have 1 second to press either yes (accept) vs. no (reject).
Fig 2. Results from the AlAn’s Game…
Fig 2. Results from the AlAn’s Game in-magnet study (left panel) and results from the previously published out-of-magnet study (right panel).
Error bars indicate standard error. Money taken for self in blue and money left in the Red Cross donation in orange.
Fig 3
Fig 3
Aggregate Elevation Scores Prior to (Panel A) and Post-stimulus Administration (Panel B); error bars indicate standard errors. Mediation Model, testing whether Elevation mediates the association between level of Callous Unemotional Traits and AlAn’s Game behavior (Panel C). Bracket above bars indicates ANOVA was significant for group differences; * p

References

    1. APA. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition Arlington, VA: APA; 2013.
    1. Nock MK, Kazdin AE, Hiripi E, Kessler RC. Prevalence, subtypes, and correlates of DSM-IV conduct disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychol Med. 2006;36(5):699–710. 10.1017/S0033291706007082
    1. Kessler RC, Avenevoli S, Costello EJ, Georgiades K, Green JG, Gruber MJ, et al. Prevalence, persistence, and sociodemographic correlates of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(4):372–80. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.160
    1. Hopfer C, Salomonsen-Sautel S, Mikulich-Gilbertson S, Min SJ, McQueen M, Crowley T, et al. Conduct disorder and initiation of substance use: a prospective longitudinal study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(5):511–8 e4 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.02.014
    1. Armstrong TD, Costello EJ. Community studies on adolescent substance use, abuse, or dependence and psychiatric comorbidity. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002;70(6):1224–39. .
    1. Thurstone C, Salomonsen-Sautel S, Mikulich-Gilbertson SK, Hartman CA, Sakai JT, Hoffenberg AS, et al. Prevalence and predictors of injection drug use and risky sexual behaviors among adolescents in substance treatment. Am J Addict. 2013;22(6):558–65. 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2013.12064.x
    1. Zoccolillo M, Rogers K. Characteristics and outcome of hospitalized adolescent girls with conduct disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1991;30(6):973–81. 10.1097/00004583-199111000-00016 .
    1. Martin RL, Cloninger CR, Guze SB, Clayton PJ. Mortality in a follow-up of 500 psychiatric outpatients. II. Cause-specific mortality. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42(1):58–66. .
    1. Black DW, Baumgard CH, Bell SE, Kao C. Death rates in 71 men with antisocial personality disorder. A comparison with general population mortality. Psychosomatics. 1996;37(2):131–6. 10.1016/S0033-3182(96)71579-7 .
    1. Rydelius PA. The development of antisocial behaviour and sudden violent death. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1988;77(4):398–403. .
    1. Teplin LA, Jakubowski JA, Abram KM, Olson ND, Stokes ML, Welty LJ. Firearm homicide and other causes of death in delinquents: a 16-year prospective study. Pediatrics. 2014;134(1):63–73. 10.1542/peds.2013-3966
    1. Crowley TJ, Mikulich SK, Ehlers KM, Whitmore EA, MacDonald MJ. Validity of structured clinical evaluations in adolescents with conduct and substance problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(3):265–73. 10.1097/00004583-200103000-00005 .
    1. Coker KL, Smith PH, Westphal A, Zonana HV, McKee SA. Crime and psychiatric disorders among youth in the US population: an analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey-Adolescent Supplement. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(8):888–98, 98 e1–2. 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.007
    1. Wasserman GA, McReynolds LS, Lucas CP, Fisher P, Santos L. The voice DISC-IV with incarcerated male youths: prevalence of disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(3):314–21. 10.1097/00004583-200203000-00011 .
    1. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):2693–8. 10.1073/pnas.1010076108
    1. Cohen M, Piquero A. New Evidence on the Monetary Value of Saving a High Risk Youth. Journal Quant Criminology. 2009;25:25–49. 10.1007/s10940-008-9057-3
    1. Steiner H, Dunne JE. Summary of the practice parameters for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with conduct disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(10):1482–5. .
    1. Moffitt TE. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychol Rev. 1993;100(4):674–701. .
    1. Lahey BB. What we need to know about callous-unemotional traits: comment on Frick, Ray, Thornton, and Kahn (2014). Psychol Bull. 2014;140(1):58–63. 10.1037/a0033387
    1. Frick PJ, Cornell AH, Barry CT, Bodin SD, Dane HE. Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity, aggression, and self-report of delinquency. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31(4):457–70. .
    1. Manders WA, Dekovic M, Asscher JJ, van der Laan PH, Prins PJ. Psychopathy as predictor and moderator of multisystemic therapy outcomes among adolescents treated for antisocial behavior. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41(7):1121–32. 10.1007/s10802-013-9749-5 .
    1. Hogstrom J, Enebrink P, Ghaderi A. The moderating role of child callous-unemotional traits in an Internet-based parent-management training program. J Fam Psychol. 2013;27(2):314–23. 10.1037/a0031883 .
    1. Gao Y, Glenn AL, Schug RA, Yang Y, Raine A. The neurobiology of psychopathy: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Can J Psychiatry. 2009;54(12):813–23. .
    1. Anderson NE, Kiehl KA. Psychopathy and aggression: when paralimbic dysfunction leads to violence. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2014;17:369–93. 10.1007/7854_2013_257
    1. Blair RJ. The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14(11):786–99. 10.1038/nrn3577
    1. Blair RJ, Leibenluft E, Pine DS. Conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits in youth. N Engl J Med. 2014;371(23):2207–16. 10.1056/NEJMra1315612 .
    1. Blair RJ, Colledge E, Murray L, Mitchell DG. A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2001;29(6):491–8. .
    1. Blair RJ, Budhani S, Colledge E, Scott S. Deafness to fear in boys with psychopathic tendencies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(3):327–36. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00356.x .
    1. Decety J, Michalska KJ, Akitsuki Y, Lahey BB. Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: a functional MRI investigation. Biol Psychol. 2009;80(2):203–11. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004
    1. Blair RJ, Jones L, Clark F, Smith M. The psychopathic individual: a lack of responsiveness to distress cues? Psychophysiology. 1997;34(2):192–8. .
    1. Carlo G, Mestre MV, McGinley MM, Tur-Porcar A, Samper P, Opal D. The protective role of prosocial behaviors on antisocial behaviors: the mediating effects of deviant peer affiliation. J Adolesc. 2014;37(4):359–66. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.02.009 .
    1. Algoe SB, Haidt J. Witnessing excellence in action: the 'other-praising' emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. J Posit Psychol. 2009;4(2):105–27. 10.1080/17439760802650519
    1. Haidt J. Elevation and the positive psychology of morality In: Keyes C, Haidt J., editor. Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2003. p. 275–89.
    1. Aquino K, McFerran B, Laven M. Moral identity and the experience of moral elevation in response to acts of uncommon goodness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011;100(4):703–18. 10.1037/a0022540 .
    1. Freeman D, Aquino K, McFerran B. Overcoming beneficiary race as an impediment to charitable donations: social dominance orientation, the experience of moral elevation, and donation behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2009;35(1):72–84. 10.1177/0146167208325415 .
    1. Sakai JT, Dalwani MS, Gelhorn HL, Mikulich-Gilbertson SK, Crowley TJ. A behavioral test of accepting benefits that cost others: associations with conduct problems and callous-unemotionality. PLoS One. 2012;7(4):e36158 10.1371/journal.pone.0036158
    1. Harbaugh WT, Mayr U, Burghart DR. Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science. 2007;316(5831):1622–5. 10.1126/science.1140738 .
    1. Sawyer J. The altruism scale: a measure of co-operative, individualistic, and competitive interpersonal orientation. AJS. 1966;71(4):407–16. .
    1. Moll J, Krueger F, Zahn R, Pardini M, de Oliveira-Souza R, Grafman J. Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(42):15623–8. 10.1073/pnas.0604475103
    1. Hoffman E. M K, Sharchat K., Smith V. Preferences, property rights and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior. 1994;7(3):346–80.
    1. Eckel C, Grossman P. Altruism in anonymous Dictatator Games. Games and Economic Behavior. 1996;16:181–91.
    1. Knoch D, Gianotti LR, Baumgartner T, Fehr E. A neural marker of costly punishment behavior. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(3):337–42. 10.1177/0956797609360750 .
    1. Henrich J, McElreath R, Barr A, Ensminger J, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, et al. Costly punishment across human societies. Science. 2006;312(5781):1767–70. 10.1126/science.1127333 .
    1. Przepiorka W, Diekmann A. Individual heterogeneity and costly punishment: a volunteer's dilemma. Proc Biol Sci. 2013;280(1759):20130247 10.1098/rspb.2013.0247
    1. Fehr E. Human behaviour: don't lose your reputation. Nature. 2004;432(7016):449–50. 10.1038/432449a .
    1. Trivers R. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol. 1971;46:35–57.
    1. Konner M. Conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits in youth. N Engl J Med. 2015;372(8):784 10.1056/NEJMc1415936#SA1 .
    1. Eme RF. Sex differences in child-onset, life-course-persistent conduct disorder. A review of biological influences. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27(5):607–27. 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.02.001 .
    1. Eme RF, Kavanaugh L. Sex differences in conduct disorder. J Clin Child Psychol. 1995;4:406–26.
    1. Loeber R, Burke JD, Lahey BB, Winters A, Zera M. Oppositional defiant and conduct disorder: a review of the past 10 years, part I. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(12):1468–84. 10.1097/00004583-200012000-00007 .
    1. Eisenberg N, Lennon R. Sex Differences in Empathy and Related Capacities. Psychol Bull. 1983;94(1):100–31.
    1. Eisenberg N, Carlo G, Murphy B, Van Court P. Prosocial development in late adolescence: a longitudinal study. Child Dev. 1995;66(4):1179–97. .
    1. Essau CA, Sasagawa S, Frick PJ. Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment. 2006;13(4):454–69. 10.1177/1073191106287354 .
    1. Baum K. Juvenile victimization and offending, 1993–2003 In: US Department of Justice; 2005. National Crime Victimization Survey BoJSPN, editor. Washington DC: US Department of Justice; 2005.
    1. Wrangham D, Peterson D. Demonic Males: Apes and the Origins of Human Violence. New York, NY: Mariner Books, Houghton Mifflin Company; 1996.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated intelligence scale. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1999.
    1. Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas CP, Dulcan MK, Schwab-Stone ME. NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(1):28–38. 10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014 .
    1. Robins LN, Wing J, Wittchen HU, Helzer JE, Babor TF, Burke J, et al. The Composite International Diagnostic Interview. An epidemiologic Instrument suitable for use in conjunction with different diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45(12):1069–77. .
    1. Cottler LB, Schuckit MA, Helzer JE, Crowley T, Woody G, Nathan P, et al. The DSM-IV field trial for substance use disorders: major results. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1995;38(1):59–69; discussion 71–83. .
    1. Frick PJ. The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits. New Orleans, AL: University of Louisiana; 2004.
    1. Achenbach T. Manual for the Youth Self-Report and 1991 Profile. Burlington VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991.
    1. Englander ZA, Haidt J, Morris JP. Neural basis of moral elevation demonstrated through inter-subject synchronization of cortical activity during free-viewing. PLoS One. 2012;7(6):e39384 10.1371/journal.pone.0039384
    1. Hollingshead A, Redlich R. Social class and mental illness: A community study. New York: Wiley; 1958.
    1. Achenbach T. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991.
    1. Frick PJ, Moffitt T. A Proposal to the DSM-V Childhood Disorders and the ADHD and Disruptive Behavior Disorders work Groups to Include a Specifier to the Diagnosis of Conduct Disorder Based on the Presence of Callous-Unemotional Traits: American Psychiatric Association; 2010
    1. Colins OF, Andershed H. The DSM-5 with limited prosocial emotions specifier for conduct disorder among detained girls. Law Hum Behav. 2015;39(2):198–207. 10.1037/lhb0000108 .
    1. Kimonis ER, Fanti KA, Frick PJ, Moffitt TE, Essau C, Bijttebier P, et al. Using self-reported callous-unemotional traits to cross-nationally assess the DSM-5 'With Limited Prosocial Emotions' specifier. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015. 10.1111/jcpp.12357 .
    1. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40(3):879–91. .
    1. Crowley TJ, Dalwani MS, Mikulich-Gilbertson SK, Young SE, Sakai JT, Raymond KM, et al. Adolescents' Neural Processing of Risky Decisions: Effects of Sex and Behavioral Disinhibition. PLoS One. 2015;10(7):e0132322 10.1371/journal.pone.0132322
    1. Crowley TJ, Raymond KM, Mikulich-Gilbertson SK, Thompson LL, Lejuez CW. A risk-taking "set" in a novel task among adolescents with serious conduct and substance problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45(2):175–83. 10.1097/01.chi.0000188893.60551.31 .
    1. Newman JP, Patterson CM, Kosson DS. Response perseveration in psychopaths. J Abnorm Psychol. 1987;96(2):145–8. .
    1. Blair RJ. A cognitive developmental approach to mortality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition. 1995;57(1):1–29. .
    1. Aharoni E, Sinnott-Armstrong W, Kiehl KA. Can psychopathic offenders discern moral wrongs? A new look at the moral/conventional distinction. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121(2):484–97. 10.1037/a0024796
    1. Glenn AL, Raine A, Schug RA, Young L, Hauser M. Increased DLPFC activity during moral decision making in psycholpathology. Mol Psychiatry. 2009;14:909–11.
    1. Tassy S, Deruelle C, Mancini J, Leistedt S, Wicker B. High levels of psychopathic traits alters moral choice but not moral judgment. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:229 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00229
    1. Frick PJ, White SF. Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(4):359–75. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x .
    1. O'Brien BS, Frick PJ. Reward dominance: associations with anxiety, conduct problems, and psychopathy in children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1996;24(2):223–40. .

Source: PubMed

3
S'abonner