Computerized Cognitive Training for Amelioration of Cognitive Late Effects Among Childhood Cancer Survivors: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Heather M Conklin, Robert J Ogg, Jason M Ashford, Matthew A Scoggins, Ping Zou, Kellie N Clark, Karen Martin-Elbahesh, Kristina K Hardy, Thomas E Merchant, Sima Jeha, Lu Huang, Hui Zhang, Heather M Conklin, Robert J Ogg, Jason M Ashford, Matthew A Scoggins, Ping Zou, Kellie N Clark, Karen Martin-Elbahesh, Kristina K Hardy, Thomas E Merchant, Sima Jeha, Lu Huang, Hui Zhang

Abstract

Purpose: Children receiving CNS-directed therapy for cancer are at risk for cognitive problems, with few available empirically supported interventions. Cognitive problems indicate neurodevelopmental disruption that may be modifiable with intervention. This study evaluated short-term efficacy of a computerized cognitive training program and neural correlates of cognitive change.

Patient and methods: A total of 68 survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia (ALL) or brain tumor (BT) with identified cognitive deficits were randomly assigned to computerized cognitive intervention (male, n = 18; female, n = 16; ALL, n = 23; BT, n = 11; mean age ± standard deviation, 12.21 ± 2.47 years) or waitlist (male, n = 18; female, n = 16; ALL, n = 24; BT, n = 10; median age ± standard deviation, 11.82 ± 2.42 years). Intervention participants were asked to complete 25 training sessions at home with weekly, telephone-based coaching. Cognitive assessments and functional magnetic resonance imaging scans (intervention group) were completed pre- and postintervention, with immediate change in spatial span backward as the primary outcome.

Results: Survivors completing the intervention (n = 30; 88%) demonstrated greater improvement than controls on measures of working memory (mean ± SEM; eg, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children [fourth edition; WISC-IV] spatial span backward, 3.13 ± 0.58 v 0.75 ± 0.43; P = .002; effect size [ES], 0.84), attention (eg, WISC-IV spatial span forward, 3.30 ± 0.71 v 1.25 ± 0.39; P = .01; ES, 0.65), and processing speed (eg, Conners' Continuous Performance Test hit reaction time, -2.10 ± 1.47 v 2.54 ± 1.25; P = .02; ES, .61) and showed greater reductions in reported executive dysfunction (eg, Conners' Parent Rating Scale III, -6.73 ± 1.51 v 0.41 ± 1.53; P = .002; ES, 0.84). Functional magnetic resonance imaging revealed significant pre- to post-training reduction in activation of left lateral prefrontal and bilateral medial frontal areas.

Conclusion: Study findings show computerized cognitive training is feasible and efficacious for childhood cancer survivors, with evidence for training-related neuroplasticity.

Conflict of interest statement

Authors' disclosures of potential conflicts of interest are found in the article online at www.jco.org. Author contributions are found at the end of this article.

© 2015 by American Society of Clinical Oncology.

Figures

Fig 1.
Fig 1.
CONSORT diagram. WM, working memory. (*) Completed visit one functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI; n = 31); one participant supplied partial preintervention fMRI data because of fatigue. (†) Completed visit two fMRI (n = 28).
Fig 2.
Fig 2.
Pre- to post-training cognitive scores. (A) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (fourth edition; WISC-IV) spatial span backward; (B) WISC-IV digit span backward; (C) Conners' Parent Rating Scale III (CPRS-3) executive function; (D) CPRS-3 inattention. (*) P < .05 group × time interaction on repeated-measures analysis of variance.
Fig 3.
Fig 3.
Functional neuroimaging. (A) Preintervention activation during Olesen working memory (WM) task (contrast: WM trials > control trials in random-effects group analysis; t test P < .05 with family wise error [FWE] correction). (B) Neural correlates of WM ability and training. Green denotes areas of decreased activation after WM intervention (contrast: WM trials > control trials in random-effects group analysis; paired t test P < .05 with FWE correction). Yellow denotes areas where activity was positively associated with Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) WM index (regression: WM trials > control trials v WM index in random-effects analysis; P < .05 with FWE correction). Purple denotes area where low preintervention activity (contrast: WM trials > control trials) predicted good response to intervention (median split on spatial span backward change; P < .05 with cluster correction). (C) Activity (fixed-effects parameter estimate for each participant) of green left middle frontal gyrus cluster in (B) showing significant groupwise decrease in activation after intervention. (D) Activity of yellow right postcentral gyrus cluster in (B) versus WM index scores. Blue symbols identify patients who responded to intervention (median split on WM index change). SS, standard score (mean = 100, standard deviation = 15). Gold symbols identify nonresponders. (E) Change in spatial span backward score versus preintervention activity for purple right frontal cluster in (B). ScS, scaled score (mean = 10, standard deviation = 3). Dots are colored as in (D).
Fig A1.
Fig A1.
Block-design spatial working memory task designed by Olesen et al. ISI, interstimulus interval.
Fig A2.
Fig A2.
Working memory task (designed by Olesen et al) performance. (A) Trial time versus trial type; (B) accuracy versus load; (C) high-load performance (order).

References

    1. Moleski M. Neuropsychological, neuroanatomical, and neurophysiological consequences of CNS chemotherapy for acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000;15:603–630.
    1. Moore BD., 3rd Neurocognitive outcomes in survivors of childhood cancer. J Pediatr Psychol. 2005;30:51–63.
    1. Mulhern RK, Butler RW. Neurocognitive sequelae of childhood cancers and their treatment. Pediatr Rehabil. 2004;7:1–14.
    1. Ris MD, Noll RB. Long-term neurobehavioral outcome in pediatric brain-tumor patients: Review and methodological critique. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1994;16:21–42.
    1. Crom DB, Lensing SY, Rai SN, et al. Marriage, employment, and health insurance in adult survivors of childhood cancer. J Cancer Surviv. 2007;1:237–245.
    1. Haupt R, Fears TR, Robinson LL, et al. Educational attainment in long-term survivors of acute lymphoblastic leukemia. JAMA. 1994;272:1427–1432.
    1. Mitby PA, Robison LL, Whitton JA, et al. Utilization of special education services and educational attainment among long-term survivors of childhood cancer: A report from the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study. Cancer. 2003;97:1115–1126.
    1. Mostow EN, Byrne J, Connelly RR, et al. Quality of life in long-term survivors of CNS tumors of childhood and adolescence. J Clin Oncol. 1991;9:592–599.
    1. Conklin HM, Ashford JM, Howarth RA, et al. Working memory performance among childhood brain tumor survivors. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:996–1005.
    1. Mabbott DJ, Penkman L, Witol A, et al. Core neurocognitive functions in children treated for posterior fossa tumors. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:159–168.
    1. Reeves CB, Palmer SL, Reddick WE, et al. Attention and memory function among pediatric patients with medulloblastoma. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31:272–280.
    1. Schatz J, Kramer JH, Ablin A, et al. Processing speed, working memory, and IQ: A developmental model of cognitive deficits following cranial radiation therapy. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:189–200.
    1. Waber DP, Pomeroy SL, Chiverton AM, et al. Everyday cognitive function after craniopharyngioma in childhood. Pediatr Neurol. 2006;34:13–19.
    1. Carey ME, Haut MW, Reminger SL, et al. Reduced frontal white matter volume in long-term childhood leukemia survivors: A voxel-based morphometry study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2008;29:792–797.
    1. Mulhern RK, White HA, Glass JO, et al. Attentional functioning and white matter integrity among survivors of malignant brain tumors of childhood. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:180–189.
    1. Qiu D, Kwong DL, Chan GC, et al. Diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging finding of discrepant fractional anisotropy between the frontal and parietal lobes after whole-brain irradiation in childhood medulloblastoma survivors: Reflection of regional white matter radiosensitivity? Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2007;69:846–851.
    1. Reddick WE, Taghipour DJ, Glass JO, et al. Prognostic factors that increase the risk for reduced white matter volumes and deficits in attention and learning for survivors of childhood cancers. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2014;61:1074–1079.
    1. Law N, Bouffet E, Laughlin S, et al. Cerebello-thalamo-cerebral connections in pediatric brain tumor patients: Impact on working memory. Neurimage. 2011;56:2238–2248.
    1. Jemal A, Siegel R, Ward E, et al. Cancer statistics. CA Cancer J Clin. 2008;58:71–96.
    1. Pui CH, Campana D, Pei D, et al. Treating childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia without cranial irradiation. N Engl J Med. 2009;360:2730–2741.
    1. Conklin HM, Reddick WE, Ashford J, et al. Long-term efficacy of methylphenidate in enhancing attention regulation, social skills, and academic abilities of childhood cancer survivors. J Clin Oncol. 2010;28:4465–4472.
    1. Mulhern RK, Khan RB, Kaplan S, et al. Short-term efficacy of methylphenidate: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial among survivors of childhood cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22:4795–4803.
    1. Butler RW, Copeland DR, Fairclough DL, et al. A multicenter, randomized clinical trial of cognitive remediation program for childhood survivors of pediatric malignancy. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76:367–378.
    1. Patel SK, Katz ER, Richardson R, et al. Cognitive and problem solving training in children with cancer: A pilot project. J Pediatr Hematol Oncol. 2009;31:670–677.
    1. Moore IM, Hockenberry MJ, Anhault C, et al. Mathematics intervention for prevention of neurocognitive deficits in childhood leukemia. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2012;59:278–284.
    1. Conklin HM, Khan RB, Reddick WE, et al. Acute neurocognitive response to methylphenidate among survivors of childhood cancer: A randomized, double-blind, cross-over trial. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32:1127–1139.
    1. Conklin HM, Lawford J, Jasper BW, et al. Side effects of methylphenidate in survivors of childhood cancer: A randomized double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2009;124:226–233.
    1. Conklin HM, Helton S, Ashford J, et al. Predicting methylphenidate response in long-term survivors of childhood cancer: A randomized, double-blinded, placebo-controlled, cross-over trial. J Pediatr Psychol. 2010;35:144–155.
    1. Jasper BW, Conklin HM, Lawford J, et al. Growth effects of methylphenidate among childhood cancer survivors: A 12-month case-matched open-label study. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2009;52:39–43.
    1. Holmes J, Gathercole SE, Dunning DL. Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Dev Sci. 2009;12:F9–F15.
    1. Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen P, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:177–186.
    1. Westerberg H, Jacobaeus H, Hirvikoski T, et al. Computerized working memory training after stroke: A pilot study. Brain Inj. 2007;21:21–29.
    1. Grunewaldt KH, Løhaugen GC, Austeng D. Working memory training improves cognitive function in VLBW preschoolers. Pediatrics. 2013;131:747–754.
    1. Hardy KK, Willard VW, Allen TM. Working memory training in survivors of pediatric cancer: A randomized pilot study. Psychooncology. 2013;22:1856–1865.
    1. Cox LE, Ashford JM, Clark KN, et al. Feasibility and acceptability of a remotely-administered computerized intervention to address cognitive late effects among childhood cancer survivors. Neuro Oncol Pract. 2015;2:70–77.
    1. Olesen PJ, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:75–79.
    1. Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Changes in cortical activity after training of working memory: A single-subject analysis. Physiol Behav. 2007;92:186–192.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (ed 4, integrated) San Antonio, TX: Pearson Corporation; 2004.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 1999.
    1. Zelen M. The randomization and stratification of patients to clinical trials. J Chronic Dis. 1974;27:365–375.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (ed 3) San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1991.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (ed 3) San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 1999.
    1. Conners CK. Conners' Continuous Performance Test II. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Corporation; 2004.
    1. Woodcock RW, McGrew KS, Mather N. Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing; 2001.
    1. Conners CK. Conners' Rating Scales (ed 3) Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems; 2008.
    1. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, et al. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000.
    1. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analyses for the Behavioral Sciences (ed 2) Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
    1. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, et al. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp. 1995;2:189–210.
    1. Nee DE, Brown JW, Askren MK, et al. A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:264–282.
    1. Kesler SR, Lacayo NJ, Jo B. A pilot study of an online cognitive rehabilitation program for executive function skills in children with cancer-related brain injury. Brain Inj. 2011;25:101–112.
    1. Palmer SL, Armstrong C, Onar-Thomas A, et al. Processing speed, attention, and working memory after treatment for medulloblastoma: An international, prospective, and longitudinal study. J Clin Oncol. 2013;31:3494–3500.
    1. Barnett R, Maruff P, Vance A, et al. Abnormal executive function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The effect of stimulant medication and age on spatial working memory. Psychol Med. 2001;31:1107–1115.
    1. Bedard AC, Martinussen R, Ickowicz A, et al. Methylphenidate improves visual-spatial memory in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43:260–268.
    1. Kempton S, Vance A, Maruff P, et al. Executive function and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Stimulant medication and better executive function performance in children. Psychol Med. 1999;29:527–538.
    1. Patel R, Spreng RN, Turner GR. Functional brain changes following cognitive and motor skills training: A quantitative meta-analysis. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27:187–199.
    1. Kelly C, Foxe JJ, Garavan H. Patterns of normal human brain plasticity after practice and their implications for neurorehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2006;87(suppl):S20–S29.
    1. Garavan H, Kelley D, Rosen A, et al. Practice-related functional activation changes in a working memory task. Microsc Res Tech. 2000;51:54–63.
    1. Jansma JM, Ramsey NF, Slagter HA, et al. Functional anatomical correlates of controlled and automatic processing. J Cogn Neurosci. 2001;13:730–743.
    1. Sayala S, Sala JB, Courtney SM. Increased neural efficiency with repeated performance of a working memory task is information-type dependent. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:609–617.
    1. Hempel A, Giesel FL, Caraballo NM, et al. Plasticity of cortical activation related to working memory during training. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:745–747.
    1. Poldrack RA. Is “efficiency” a useful concept in cognitive neuroscience? Dev Cog Neurosci. 2015;11:12–17.
    1. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333:959–963.
    1. Klingberg T. The Overflowing Brain: Information Overload and the Limits of Working Memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2008.
    1. Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Training of working memory in children with ADHD. J Clin Exper Neuropsych. 2002;24:781–791.
    1. Dunning DL, Holmes J, Gathercole SE. Does working memory training lead to generalized improvements in children with low working memory? A randomized controlled trial. Dev Sci. 2013;16:915–925.
    1. Green CT, Long DL, Green D, et al. Will working memory training generalize to improve off-task behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9:639–648.
    1. Chacko A, Feirsen N, Bedard A, et al. Cogmed working memory training for youth with ADHD: A closer examination of efficacy utilizing evidence-based criteria. J Clin Child Adol Psychol. 2013;42:769–783.
    1. Thorell LB, Lindqvist S, Bergman Nutley S, et al. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev Sci. 2009;12:106–133.
    1. Dahlin KI. Effects of working memory training on reading in children with special needs. Read Writ. 2011;24:479–491.

Source: PubMed

3
S'abonner