Integrative frontal-parietal dynamics supporting cognitive control

Derek Evan Nee, Derek Evan Nee

Abstract

Coordinating among the demands of the external environment and internal plans requires cognitive control supported by a fronto-parietal control network (FPCN). Evidence suggests that multiple control systems span the FPCN whose operations are poorly understood. Previously (Nee and D'Esposito, 2016; 2017), we detailed frontal dynamics that support control processing, but left open their role in broader cortical function. Here, I show that the FPCN consists of an external/present-oriented to internal/future-oriented cortical gradient extending outwardly from sensory-motor cortices. Areas at the ends of this gradient act in a segregative manner, exciting areas at the same level, but suppressing areas at different levels. By contrast, areas in the middle of the gradient excite areas at all levels, promoting integration of control processing. Individual differences in integrative dynamics predict higher level cognitive ability and amenability to neuromodulation. These data suggest that an intermediary zone within the FPCN underlies integrative processing that supports cognitive control.

Keywords: cognitive control; effective connectivity; fMRI; frontoparietal control network; human; macroscale cortical gradient; neuroscience.

Conflict of interest statement

DN No competing interests declared

© 2021, Nee.

Figures

Figure 1.. Comprehensive control task.
Figure 1.. Comprehensive control task.
On each trial, participants observed a letter at a spatial location and made a keypress in response. Keypresses mapped onto ‘yes’/'no' responses. Participants either responded ‘no’ to the stimulus without regard to the stimulus features, or made a choice response based on a pre-learned sequence to a color cued feature (T-A-B-L-E-T for letters; star trace for locations) thereby engaging sensory-motor control. The correct response was either based upon a reference stimulus for which participants responded regarding whether the present stimulus followed a reference stimulus (sequence-back), or whether the stimulus was the start of the sequence (sequence-start). Switching among these tasks engages contextual control. Finally, the reference stimulus could either be the last item presented, or a distant item. In the latter case, the reference item had to be sustained over several trials requiring temporal control to prepare for the future. Colored frames indicated relevant stimulus features (letter or location), whereas frame shapes indicated cognitive control demands which were manipulated in the middle of each block (sub-task). Stimulus domain and cognitive control demands were independently manipulated in a factorial design wherein orthogonal contrasts separately isolated sensory-motor control, contextual control, and temporal control. Refer to the Materials and methods and Nee and D'Esposito, 2016; Nee and D'Esposito, 2017 for a more complete description.
Figure 2.. Control sub-networks.
Figure 2.. Control sub-networks.
(A) Activations for temporal control (red), contextual control (green), and sensory-motor control (blue). Activations are depicted separately for each sample, as well as the samples combined. (B) Activations overlaid to demonstrate the gradient of cognitive control. Spheres indicate the location of regions-of-interest based upon activation peaks. (C) Overlap between activation contrasts (red – temporal control; green – contextual control; blue – sensory-motor control) and the 17-network parcellation described by Yeo et al. FPl – lateral frontal pole; MFG – middle frontal gyrus; VLPFC – ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; cMFG – caudal middle frontal gyrus; IFJ – inferior frontal junction; SFS – superior frontal sulcus; aIPS – anterior intra-parietal sulcus; IPL – inferior parietal lobule; mIPS – mid intra-parietal sulcus; SPL – superior parietal lobule; DMN – default-mode network; DAN – dorsal attention network; SAL – salience network; FPN – frontoparietal network.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.. Contrasts of temporal…
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.. Contrasts of temporal control (red), contextual control (green), and sensory-motor control (blue) and their overlap using reduced, 4 mm full-width half-maximum volumetric smoothing to reduce blurring.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2.. Contrasts of temporal…
Figure 2—figure supplement 2.. Contrasts of temporal control (red), contextual control (green), and sensory-motor control (blue) and their overlap using surface-based processing and reduced smoothing to minimize blurring.
Figure 3.. Activation profiles.
Figure 3.. Activation profiles.
Activations across the eight conditions of the task design are depicted as radar plots (SB – spatial baseline; VB – verbal baseline; SS – spatial switching; VS – verbal switching; SP – spatial planning; VP – verbal planning; SD – spatial dual; VD – verbal dual). The top panel depicts idealized profiles for areas sensitive solely to temporal control (red), contextual control (green), sensory-motor control (blue), verbal stimulus domain (purple), and spatial stimulus domain (orange). Inset: results of multi-dimensional scaling of the activation profiles across regions. Colored by abstraction refers to coloring as a function of position along the control gradient (blue – sensory-motor proximal; green – intermediary; red – sensory-motor distal). Colored by stim domain refers to coloring as a function of sensitivity to stimulus domains (orange – spatial; purple – verbal; gray – neither).
Figure 3—figure supplement 1.. Activation profiles with…
Figure 3—figure supplement 1.. Activation profiles with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 3.
Figure 4.. Brain-behavior relationships.
Figure 4.. Brain-behavior relationships.
(A) Top: Repeated measures correlations between activation and present behavior (i.e. behavior during sub-task trials; see Figure 1). Areas related to sensory-motor control (blue) and contextual control (green) showed positive associations, while areas related to temporal control (red) showed no or negative associations. Bottom: Repeated measures correlations between activation and future behavior (i.e. behavior during return trials; see Figure 1). Areas related to temporal control (red) and contextual control (green) tended to show positive associations, while areas related to sensory-motor control (blue) tended to show no associations. ** indicates Bonferroni-corrected p<0.05. * indicates uncorrected p<0.05. (B) Voxel-wise partial correlations between activation and present behavior (blue), future behavior (red), and both (green). Results are visualized at p<0.001 with 124 voxel cluster extent. (C) Linear mixed effects modeling of the activation-present behavior slope (top) and activation-future behavior slope (bottom) using the first dimension of multi-dimensional scaling (MDS) depicted in Figure 3. * indicates p<0.05; ** indicates p<0.005.
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. Brain-behavior relationships with…
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. Brain-behavior relationships with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 4.
Figure 5.. Estimates of static effective connectivity.
Figure 5.. Estimates of static effective connectivity.
Bars are color coded by source>target lobe pairs. Top: estimates for each connection. Inset: correlation among sample averaged parameter estimates. Bottom: estimates averaged over source>target lobe pairs. Influences arising from the PFC were significantly stronger in magnitude than those arising from the PPC. * indicates p

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.. Estimates of static…

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.. Estimates of static effective connectivity with reduced smoothing.

Details are identical…

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.. Estimates of static effective connectivity with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 5.

Figure 6.. Static network interactions.

Top: Effective…

Figure 6.. Static network interactions.

Top: Effective connectivity was averaged as a function of source…

Figure 6.. Static network interactions.
Top: Effective connectivity was averaged as a function of source>target network. Bars are colored as a function of the source network: TC – temporal control (red); CC – contextual control (green); SC – sensory-motor control (blue). ** denotes Bonferroni-corrected p

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.. Effect of task…

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.. Effect of task signals on effective connectivity parameters and source x…

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.. Effect of task signals on effective connectivity parameters and source x network interactions.
(A) Comparison of effective connectivity parameters with and without regressing out task signals. Bias (e.g. task-induced increases) would be apparent as a shift off of the diagonal (red line). No bias was observed. (B) Source x target network interactions after regressing out task signals. The same interaction was observed after regressing out task signals as was observed in the main text indicating that the interactions were not driven by task signals.

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.. Static network interactions…

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.. Static network interactions with reduced smoothing.

Details are identical to Figure…

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.. Static network interactions with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 6.

Figure 7.. Dynamic Interactions.

( A )…

Figure 7.. Dynamic Interactions.

( A ) Psychophysiological interactions (PPIs) for temporal control, contextual control,…

Figure 7.. Dynamic Interactions.
(A) Psychophysiological interactions (PPIs) for temporal control, contextual control, and stimulus domain contrasts. Solid lines indicate positive modulations of effective connectivity while dashed lines indicate negative modulations of effective connectivity. Arrows are colored by the network of the source node. Thickness of the arrows indicates the strength of modulations. Modulations are visualized at p<0.05 uncorrected. (B) Averaged within- and between-network modulations for the temporal control (red), contextual control (green), and stimulus domain (blue) contrasts. (C) Integration indices computed by contrasting between- minus within-network modulations for temporal control (TC), contextual control (CC), and stimulus domain (SD) contrasts.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. Sample averaged psychophysiological…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. Sample averaged psychophysiological interaction (PPI) parameter estimates.

TC – temporal control;…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. Sample averaged psychophysiological interaction (PPI) parameter estimates.
TC – temporal control; CC – contextual control; SC – sensory-motor control; SD – stimulus domain.

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.. Comparison of the…

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.. Comparison of the sensory-motor control contrast and stimulus domain contrasts.

Stimulus…

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.. Comparison of the sensory-motor control contrast and stimulus domain contrasts.
Stimulus domain is jointly determined by verbal > spatial and spatial > verbal.

Figure 7—figure supplement 3.. Dynamic interactions with…

Figure 7—figure supplement 3.. Dynamic interactions with reduced smoothing.

Details are identical to Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 3.. Dynamic interactions with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 7.

Figure 8.. Integration predicts higher level cognitive…

Figure 8.. Integration predicts higher level cognitive ability.

( A ) Static integration was computed…

Figure 8.. Integration predicts higher level cognitive ability.
(A) Static integration was computed by contrasting between minus within-network effective connectivity of the contextual control network while controlling for (regressing out) the same contrast of the temporal control network and sensory-motor control network and individual differences in head motion. (B) Dynamic integration was based upon the integration index of the contextual control PPI’s while controlling for (regressing out) the integration indices of the temporal control and stimulus domain PPI’s and individual differences in head motion. (C) Higher level cognitive ability was computed as the first principle component of a battery of cognitive tests measuring capacity. (D) Cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict cognitive capacity based upon static and dynamic integration. The scatterplot depicts correlations between the predicted cognitive ability and actual cognitive ability. (E) Correlations among static integration, dynamic integration, and cognitive ability.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.. Activations predict higher…

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.. Activations predict higher level cognitive ability.

Cognitive control and stimulus domain…

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.. Activations predict higher level cognitive ability.
Cognitive control and stimulus domain related activations were used as features to predict cognitive ability in a similar manner to the use of integration measures to predict cognitive ability. Principle components analysis was used to create factors associated with temporal control (temporal control contrast activations in FPl, MFG, and IPL), contextual control (contextual control contrast activations in VLPFC, cMFG, and midIPS), sensory-motor control (sensory-motor control contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL), and stimulus domain (stimulus domain contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL). Note that all contrasts are orthogonal to one another. Cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict cognitive ability based upon the activation measures.

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.. Integration does not…

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.. Integration does not predict higher-level cognitive ability with reduced smoothing.

Details…

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.. Integration does not predict higher-level cognitive ability with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 8D–E.

Figure 8—figure supplement 3.. With reduced smoothing,…

Figure 8—figure supplement 3.. With reduced smoothing, activation-based measures remain significantly predictive of higher-level cognitive…

Figure 8—figure supplement 3.. With reduced smoothing, activation-based measures remain significantly predictive of higher-level cognitive ability.

Figure 9.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation…

Figure 9.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects.

( A ) Previously reportedNee and…

Figure 9.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects.
(A) Previously reportedNee and D'Esposito, 2017 cognitive control impairments induced by continuous theta-burst TMS were combined using principle components analysis to derive a general susceptibility to PFC TMS. (B) Correlations between cognitive control TMS effects and static and dynamic integration. Leave-one-out cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict TMS effects using static and dynamic integration. Scatterplot depicts the correlation between predicted and actual TMS impairments.

Figure 9—figure supplement 1.. Integration predicts transcranial…

Figure 9—figure supplement 1.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects with reduced smoothing.

Details…

Figure 9—figure supplement 1.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 9B.

Figure 9—figure supplement 2.. Integration predicts transcranial…

Figure 9—figure supplement 2.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for cognitive…

Figure 9—figure supplement 2.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for cognitive ability.
Details are the same as Figure 9 except that cognitive ability has been regressed out of static and dynamic integration measures. Top: standard (8 mm smoothing). Bottom: reduced (4 mm smoothing).

Figure 9—figure supplement 3.. Activations do not…

Figure 9—figure supplement 3.. Activations do not predict TMS effects.

Cognitive control and stimulus domain…

Figure 9—figure supplement 3.. Activations do not predict TMS effects.
Cognitive control and stimulus domain related activations were used as features to predict TMS effects in a similar manner to the use of integration measures to predict TMS effects. Principle components analysis was used to create factors associated with temporal control (temporal control contrast activations in FPl, MFG, and IPL), contextual control (contextual control contrast activations in VLPFC, cMFG, and midIPS), sensory-motor control (sensory-motor control contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL), and stimulus domain (stimulus domain contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL). Note that all contrasts are orthogonal to one another. Cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict TMS effects based upon the activation measures. Note in both cases, predicted and actual TMS effects were negatively correlated indicating that activation measures are not useful for predicting TMS effects. Left: standard (8 mm smoothing). Right: reduced (4 mm smoothing).

Figure 9—figure supplement 4.. Integration predicts transcranial…

Figure 9—figure supplement 4.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for both…

Figure 9—figure supplement 4.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for both cognitive ability and activations.
Details are the same as Figure 9 except that cognitive ability and activations have been regressed out of static and dynamic integration measures. Top: standard (8 mm smoothing). Bottom: reduced (4 mm smoothing).
All figures (26)
Similar articles
Cited by
References
    1. Amunts K, Zilles K. Architectonic mapping of the human brain beyond brodmann. Neuron. 2015;88:1086–1107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.001. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Andersson JL, Hutton C, Ashburner J, Turner R, Friston K. Modeling geometric deformations in EPI time series. NeuroImage. 2001;13:903–919. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0746. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8:170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Aron AR. The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. The Neuroscientist. 2007;13:214–228. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baddeley A. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology. 2012;63:1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422. - DOI - PubMed
Show all 102 references
Publication types
MeSH terms
Related information
LinkOut - more resources
[x]
Cite
Copy Download .nbib .nbib
Format: AMA APA MLA NLM
Figure 5—figure supplement 1.. Estimates of static…
Figure 5—figure supplement 1.. Estimates of static effective connectivity with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 5.
Figure 6.. Static network interactions.
Figure 6.. Static network interactions.
Top: Effective connectivity was averaged as a function of source>target network. Bars are colored as a function of the source network: TC – temporal control (red); CC – contextual control (green); SC – sensory-motor control (blue). ** denotes Bonferroni-corrected p

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.. Effect of task…

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.. Effect of task signals on effective connectivity parameters and source x…

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.. Effect of task signals on effective connectivity parameters and source x network interactions.
(A) Comparison of effective connectivity parameters with and without regressing out task signals. Bias (e.g. task-induced increases) would be apparent as a shift off of the diagonal (red line). No bias was observed. (B) Source x target network interactions after regressing out task signals. The same interaction was observed after regressing out task signals as was observed in the main text indicating that the interactions were not driven by task signals.

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.. Static network interactions…

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.. Static network interactions with reduced smoothing.

Details are identical to Figure…

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.. Static network interactions with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 6.

Figure 7.. Dynamic Interactions.

( A )…

Figure 7.. Dynamic Interactions.

( A ) Psychophysiological interactions (PPIs) for temporal control, contextual control,…

Figure 7.. Dynamic Interactions.
(A) Psychophysiological interactions (PPIs) for temporal control, contextual control, and stimulus domain contrasts. Solid lines indicate positive modulations of effective connectivity while dashed lines indicate negative modulations of effective connectivity. Arrows are colored by the network of the source node. Thickness of the arrows indicates the strength of modulations. Modulations are visualized at p<0.05 uncorrected. (B) Averaged within- and between-network modulations for the temporal control (red), contextual control (green), and stimulus domain (blue) contrasts. (C) Integration indices computed by contrasting between- minus within-network modulations for temporal control (TC), contextual control (CC), and stimulus domain (SD) contrasts.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. Sample averaged psychophysiological…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. Sample averaged psychophysiological interaction (PPI) parameter estimates.

TC – temporal control;…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. Sample averaged psychophysiological interaction (PPI) parameter estimates.
TC – temporal control; CC – contextual control; SC – sensory-motor control; SD – stimulus domain.

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.. Comparison of the…

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.. Comparison of the sensory-motor control contrast and stimulus domain contrasts.

Stimulus…

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.. Comparison of the sensory-motor control contrast and stimulus domain contrasts.
Stimulus domain is jointly determined by verbal > spatial and spatial > verbal.

Figure 7—figure supplement 3.. Dynamic interactions with…

Figure 7—figure supplement 3.. Dynamic interactions with reduced smoothing.

Details are identical to Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 3.. Dynamic interactions with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 7.

Figure 8.. Integration predicts higher level cognitive…

Figure 8.. Integration predicts higher level cognitive ability.

( A ) Static integration was computed…

Figure 8.. Integration predicts higher level cognitive ability.
(A) Static integration was computed by contrasting between minus within-network effective connectivity of the contextual control network while controlling for (regressing out) the same contrast of the temporal control network and sensory-motor control network and individual differences in head motion. (B) Dynamic integration was based upon the integration index of the contextual control PPI’s while controlling for (regressing out) the integration indices of the temporal control and stimulus domain PPI’s and individual differences in head motion. (C) Higher level cognitive ability was computed as the first principle component of a battery of cognitive tests measuring capacity. (D) Cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict cognitive capacity based upon static and dynamic integration. The scatterplot depicts correlations between the predicted cognitive ability and actual cognitive ability. (E) Correlations among static integration, dynamic integration, and cognitive ability.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.. Activations predict higher…

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.. Activations predict higher level cognitive ability.

Cognitive control and stimulus domain…

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.. Activations predict higher level cognitive ability.
Cognitive control and stimulus domain related activations were used as features to predict cognitive ability in a similar manner to the use of integration measures to predict cognitive ability. Principle components analysis was used to create factors associated with temporal control (temporal control contrast activations in FPl, MFG, and IPL), contextual control (contextual control contrast activations in VLPFC, cMFG, and midIPS), sensory-motor control (sensory-motor control contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL), and stimulus domain (stimulus domain contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL). Note that all contrasts are orthogonal to one another. Cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict cognitive ability based upon the activation measures.

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.. Integration does not…

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.. Integration does not predict higher-level cognitive ability with reduced smoothing.

Details…

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.. Integration does not predict higher-level cognitive ability with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 8D–E.

Figure 8—figure supplement 3.. With reduced smoothing,…

Figure 8—figure supplement 3.. With reduced smoothing, activation-based measures remain significantly predictive of higher-level cognitive…

Figure 8—figure supplement 3.. With reduced smoothing, activation-based measures remain significantly predictive of higher-level cognitive ability.

Figure 9.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation…

Figure 9.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects.

( A ) Previously reportedNee and…

Figure 9.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects.
(A) Previously reportedNee and D'Esposito, 2017 cognitive control impairments induced by continuous theta-burst TMS were combined using principle components analysis to derive a general susceptibility to PFC TMS. (B) Correlations between cognitive control TMS effects and static and dynamic integration. Leave-one-out cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict TMS effects using static and dynamic integration. Scatterplot depicts the correlation between predicted and actual TMS impairments.

Figure 9—figure supplement 1.. Integration predicts transcranial…

Figure 9—figure supplement 1.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects with reduced smoothing.

Details…

Figure 9—figure supplement 1.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 9B.

Figure 9—figure supplement 2.. Integration predicts transcranial…

Figure 9—figure supplement 2.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for cognitive…

Figure 9—figure supplement 2.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for cognitive ability.
Details are the same as Figure 9 except that cognitive ability has been regressed out of static and dynamic integration measures. Top: standard (8 mm smoothing). Bottom: reduced (4 mm smoothing).

Figure 9—figure supplement 3.. Activations do not…

Figure 9—figure supplement 3.. Activations do not predict TMS effects.

Cognitive control and stimulus domain…

Figure 9—figure supplement 3.. Activations do not predict TMS effects.
Cognitive control and stimulus domain related activations were used as features to predict TMS effects in a similar manner to the use of integration measures to predict TMS effects. Principle components analysis was used to create factors associated with temporal control (temporal control contrast activations in FPl, MFG, and IPL), contextual control (contextual control contrast activations in VLPFC, cMFG, and midIPS), sensory-motor control (sensory-motor control contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL), and stimulus domain (stimulus domain contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL). Note that all contrasts are orthogonal to one another. Cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict TMS effects based upon the activation measures. Note in both cases, predicted and actual TMS effects were negatively correlated indicating that activation measures are not useful for predicting TMS effects. Left: standard (8 mm smoothing). Right: reduced (4 mm smoothing).

Figure 9—figure supplement 4.. Integration predicts transcranial…

Figure 9—figure supplement 4.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for both…

Figure 9—figure supplement 4.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for both cognitive ability and activations.
Details are the same as Figure 9 except that cognitive ability and activations have been regressed out of static and dynamic integration measures. Top: standard (8 mm smoothing). Bottom: reduced (4 mm smoothing).
All figures (26)
Figure 6—figure supplement 1.. Effect of task…
Figure 6—figure supplement 1.. Effect of task signals on effective connectivity parameters and source x network interactions.
(A) Comparison of effective connectivity parameters with and without regressing out task signals. Bias (e.g. task-induced increases) would be apparent as a shift off of the diagonal (red line). No bias was observed. (B) Source x target network interactions after regressing out task signals. The same interaction was observed after regressing out task signals as was observed in the main text indicating that the interactions were not driven by task signals.
Figure 6—figure supplement 2.. Static network interactions…
Figure 6—figure supplement 2.. Static network interactions with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 6.
Figure 7.. Dynamic Interactions.
Figure 7.. Dynamic Interactions.
(A) Psychophysiological interactions (PPIs) for temporal control, contextual control, and stimulus domain contrasts. Solid lines indicate positive modulations of effective connectivity while dashed lines indicate negative modulations of effective connectivity. Arrows are colored by the network of the source node. Thickness of the arrows indicates the strength of modulations. Modulations are visualized at p<0.05 uncorrected. (B) Averaged within- and between-network modulations for the temporal control (red), contextual control (green), and stimulus domain (blue) contrasts. (C) Integration indices computed by contrasting between- minus within-network modulations for temporal control (TC), contextual control (CC), and stimulus domain (SD) contrasts.
Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. Sample averaged psychophysiological…
Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. Sample averaged psychophysiological interaction (PPI) parameter estimates.
TC – temporal control; CC – contextual control; SC – sensory-motor control; SD – stimulus domain.
Figure 7—figure supplement 2.. Comparison of the…
Figure 7—figure supplement 2.. Comparison of the sensory-motor control contrast and stimulus domain contrasts.
Stimulus domain is jointly determined by verbal > spatial and spatial > verbal.
Figure 7—figure supplement 3.. Dynamic interactions with…
Figure 7—figure supplement 3.. Dynamic interactions with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 7.
Figure 8.. Integration predicts higher level cognitive…
Figure 8.. Integration predicts higher level cognitive ability.
(A) Static integration was computed by contrasting between minus within-network effective connectivity of the contextual control network while controlling for (regressing out) the same contrast of the temporal control network and sensory-motor control network and individual differences in head motion. (B) Dynamic integration was based upon the integration index of the contextual control PPI’s while controlling for (regressing out) the integration indices of the temporal control and stimulus domain PPI’s and individual differences in head motion. (C) Higher level cognitive ability was computed as the first principle component of a battery of cognitive tests measuring capacity. (D) Cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict cognitive capacity based upon static and dynamic integration. The scatterplot depicts correlations between the predicted cognitive ability and actual cognitive ability. (E) Correlations among static integration, dynamic integration, and cognitive ability.
Figure 8—figure supplement 1.. Activations predict higher…
Figure 8—figure supplement 1.. Activations predict higher level cognitive ability.
Cognitive control and stimulus domain related activations were used as features to predict cognitive ability in a similar manner to the use of integration measures to predict cognitive ability. Principle components analysis was used to create factors associated with temporal control (temporal control contrast activations in FPl, MFG, and IPL), contextual control (contextual control contrast activations in VLPFC, cMFG, and midIPS), sensory-motor control (sensory-motor control contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL), and stimulus domain (stimulus domain contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL). Note that all contrasts are orthogonal to one another. Cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict cognitive ability based upon the activation measures.
Figure 8—figure supplement 2.. Integration does not…
Figure 8—figure supplement 2.. Integration does not predict higher-level cognitive ability with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 8D–E.
Figure 8—figure supplement 3.. With reduced smoothing,…
Figure 8—figure supplement 3.. With reduced smoothing, activation-based measures remain significantly predictive of higher-level cognitive ability.
Figure 9.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation…
Figure 9.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects.
(A) Previously reportedNee and D'Esposito, 2017 cognitive control impairments induced by continuous theta-burst TMS were combined using principle components analysis to derive a general susceptibility to PFC TMS. (B) Correlations between cognitive control TMS effects and static and dynamic integration. Leave-one-out cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict TMS effects using static and dynamic integration. Scatterplot depicts the correlation between predicted and actual TMS impairments.
Figure 9—figure supplement 1.. Integration predicts transcranial…
Figure 9—figure supplement 1.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects with reduced smoothing.
Details are identical to Figure 9B.
Figure 9—figure supplement 2.. Integration predicts transcranial…
Figure 9—figure supplement 2.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for cognitive ability.
Details are the same as Figure 9 except that cognitive ability has been regressed out of static and dynamic integration measures. Top: standard (8 mm smoothing). Bottom: reduced (4 mm smoothing).
Figure 9—figure supplement 3.. Activations do not…
Figure 9—figure supplement 3.. Activations do not predict TMS effects.
Cognitive control and stimulus domain related activations were used as features to predict TMS effects in a similar manner to the use of integration measures to predict TMS effects. Principle components analysis was used to create factors associated with temporal control (temporal control contrast activations in FPl, MFG, and IPL), contextual control (contextual control contrast activations in VLPFC, cMFG, and midIPS), sensory-motor control (sensory-motor control contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL), and stimulus domain (stimulus domain contrast activations in IFJ, SFS, antIPS, and SPL). Note that all contrasts are orthogonal to one another. Cross-validated ridge regression was used to predict TMS effects based upon the activation measures. Note in both cases, predicted and actual TMS effects were negatively correlated indicating that activation measures are not useful for predicting TMS effects. Left: standard (8 mm smoothing). Right: reduced (4 mm smoothing).
Figure 9—figure supplement 4.. Integration predicts transcranial…
Figure 9—figure supplement 4.. Integration predicts transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) effects after controlling for both cognitive ability and activations.
Details are the same as Figure 9 except that cognitive ability and activations have been regressed out of static and dynamic integration measures. Top: standard (8 mm smoothing). Bottom: reduced (4 mm smoothing).

References

    1. Amunts K, Zilles K. Architectonic mapping of the human brain beyond brodmann. Neuron. 2015;88:1086–1107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.001.
    1. Andersson JL, Hutton C, Ashburner J, Turner R, Friston K. Modeling geometric deformations in EPI time series. NeuroImage. 2001;13:903–919. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0746.
    1. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8:170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010.
    1. Aron AR. The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. The Neuroscientist. 2007;13:214–228. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288.
    1. Baddeley A. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology. 2012;63:1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422.
    1. Badre D. Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2008;12:193–200. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004.
    1. Badre D, D'Esposito M. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2007;19:2082–2099. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2082.
    1. Badre D, D'Esposito M. Is the rostro-caudal Axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:659–669. doi: 10.1038/nrn2667.
    1. Badre D, Frank MJ. Mechanisms of hierarchical reinforcement learning in cortico-striatal circuits 2: evidence from fMRI. Cerebral Cortex. 2012;22:527–536. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr117.
    1. Badre D, Nee DE. Frontal cortex and the hierarchical control of behavior. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2018;22:170–188. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.005.
    1. Bahlmann J, Blumenfeld RS, D'Esposito M. The Rostro-Caudal Axis of frontal cortex is sensitive to the domain of stimulus information. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;25:bht419. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht419.
    1. Bahlmann J, Aarts E, D'Esposito M. Influence of motivation on control hierarchy in the human frontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:3207–3217. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2389-14.2015.
    1. Blumberger DM, Vila-Rodriguez F, Thorpe KE, Feffer K, Noda Y, Giacobbe P, Knyahnytska Y, Kennedy SH, Lam RW, Daskalakis ZJ, Downar J. Effectiveness of theta burst versus high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with depression (THREE-D): a randomised non-inferiority trial. The Lancet. 2018;391:1683–1692. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30295-2.
    1. Braga RM, Buckner RL. Parallel interdigitated distributed networks within the individual estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Neuron. 2017;95:457–471. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.038.
    1. Braun U, Schäfer A, Walter H, Erk S, Romanczuk-Seiferth N, Haddad L, Schweiger JI, Grimm O, Heinz A, Tost H, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Bassett DS. Dynamic reconfiguration of frontal brain networks during executive cognition in humans. PNAS. 2015;112:11678–11683. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422487112.
    1. Brodmann K. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre Der Grosshirnrinde in Ihren Prinzipien Dargestellt Auf Grund Des Zellenbaues. Barth; 1909.
    1. Buckner RL, DiNicola LM. The brain's default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2019;20:593–608. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7.
    1. Buckner RL, Krienen FM. The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17:648–665. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.017.
    1. Charron S, Koechlin E. Divided representation of concurrent goals in the human frontal lobes. Science. 2010;328:360–363. doi: 10.1126/science.1183614.
    1. Chaudhuri R, Knoblauch K, Gariel MA, Kennedy H, Wang XJ. A Large-Scale circuit mechanism for hierarchical dynamical processing in the primate cortex. Neuron. 2015;88:419–431. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.008.
    1. Choi EY, Drayna GK, Badre D. Evidence for a functional hierarchy of association networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2018;30:722–736. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01229.
    1. Cocchi L, Zalesky A, Fornito A, Mattingley JB. Dynamic cooperation and competition between brain systems during cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17:493–501. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.006.
    1. Cocchi L, Halford GS, Zalesky A, Harding IH, Ramm BJ, Cutmore T, Shum DH, Mattingley JB. Complexity in relational processing predicts changes in functional brain network dynamics. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24:2283–2296. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht075.
    1. Cohen JR, Gallen CL, Jacobs EG, Lee TG, D'Esposito M. Quantifying the reconfiguration of intrinsic networks during working memory. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e106636. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106636.
    1. Cohen JR, D'Esposito M. The segregation and integration of distinct brain networks and their relationship to cognition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:12083–12094. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2965-15.2016.
    1. Cole MW, Yarkoni T, Repovs G, Anticevic A, Braver TS. Global connectivity of prefrontal cortex predicts cognitive control and intelligence. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:8988–8999. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0536-12.2012.
    1. Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD, Repovs G, Anticevic A, Braver TS. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470.
    1. Cole MW, Bassett DS, Power JD, Braver TS, Petersen SE. Intrinsic and Task-Evoked network architectures of the human brain. Neuron. 2014;83:238–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.014.
    1. Cole MW, Ito T, Bassett DS, Schultz DH. Activity flow over resting-state networks shapes cognitive task activations. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:1718–1726. doi: 10.1038/nn.4406.
    1. Cole MW, Ito T, Schultz D, Mill R, Chen R, Cocuzza C. Task activations produce spurious but systematic inflation of task functional connectivity estimates. NeuroImage. 2019;189:1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.054.
    1. Cole MW, Schneider W. The cognitive control network: integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. NeuroImage. 2007;37:343–360. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071.
    1. Dixon ML, De La Vega A, Mills C, Andrews-Hanna J, Spreng RN, Cole MW, Christoff K. Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks. PNAS. 2018;115:E1598–E1607. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1715766115.
    1. Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Cohen AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2008;12:99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001.
    1. Dubois J, Galdi P, Paul LK, Adolphs R. A distributed brain network predicts general intelligence from resting-state human neuroimaging data. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2018;373:20170284. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0284.
    1. Duncan J. The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010;14:172–179. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004.
    1. Duncan J. The structure of cognition: attentional episodes in mind and brain. Neuron. 2013;80:35–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.015.
    1. Duncan J, Assem M, Shashidhara S. Integrated intelligence from distributed brain activity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2020;24:838–852. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.06.012.
    1. Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends in Neurosciences. 2000;23:475–483. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01633-7.
    1. D’Mello AM, Gabrieli JDE, Nee DE. Evidence for hierarchical cognitive control in the human cerebellum. Current Biology. 2020;30:1881–1892. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.03.028.
    1. Egner T. The Wiley Handbook of Cognitive Control. John Wiley & Sons; 2017.
    1. Fedorenko E, Duncan J, Kanwisher N. Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. PNAS. 2013;110:16616–16621. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1315235110.
    1. Feredoes E, Heinen K, Weiskopf N, Ruff C, Driver J. Causal evidence for frontal involvement in memory target maintenance by posterior brain Areas during distracter interference of visual working memory. PNAS. 2011;108:17510–17515. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1106439108.
    1. Frank MJ, Loughry B, O'Reilly RC. Interactions between frontal cortex and basal ganglia in working memory: a computational model. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2001;1:137–160. doi: 10.3758/CABN.1.2.137.
    1. Frank MJ, Badre D. Mechanisms of hierarchical reinforcement learning in corticostriatal circuits 1: computational analysis. Cerebral Cortex. 2012;22:509–526. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr114.
    1. Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. NeuroImage. 1997;6:218–229. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0291.
    1. Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W. Dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage. 2003;19:1273–1302. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00202-7.
    1. Friston KJ, Kahan J, Biswal B, Razi A. A DCM for resting state fMRI. NeuroImage. 2014;94:396–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.009.
    1. Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex--an update: time is of the essence. Neuron. 2001;30:319–333. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00285-9.
    1. Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Gilmore AW, Newbold DJ, Greene DJ, Berg JJ, Ortega M, Hoyt-Drazen C, Gratton C, Sun H, Hampton JM, Coalson RS, Nguyen AL, McDermott KB, Shimony JS, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE, Nelson SM, Dosenbach NUF. Precision functional mapping of individual human brains. Neuron. 2017;95:791–807. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.011.
    1. Gratton C, Nomura EM, Pérez F, D'Esposito M. Focal brain lesions to critical locations cause widespread disruption of the modular organization of the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;24:1275–1285. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00222.
    1. Gratton C, Laumann TO, Gordon EM, Adeyemo B, Petersen SE. Evidence for two independent factors that modify brain networks to meet task goals. Cell Reports. 2016;17:1276–1288. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.10.002.
    1. Gu S, Pasqualetti F, Cieslak M, Telesford QK, Yu AB, Kahn AE, Medaglia JD, Vettel JM, Miller MB, Grafton ST, Bassett DS. Controllability of structural brain networks. Nature Communications. 2015;6:1–10. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9414.
    1. Hasson U, Chen J, Honey CJ. Hierarchical process memory: memory as an integral component of information processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2015;19:304–313. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.006.
    1. Huang YZ, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC. Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron. 2005;45:201–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.033.
    1. Huntenburg JM, Bazin PL, Margulies DS. Large-Scale gradients in human cortical organization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2018;22:21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.002.
    1. Jeon H-A, Anwander A, Friederici AD. Functional network mirrored in the prefrontal cortex, caudate nucleus, and thalamus: high-resolution functional imaging and structural connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:9202–9212. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0228-14.2014.
    1. Jeon HA, Friederici AD. Two principles of organization in the prefrontal cortex are cognitive hierarchy and degree of automaticity. Nature Communications. 2013;4:2041. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3041.
    1. Koechlin E, Basso G, Pietrini P, Panzer S, Grafman J. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature. 1999;399:148–151. doi: 10.1038/20178.
    1. Koechlin E, Ody C, Kouneiher F. The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science. 2003;302:1181–1185. doi: 10.1126/science.1088545.
    1. Koechlin E, Summerfield C. An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11:229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.005.
    1. Kragel PA, Han X, Kraynak TE, Gianaros PJ, Wager TD. fMRI can be highly reliable, but it depends on what you measure. PsyArXiv. 2020 doi: 10.31234/.
    1. Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PS, Baker CI. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:535–540. doi: 10.1038/nn.2303.
    1. Krienen FM, Yeo BTT, Buckner RL. Reconfigurable task-dependent functional coupling modes cluster around a core functional architecture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2014;369:20130526. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0526.
    1. Logan GD, Gordon RD. Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review. 2001;108:393–434. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.393.
    1. Lund TE, Nørgaard MD, Rostrup E, Rowe JB, Paulson OB. Motion or activity: their role in Intra- and inter-subject variation in fMRI. NeuroImage. 2005;26:960–964. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.021.
    1. Luria AR. The directive function of speech in development and dissolution. Word. 1959;15:341–352. doi: 10.1080/00437956.1959.11659703.
    1. Marek S, Hwang K, Foran W, Hallquist MN, Luna B. The contribution of network organization and integration to the development of cognitive control. PLOS Biology. 2015;13:e1002328. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002328.
    1. Marek S. Towards reproducible Brain-Wide association studies. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.08.21.257758.
    1. Margulies DS, Ghosh SS, Goulas A, Falkiewicz M, Huntenburg JM, Langs G, Bezgin G, Eickhoff SB, Castellanos FX, Petrides M, Jefferies E, Smallwood J. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. PNAS. 2016;113:12574–12579. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608282113.
    1. Mesulam M. From sensation to cognition. Brain. 1998;121:1013–1052. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013.
    1. Miller BT, Vytlacil J, Fegen D, Pradhan S, D'Esposito M. The prefrontal cortex modulates category selectivity in human extrastriate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23:1–10. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21516.
    1. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167.
    1. Miller BT, D'Esposito M. Searching for "the top" in top-down control. Neuron. 2005;48:535–538. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.11.002.
    1. Murphy AC, Bertolero MA, Papadopoulos L, Lydon-Staley DM, Bassett DS. Multimodal network dynamics underpinning working memory. Nature Communications. 2020;11:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-15541-0.
    1. Murray JD, Bernacchia A, Freedman DJ, Romo R, Wallis JD, Cai X, Padoa-Schioppa C, Pasternak T, Seo H, Lee D, Wang XJ. A hierarchy of intrinsic timescales across primate cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1661–1663. doi: 10.1038/nn.3862.
    1. Nee DE, Jahn A, Brown JW. Prefrontal cortex organization: dissociating effects of temporal abstraction, relational abstraction, and integration with FMRI. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24:2377–2387. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht091.
    1. Nee DE. fMRI replicability depends upon sufficient individual-level data. Communications Biology. 2019;2:1–4. doi: 10.1038/s42003-019-0378-6.
    1. Nee DE, Brown JW. Rostral-caudal gradients of abstraction revealed by multi-variate pattern analysis of working memory. NeuroImage. 2012;63:1285–1294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.034.
    1. Nee DE, Brown JW. Dissociable frontal-striatal and frontal-parietal networks involved in updating hierarchical contexts in working memory. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23:2146–2158. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs194.
    1. Nee DE, D'Esposito M. The hierarchical organization of the lateral prefrontal cortex. eLife. 2016;5:e12112. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12112.
    1. Nee DE, D'Esposito M. Causal evidence for lateral prefrontal cortex dynamics supporting cognitive control. eLife. 2017;6:e28040. doi: 10.7554/eLife.28040.
    1. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018.
    1. Power JD, Petersen SE. Control-related systems in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2013;23:223–228. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.009.
    1. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. PNAS. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676.
    1. Razi A, Kahan J, Rees G, Friston KJ. Construct validation of a DCM for resting state fMRI. NeuroImage. 2015;106:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.027.
    1. Razi A, Seghier ML, Zhou Y, McColgan P, Zeidman P, Park HJ, Sporns O, Rees G, Friston KJ. Large-scale DCMs for resting-state fMRI. Network Neuroscience. 2017;1:222–241. doi: 10.1162/NETN_a_00015.
    1. Salehi M, Karbasi A, Barron DS, Scheinost D, Constable RT. Individualized functional networks reconfigure with cognitive state. NeuroImage. 2020a;206:116233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116233.
    1. Salehi M, Greene AS, Karbasi A, Shen X, Scheinost D, Constable RT. There is no single functional atlas even for a single individual: functional parcel definitions change with task. NeuroImage. 2020b;208:116366. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116366.
    1. Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Gerraty RT, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Calkins ME, Eickhoff SB, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE, Wolf DH. An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. NeuroImage. 2013;64:240–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052.
    1. Shine JM, Breakspear M, Bell PT, Ehgoetz Martens KA, Shine R, Koyejo O, Sporns O, Poldrack RA. Human cognition involves the dynamic integration of neural activity and neuromodulatory systems. Nature Neuroscience. 2019a;22:289–296. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0312-0.
    1. Shine JM. Neuromodulatory influences on integration and segregation in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2019b;23:572–583. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.04.002.
    1. Sormaz M, Murphy C, Wang HT, Hymers M, Karapanagiotidis T, Poerio G, Margulies DS, Jefferies E, Smallwood J. Default mode network can support the level of detail in experience during active task states. PNAS. 2018;115:9318–9323. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721259115.
    1. Sporns O. Networks of the Brain. MIT press; 2010.
    1. Stevens AA, Tappon SC, Garg A, Fair DA. Functional brain network modularity captures inter- and intra-individual variation in working memory capacity. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e30468. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030468.
    1. Tan PK, Tang C, Herikstad R, Pillay A, Libedinsky C. Distinct lateral prefrontal regions are organized in an anterior-posterior functional gradient. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.12.16.423034.
    1. Turner ML, Engle RW. Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language. 1989;28:127–154. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(89)90040-5.
    1. Vincent JL, Kahn I, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME, Buckner RL. Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;100:3328–3342. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008.
    1. Wang XJ. Macroscopic gradients of synaptic excitation and inhibition in the neocortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2020;21:169–178. doi: 10.1038/s41583-020-0262-x.
    1. Warren DE, Power JD, Bruss J, Denburg NL, Waldron EJ, Sun H, Petersen SE, Tranel D. Network measures predict neuropsychological outcome after brain injury. PNAS. 2014;111:14247–14252. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1322173111.
    1. Wig GS, Laumann TO, Cohen AL, Power JD, Nelson SM, Glasser MF, Miezin FM, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Parcellating an individual subject's cortical and subcortical brain structures using snowball sampling of resting-state correlations. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24:2036–2054. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht056.
    1. Yarkoni T, Braver TS. Cognitive neuroscience approaches to individual differences in working memory and executive control: Conceptual and methodological issues. In: Gruszka A, Matthews G, Szymura B, editors. Handbook of Individual Differences in Cognition: Attention, Memory and Executive Control. Springer; 2010. pp. 87–107.
    1. Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Zöllei L, Polimeni JR, Fischl B, Liu H, Buckner RL. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011.

Source: PubMed

3
S'abonner