The effect of changes in cerebral blood flow on cognitive function during exercise

Shigehiko Ogoh, Hayato Tsukamoto, Ai Hirasawa, Hiroshi Hasegawa, Norikazu Hirose, Takeshi Hashimoto, Shigehiko Ogoh, Hayato Tsukamoto, Ai Hirasawa, Hiroshi Hasegawa, Norikazu Hirose, Takeshi Hashimoto

Abstract

No studies have identified the direct effect of changes in cerebral blood flow (CBF) on cognitive function at rest and during exercise. In this study, we manipulated CBF using hypercapnic gas to examine whether an increase in CBF improves cognitive function during prolonged exercise. The speed and the accuracy of cognitive function were assessed using the Stroop color-word test. After the Stroop test at rest, the subjects began exercising on a cycling ergometer in which the workload was increased by 0.5 kilopond every minute until a target heart rate of 140 beats/min was achieved. Then, the subjects continued to cycle at a constant rate for 50 min. At four time points during the exercise (0, 10, 20, 50 min), the subjects performed a Stroop test with and without hypercapnic respiratory gas (2.0% CO2), with a random order of the exposures in the two tests. Despite a decrease in the mean blood flow velocity in the middle cerebral artery (MCA Vmean), the reaction time for the Stroop test gradually decreased during the prolonged exercise without any loss of performance accuracy. In addition, the hypercapnia-induced increase in MCA Vmean produced neither changes in the reaction time nor error in the Stroop test during exercise. These findings suggest that the changes in CBF are unlikely to affect cognitive function during prolonged exercise. Thus, we conclude that improved cognitive function may be due to cerebral neural activation associated with exercise rather than global cerebral circulatory condition.

Keywords: arterial blood pressure; brain; cognition; humans; hypercapnia.

© 2014 The Authors. Physiological Reports published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. on behalf of the American Physiological Society and The Physiological Society.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
(A) Experimental protocol. (B) An example of randomized incongruent words and colors‐recognition tests. The example indicates that the word is blue (underlined) while it is printed in red, and then the subjects have to press “blue” key and not “red” key.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Cognitive function improved during prolonged exercise despite decreases in CBF. (A) MCA mean blood flow velocity (MCA Vmean), (B) reaction time, (C) performance accuracy at rest, and during prolonged exercise. The values shown represent the mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 between the groups.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Hypercapnia‐induced increase in CBF did not affect cognitive function at rest as well as any time point during the prolonged exercise. (A) Changes in MCA mean blood flow velocity (ΔMCA Vmean) in response to hypercapnic stimulation at rest and during exercise. Hypercapnic stimulation significantly increased the MCA Vmean at each time point (P < 0.01). Changes in (B) reaction time (Δreaction time), (C) performance accuracy (Δperformance accuracy) in response to hypercapnic stimulation at rest and during exercise. Hypercapnic stimulation did not change reaction time and performance accuracy. The values shown represent the mean ± SEM.

References

    1. Ainslie P. N., Cotter J. D., George K. P., Lucas S., Murrell C., Shave R. 2008. Elevation in cerebral blood flow velocity with aerobic fitness throughout healthy human ageing. J. Physiol.; 586:4005-4010.
    1. Ando S., Hatamoto Y., Sudo M., Kiyonaga A., Tanaka H., Higaki Y. 2013. The effects of exercise under hypoxia on cognitive function. PLoS ONE; 8:e63630.
    1. Bertsch K., Hagemann D., Hermes M., Walter C., Khan R., Naumann E. 2009. Resting cerebral blood flow, attention, and aging. Brain Res.; 1267:77-88.
    1. Brisswalter J., Collardeau M., Rene A. 2002. Effects of acute physical exercise characteristics on cognitive performance. Sports Med.; 32:555-566.
    1. Byun K., Hyodo K., Suwabe K., Ochi G., Sakairi Y., Kato M. 2014. Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal‐related prefrontal activations: an fNIRS study. Neuroimage; 98:336-345.
    1. Colcombe S., Kramer A. F. 2003. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta‐analytic study. Psychol. Sci.; 14:125-130.
    1. Davranche K., McMorris T. 2009. Specific effects of acute moderate exercise on cognitive control. Brain Cogn.; 69:565-570.
    1. Dietrich A., Sparling P. B. 2004. Endurance exercise selectively impairs prefrontal‐dependent cognition. Brain Cogn.; 55:516-524.
    1. Endo K., Matsukawa K., Liang N., Nakatsuka C., Tsuchimochi H., Okamura H. 2013. Dynamic exercise improves cognitive function in association with increased prefrontal oxygenation. J. Physiol. Sci.; 63:287-298.
    1. Etnier J. L., Chang Y. K. 2009. The effect of physical activity on executive function: a brief commentary on definitions, measurement issues, and the current state of the literature. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.; 31:469-483.
    1. Fritzsche R. G., Switzer T. W., Hodgkinson B. J., Coyle E. F. 1999. Stroke volume decline during prolonged exercise is influenced by the increase in heart rate. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985); 86:799-805.
    1. Grego F., Vallier J. M., Collardeau M., Rousseu C., Cremieux J., Brisswalter J. 2005. Influence of exercise duration and hydration status on cognitive function during prolonged cycling exercise. Int. J. Sports Med.; 26:27-33.
    1. Hellstrom G., Fischer‐Colbrie W., Wahlgren N. G., Jogestrand T. 1996. Carotid artery blood flow and middle cerebral artery blood flow velocity during physical exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985); 81:413-418.
    1. Hornbein T. F. 2001. The high‐altitude brain. J. Exp. Biol.; 204:3129-3132.
    1. Ide K., Secher N. H. 2000. Cerebral blood flow and metabolism during exercise. Prog. Neurobiol.; 61:397-414.
    1. Kamijo K., Nishihira Y., Hatta A., Kaneda T., Kida T., Higashiura T. 2004. Changes in arousal level by differential exercise intensity. Clin. Neurophysiol.; 115:2693-2698.
    1. Lambourne K., Tomporowski P. 2010. The effect of exercise‐induced arousal on cognitive task performance: a meta‐regression analysis. Brain Res.; 1341:12-24.
    1. Lucas S. J., Ainslie P. N., Murrell C. J., Thomas K. N., Franz E. A., Cotter J. D. 2012. Effect of age on exercise‐induced alterations in cognitive executive function: relationship to cerebral perfusion. Exp. Gerontol.; 47:541-551.
    1. MacLeod C. M. 1991. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull.; 109:163-203.
    1. Marshall R. S., Lazar R. M., Pile‐Spellman J., Young W. L., Duong D. H., Joshi S. 2001. Recovery of brain function during induced cerebral hypoperfusion. Brain; 124:1208-1217.
    1. McMorris T., Sproule J., Turner A., Hale B. J. 2011. Acute, intermediate intensity exercise, and speed and accuracy in working memory tasks: a meta‐analytical comparison of effects. Physiol. Behav.; 102:421-428.
    1. Miyazawa T., Horiuchi M., Ichikawa D., Sato K., Tanaka N., Bailey D. M. 2012. Kinetics of exercise‐induced neural activation; interpretive dilemma of altered cerebral perfusion. Exp. Physiol.; 97:219-227.
    1. Murray N. P., Russoniello C. 2012. Acute physical activity on cognitive function: a heart rate variability examination. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback; 37:219-227.
    1. Ogoh S. 2008. Autonomic control of cerebral circulation: exercise. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.; 40:2046-2054.
    1. Ogoh S., Ainslie P. N. 2009a. Cerebral blood flow during exercise: mechanisms of regulation. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985); 107:1370-1380.
    1. Ogoh S., Ainslie P. N. 2009b. Regulatory mechanisms of cerebral blood flow during exercise: new concepts. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev.; 37:123-129.
    1. Ogoh S., Brothers R. M., Barnes Q., Eubank W. L., Hawkins M. N., Purkayastha S. 2005a. The effect of changes in cardiac output on middle cerebral artery mean blood velocity at rest and during exercise. J. Physiol.; 569:697-704.
    1. Ogoh S., Dalsgaard M. K., Yoshiga C. C., Dawson E. A., Keller D. M., Raven P. B. 2005b. Dynamic cerebral autoregulation during exhaustive exercise in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol.; 288:H1461-H1467.
    1. Ogoh S., Fadel P. J., Zhang R., Selmer C., Jans O., Secher N. H. 2005c. Middle cerebral artery flow velocity and pulse pressure during dynamic exercise in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol.; 288:H1526-H1531.
    1. Sato K., Ogoh S., Hirasawa A., Oue A., Sadamoto T. 2011. The distribution of blood flow in the carotid and vertebral arteries during dynamic exercise in humans. J. Physiol.; 589:2847-2856.
    1. Schreiber S. J., Gottschalk S., Weih M., Villringer A., Valdueza J. M. 2000. Assessment of blood flow velocity and diameter of the middle cerebral artery during the acetazolamide provocation test by use of transcranial Doppler sonography and MR imaging. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol.; 21:1207-1211.
    1. Serrador J. M., Picot P. A., Rutt B. K., Shoemaker J. K., Bondar R. L. 2000. MRI measures of middle cerebral artery diameter in conscious humans during simulated orthostasis. Stroke; 31:1672-1678.
    1. Stroop J. R. 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol.; 18:643-662.
    1. Tomporowski P. D. 2003. Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol. (Amst); 112:297-324.
    1. Vasques P. E., Moraes H., Silveira H., Deslandes A. C., Laks J. 2011. Acute exercise improves cognition in the depressed elderly: the effect of dual‐tasks. Clinics (Sao Paulo); 66:1553-1557.
    1. Wingo J. E., Lafrenz A. J., Ganio M. S., Edwards G. L., Cureton K. J. 2005. Cardiovascular drift is related to reduced maximal oxygen uptake during heat stress. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.; 37:248-255.
    1. Zappe A. C., Uludag K., Oeltermann A., Ugurbil K., Logothetis N. K. 2008. The influence of moderate hypercapnia on neural activity in the anesthetized nonhuman primate. Cereb Cortex; 18:2666-2673.

Source: PubMed

3
S'abonner