The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization

Lisa Feldman Barrett, Lisa Feldman Barrett

Abstract

The science of emotion has been using folk psychology categories derived from philosophy to search for the brain basis of emotion. The last two decades of neuroscience research have brought us to the brink of a paradigm shift in understanding the workings of the brain, however, setting the stage to revolutionize our understanding of what emotions are and how they work. In this article, we begin with the structure and function of the brain, and from there deduce what the biological basis of emotions might be. The answer is a brain-based, computational account called the theory of constructed emotion.

Keywords: affect; categorization; concepts; construction; emotion; interoception; predictive coding.

© The Author (2016). Published by Oxford University Press. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oup.com.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
The classical view of emotion. The classical view of emotion includes basic emotion theories (e.g. for a review, see Tracy and Randles, 2011), causal appraisal theories (e.g. Scherer, 2009; Roseman, 2011), and theories of emotion that rely on black-box functionalism (Davis, 1992; Anderson and Adolphs, 2014). Each emotion faculty is assumed to have its own innate ‘essence’ that distinguishes it from all other emotions. This might be a Lockean essence (an underlying causal mechanism that all instances of an emotion category share, making them that kind of emotion and not some other kind of emotion, depicted by the circles in the figure). Lockean essences might be a biological, such as a set of dedicated neurons, or psychological, such as a set of evaluative mechanisms called ‘appraisals’. An emotion category is usually assumed to have a Platonic essence [a physical fingerprint that instances of that emotion share, but that other emotions do not, such a set of facial movements (an ‘expression’), a pattern of autonomic nervous system activity, and/or a pattern of appraisals]. Of course, no one is expecting complete invariance, but it is assumed that instances of a category are similar enough to be easily diagnosed as the same emotion using objective (perceiver-independent) measures alone. (A) is adapted from Davis (1992). (B) is adapted Anderson and Adolphs (2014). (C) is adapted from Barrett (2006a), which reviews the growing evidence that contracts the classical view of emotion.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Hubs in the human brain. (A) Hubs of the rich club, adapted from van den Heuvel and Sporns (2013). These regions are strongly interconnected with one another and it is proposed that they integrate information across the brain to create large-scale patterns of information flow (i.e. synchronized activity; van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2013). They are sometimes referred to as convergence or confluence zones (e.g. Damasio, 1989; Meyer and Damasio, 2009). (B) Results of a forward inference analysis, revealing ‘hot spots’ in the brain that show a better than chance increase in BOLD signal across 5633 studies from the Neurosynth database. Activations are thresholded at FWE P < 0.05. Limbic regions (i.e. agranular/dysgranular with descending projections to visceromotor control nuclei) include the cingulate cortex [midcingulate cortex (MCC), pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (pgACC)], ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), supplementary motor and premotor areas (SMA and PMC), medial temporal lobe, the anterior insula (aINS) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) (e.g. Carrive and Morgan, 2012; Bar et al., 2016); for a discussion and additional references, see (Kleckner et al., in press). AG, angular gyrus; MC, motor cortex.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Neural activity during simulation. N = 16 (data from Wilson-Mendenhall et al., 2013). Participants listened with eyes closed to multimodal descriptions rich in sensory details and imagined each real-world scenario as if it was actually happening to them (i.e. the experiences were high in subjective realism). Contrast presented is scenario immersion > resting baseline; maps are FDR corrected P < 0.05. Left image, x = 1; right image, x = −42. Heightened neural activity in primary visual cortex (not labeled), somatosensory cortex (SSC), and MC during scenario immersion replicated prior simulation research (McNorgan, 2012) and established the validity of the paradigm. Notice that simulation was associated with an increase in BOLD response within primary interoceptive cortex (i.e. the pINS), in the sensory integration network of lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC) (Ongur et al., 2003) and in the thalamus; increased BOLD responses were also seen, as expected, in limbic and paralimbic regions such as the vmPFC, the aINS, the temporal pole (TP), SMA and vlPFC, as well as in the hypothalamus and the subcortical nuclei that control the internal milieu. PAG, periacquiductal gray; PBN, parabrachial nucleus.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
The brain is a concept generator. (A) Brodmann areas are shaded to depict their degree of laminar organization, including the insula (bottom right). The brain’s computational architecture is depicted (adapted from Barbas, 2015), where prediction signals flow from the deep layers of less granular regions (cell bodies depicted with triangles) to the upper layers of more granular regions; this, can also be thought of concept construction [as described in Barrett (2017)]. I hypothesize that agranular (i.e. limbic) cortices generatively combine past experiences to initiate the construction of embodied concepts; multimodal summaries cascade to sensory and motor systems to create the simulations that will become motor plans and perceptions. Prediction error processing, in turn, is akin to concept learning. The upper layers of cortex compress prediction errors and reduce error dimensionality, eventually creating multimodal summaries, by virtue of a cytoarchitectural gradient: prediction error flows from the upper layers of primary sensory and motor regions (highly granular cortex) populated with many small pyramidal cells with few connections towards less granular heteromodal regions (including limbic cortices) with fewer but larger pyramidal cells having many connections (Finlay and Uchiyama, 2015). (B) Evidence of conceptual processing in the default mode network: Multimodal summaries for emotion concepts [adapted from Skerry and Saxe (2015), Figure 1B]; summary representations of sensory-motor properties (color, shape, visual motion, sound and physical manipulation [Fernandino et al. (2016), Figure 5]; and, semantic processing [adapted from Binder and Desai (2011), Figure 2]. (C) Regions that consistently increase activity during emotional experience (green), emotion regulation (blue), and their overlap (red) [as appears in Clark-Polner et al. (2016); adapted from Buhle et al. (2014) and Satpute et al. (2015)]. Overlaps are observed in the aIns, vlPFC, the MCC, SMA and posterior superior temporal sulcus. Studies of emotional experience show consistent increase in activity that is consistent with manipulating predictions (i.e. the default mode and salience networks), whereas reappraisal instructions appear to manipulate the modification of those predictions (i.e. the frontoparietal and salience networks). (D) Intensity maps for five emotion categories examined by Wager et al. (2015). Maps represent the expected activations or population centers, given a specific emotion category. Maps also reflect expected co-activation patterns. Notice that population centers for all emotion categories can be found within the default mode and salience networks. These are probabilistic summaries, not brain states for emotion Adapted from Wager et al. (2015).
Fig. 5.
Fig. 5.
A depiction of predictive coding in the human brain. (A) Key limbic and paralimbic cortices (in blue) provide cortical control the body’s internal milieu. Primary MC is depicted in red, and primary sensory regions are in yellow. For simplicity, only primary visual, interoceptive and somatosensory cortices are shown; subcortical regions are not shown. (B) Limbic cortices initiate visceromotor predictions to the hypothalamus and brainstem nuclei (e.g. PAG, PBN, nucleus of the solitary tract) to regulate the autonomic, neuroendocrine, and immune systems (solid lines). The incoming sensory inputs from the internal milieu of the body are carried along the vagus nerve and small diameter C and Aδ fibers to limbic regions (dotted lines). Comparisons between prediction signals and ascending sensory input results in prediction error that is available to update the brain’s internal model. In this way, prediction errors are learning signals and therefore adjust subsequent predictions. (C) Efferent copies of visceromotor predictions are sent to MC as motor predictions (solid lines) and prediction errors are sent from MC to limbic cortices (dotted lines). (D) Sensory cortices receive sensory predictions from several sources. They receive efferent copies of visceromotor predictions (black lines) and efferent copies of motor predictions (red lines). Sensory cortices with less well developed lamination (e.g. primary interoceptive cortex) also send sensory predictions to cortices with more well-developed granular architecture (e.g. in this figure, somatosensory and primary visual cortices, gold lines). For simplicity’s sake, prediction errors are not depicted in panel D. sgACC, subgenual anterior cingulate cortex; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex; pgACC, pregenual anterior cingulate cortex; dmPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; MCC, midcingulate cortex; vaIns, ventral anterior insula; daIns, dorsal anterior insula and includes ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; SMA, supplementary motor area; PMC, premotor cortex m/pIns, mid/posterior insula (primary interoceptive cortex); SSC, somatosensory cortex; V1, primary visual cortex; and MC, motor cortex (for relevant neuroanatomy references, see Kleckner et al., in press).
Fig. 6.
Fig. 6.
A large-scale system for allostasis and interoception in the human brain. (A) The system implementing allostasis and interoception is composed of two large-scale intrinsic networks (shown in red and blue) that are interconnected by several hubs (shown in purple; for coordinates, see Kleckner et al., in press). Hubs belonging to the ‘rich club’ are labeled. These maps were constructed with resting state BOLD data from 280 participants, binarized at p < 10−5, and then replicated on a second sample of 270 participants. vaIns, ventral anterior insula; MCC, midcingulate cortex; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus; PostCG, postcentral gyrus; PAG, periaqueductal gray; PBN, parabrachial nucleus; NTS, the nucleus of the solitary tract; vStriat., ventral striatum; Hypothal, hypothalamus. (B) Reliable subcortical connections, thresholded P < 0.05 uncorrected, replicated in 270 participants.

References

    1. Adams R.A., Shipp S., Friston K.J. (2013). Predictions not commands: active inference in the motor system. Brain Structure and Function, 218(3), 611–43.
    1. Adolphs R. (2013). The biology of fear. Current Biology, 23(2), R79–93.
    1. Adolphs R., Russell J.A., Tranel D. (1999). A role for the human amygdala in recognizing emotional arousal from unpleasant stimuli. Psychological Science, 10(2),167–71.
    1. Adolphs R., Tranel D. (1999). Intact recognition of emotional prosody following amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 37(11),1285–92.
    1. Adolphs R., Tranel D. (2003). Amygdala damage impairs emotion recognition from scenes only when they contain facial expressions. Neuropsychologia, 41(10),1281–9.
    1. Alle H., Roth A., Geiger J.R. (2009). Energy-efficient action potentials in hippocampal mossy fibers. Science, 325(5946),1405–8. doi:10.1126/science.1174331
    1. Anderson D.J., Adolphs R. (2014). A framework for studying emotion across species. Cell, 157, 187–200.
    1. Anderson M.L. (2014). After Phrenology: Neural Reuse and the Interactive Brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.
    1. Anderson M.L., Finlay B.L. (2014). Allocating structure to function: the strong links between neuroplasticity and natural selection. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 918.
    1. Antoniadis E.A., Winslow J.T., Davis M., Amaral D.G. (2009). The nonhuman primate amygdala is necessary for the acquisition but not the retention of fear-potentiated startle. Biological Psychiatry, 65(3), 241–8.
    1. Arnal L.H., Giraud A.L. (2012). Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(7), 390–8.
    1. Atkinson A.P., Heberlein A.S., Adolphs R. (2007). Spared ability to recognise fear from static and moving whole-body cues following bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 45(12), 2772–82.
    1. Attwell D., Iadecola C. (2002). The neural basis of functional brain imaging signals. Trends in Neuroscience, 25(12), 621–5.
    1. Attwell D., Laughlin S.B. (2001). An energy budget for signaling in the grey matter of the brain. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 21(10), 1133–45.
    1. Bar K.J., de la Cruz F., Schumann A., et al. (2016). Functional connectivity and network analysis of midbrain and brainstem nuclei. NeuroImage, 134, 53–63.
    1. Bar M. (2009a). A cognitive neuroscience hypothesis of mood and depression. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(11), 456–63.
    1. Bar M. (2009b). The proactive brain: memory for predictions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1235–43.
    1. Barbas H. (2015). General cortical and special prefrontal connections: principles from structure to function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 38, 269–89.
    1. Barbas H., García-Cabezas M. (2015). Motor cortex layer 4: less is more. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(5),259–61.
    1. Barbas H., Rempel-Clower N. (1997). Cortical structure predicts the pattern of corticocortical connections. Cerebral Cortex, 7(7),635–46.
    1. Bargmann C.I. (2012). Beyond the connectome: how neuromodulators shape neural circuits. Bioessays, 34(6),458–65. doi:10.1002/bies.201100185
    1. Barrett L.F. (2006a). Emotions as natural kinds?. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 28–58.
    1. Barrett L.F. (2006b). Solving the emotion paradox: categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(1), 20–46.
    1. Barrett L.F. (2009). The future of psychology: connecting mind to brain. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(4), 326–39.
    1. Barrett L.F. (2011a). Bridging token identity theory and supervenience theory through psychological construction. Psychological Inquiry, 22(2), 115–27.
    1. Barrett L.F. (2011b). Was Darwin wrong about emotional expressions?. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 400–6.
    1. Barrett L.F. (2012). Emotions are real. Emotion, 12(3), 413–29.
    1. Barrett L.F. (2013). Psychological construction: A Darwinian approach to the science of emotion. Emotion Review, 5, 379–89.
    1. Barrett L.F. (2014). The conceptual act theory: a précis. Emotion Review, 6, 292–7.
    1. Barrett L.F. (2015). Ten common misconceptions about the psychological construction of emotion In: Barrett L.F., Russell J.A., editors. The psychological construction of emotion. p. 45-79, New York: Guilford.
    1. Barrett L.F. (2017). How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life the Brain. New York, NY: Houghton-Mifflin-Harcourt.
    1. Barrett L.F., Bliss-Moreau E. (2009). Affect as a psychological primitive. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 167–218.
    1. Barrett L.F., Lindquist K.A., Bliss-Moreau E., et al. (2007a). Of mice and men: natural kinds of emotions in the mammalian brain? A response to Panksepp and Izard. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 297–311.
    1. Barrett L.F., Mesquita B., Ochsner K.N., Gross J.J. (2007b). The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 373–403.
    1. Barrett L.F., Russell J.A. (1999). Structure of current affect: Controversies and emerging consensus. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(1), 10–4.
    1. Barrett L.F., Satpute A.B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 361–72.
    1. Barrett L.F., Simmons W.K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 419–29.
    1. Barrett L.F., Tugade M.M., Engle R.W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4),553–73.
    1. Barrett L.F., Wilson-Mendenhall C.D., Barsalou L.W. (2015). The conceptual act theory: a road map In: Barrett L.F., Russell J.A., editors. The Psychological Construction of Emotion. New York: Guilford.
    1. Barsalou L.W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory and Cognition, 11(3), 211–27.
    1. Barsalou L.W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Science, 22(4), 577–609. discussion 610-560.
    1. Barsalou L.W. (2003). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 513–62.
    1. Barsalou L.W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–45.
    1. Barsalou L.W. (2016). On staying grounded and avoiding Quixotic dead ends. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23(4), 1122–42.
    1. Barsalou L.W., Kyle Simmons W., Barbey A.K., Wilson C.D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 84–91.
    1. Bastos A.M., Usrey W.M., Adams R.A., Mangun G.R., Fries P., Friston K.J. (2012). Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron, 76(4), 695–711.
    1. Bechara A., Damasio H., Damasio A.R., Lee G.P. (1999). Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(13), 5473–81.
    1. Bechara A., Tranel D., Damasio H., Adolphs R., Rockland C., Damasio A.R. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269(5227), 1115–8.
    1. Becker B., Mihov Y., Scheele D., et al. (2012). Fear processing and social networking in the absence of a functional amygdala. Biological Psychiatry, 72(1), 70–7.
    1. Beckmann M., Johansen-Berg H., Rushworth M.F. (2009). Connectivity-based parcellation of human cingulate cortex and its relation to functional specialization. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(4), 1175–90.
    1. Berkes P., Orbán G., Lengyel M., Fiser J. (2011). Spontaneous cortical activity reveals hallmarks of an optimal internal model of the environment. Science, 331(6013), 83–7.
    1. Binder J.R., Desai R.H. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(11), 527–36.
    1. Binder J.R., Desai R.H., Graves W.W., Conant L.L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2767–96.
    1. Boes A.D., Mehta S., Rudrauf D., et al. (2011). Changes in cortical morphology resulting from long-term amygdala damage. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(5), 588–95.
    1. Boiger M., Mesquita B. (2012). The construction of emotion in interactions, relationships, and cultures. Emotion Review, 4
    1. Bressler S.L., Richter C.G. (2015). Interareal oscillatory synchronization in top-down neocortical processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 31, 62–6.
    1. Brodski A., Paach G.F., Helbling S., Wibral M. (2015). The faces of predictive coding. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 8997–9006.
    1. Buckner R.L. (2012). The serendipitous discovery of the brain’s default network. NeuroImage, 62(2), 1137–45.
    1. Buckner R.L., Andrews-Hanna J.R., Schacter D.L. (2008). The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38.
    1. Buckner R.L., Krienen F.M., Castellanos A., Diaz J.C., Yeo B.T. (2011). The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(5), 2322–45. doi:10.1152/jn.00339.2011
    1. Buhle J.T., Silvers J.A., Wager T.D., et al. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex
    1. Bullmore E., Sporns O. (2012). The economy of brain network organization. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(5), 336–49.
    1. Burrows M. (1996). The Neurobiology of an Insect Brain. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.
    1. Buzsáki G. (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.
    1. Cannon W.B., Britton S.W. (1925). Studies on the conditions of activity in endocrine glands. American Journal of Physiology–Legacy Content, 72(2), 283–94.
    1. Capitanio J.P., Cole S.W. (2015). Social instability and immunity in rhesus monkeys: the role of the sympathetic nervous system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal. Society B: Biological Scicnes, 370(1669), 20140104.
    1. Carrive P., Morgan M.M. (2012). Periaquiductal gray In: Mai J.K., Paxinos G., editors. The Human Nervous System, 3rd edn, pp. 367–400. Boston: Elsevier.
    1. Chanes L., Barrett L.F. (2016). Redefining the Role of Limbic Areas in Cortical Processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(2), 96–106.
    1. Clark A. (2013a). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36, 281–53.
    1. Clark A. (2013b). The many faces of precision (Replies to commentaries on “Whatever next? Neural prediction, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science”). Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 270.
    1. Clark-Polner E., Johnson T., Barrett L.F. (2016). Multivoxel pattern analysis does not provide evidence to support the existence of basic emotions. Cerebral Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw028.
    1. Clark-Polner E., Wager T.D., Satpute A.B., Barrett L.F. (2016). Neural fingerprinting: Meta-analysis, variation and the search for brain-based essences in the science of emotion In: Barrett L.F., Lewis M., Haviland-Jones J.M., editors. The handbook of emotion, 4th edn p. 146–165, New York: Guilford.
    1. Clore G.L., Ortony A. (2008). Appraisal theories: how cognition shapes affect into emotion In: Lewis M., Haviland-Jones J.M., Barrett L.F., editors. Handbook of Emotions, 3rd edn., pp. 628–42. New York: Guilford Press.
    1. Conant R.C., Ross Ashby W. (1970). Every good regulator of a system must be a model of that system†. International Journal of Systems Science, 1(2), 89–97.
    1. Counts S.E., Mufson E.J. (2012). The human nervous system In: Mai J.K., Paxinos G., editors. The Human Nervous System, pp. 425–38. Boston, MA: Elsevier.
    1. Craig A.D. (2015). How Do You Feel?: an Interoceptive Moment with Your Neurobiological Self. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
    1. Crivelli C., Jarillo S., Russell J.A., Fernandez-Dols J.M. (2016). Reading emotions from faces in two indigenous societies. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 145(7), 830–43.
    1. Crossley N.A., Mechelli A., Scott J., et al. (2014). The hubs of the human connectome are generally implicated in the anatomy of brain disorders. Brain, 137(8), 2382–95.
    1. Damasio A., Carvalho G.B. (2013). The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(2), 143–52.
    1. Damasio A.R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33(1–2), 25–62.
    1. Damasio A.R. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. Houghton: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
    1. Dantzer R., Heijnen C.J., Kavelaars A., Laye S., Capuron L. (2014). The neuroimmune basis of fatigue. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(1), 39–46.
    1. Danziger K. (1997). Naming the Mind: How Psychology Found Its Language. London, England: Sage.
    1. Darwin C. (1859/1964). On the Origin of Species A Facsimile of the First Edition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    1. Darwin C. (1872/2005). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals Stilwell, KS: Digireads. com.
    1. Davachi L., DuBrow S. (2015). How the hippocampus preserves order: the role of prediction and context. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(2), 92–9.
    1. Davis M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 353–75.
    1. de Gelder B., Terburg D., Morgan B., Hortensius R., Stein D. J., van Honk J. (2014). The role of human basolateral amygdala in ambiuous social threat perception. Cortex, 52, 28–34.
    1. De Martino B., Camerer C.F., Adolphs R. (2010). Amygdala damage eliminates monetary loss aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(8), 3788–92.
    1. Deneve S. (2008). Bayesian spiking neurons I: inference. Neural Computation, 20, 91–117.
    1. Deneve S., Jardri R. (2016). Circular inference: mistaken belief, misplaced trust. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 11, 40–8.
    1. Dewey J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. The Psychological Review, 3, 357–70.
    1. Doya K. (2008). Modulators of decision making. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 410–6.
    1. Dreyfus G., Thompson E. (2007). Asian perspectives: Indian theories of mind In: Zelazo P.D., Moscovitch M., Thompson E., editors. The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness, pp. 89–114. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    1. Dunsmoor J.E., Murty V.P., Davachi L., Phelps E.A. (2015). Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events. Nature, 520(7547), 345–8.
    1. Edelman G.M., Tononi G. (2000). A Universe of Consciousness: How Matter Becomes Imagination. New York: Basic books.
    1. Edelman G.M., Gally J.A. (2001). Degeneracy and complexity in biological systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 13763–8.
    1. Einstein A., Infeld L. (1938). Evolution of physics. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
    1. Etkin A., Buchel C., Gross J.J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(11), 693–+.
    1. Feinstein J.S. (2013). Lesion studies of human emotion and feeling. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 304–9.
    1. Feinstein J.S., Adolphs R., Damasio A., Tranel D. (2011). The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear. Current Biology, 21(1), 34–8.
    1. Feinstein J.S., Adolphs R., Tranel D. (2016). A tale of survival from the world of Patient S.M In: Amaral D.G., Adolphs R., editors. Living without an Amygdala, pp. 1–38. New York: Guilford.
    1. Feinstein J.S., Buzza C., Hurlemann R., et al. (2013). Fear and panic in humans with bilateral amygdala damage. Nature Neuroscience, 16(3), 270–2.
    1. Feinstein J.S., Rudrauf D., Khalsa S.S., et al. (2010). Bilateral limbic system destruction in man. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(1), 88–106.
    1. Feldman H., Friston K.J. (2010). Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 215.
    1. Fernandino L., Binder J.R., Desai R.H., et al. (2016). Concept representation reflects multimodal abstraction: A framework for embodied semantics. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 2018–34.
    1. Fernandino L., Humphries C.J., Seidenberg M.S., Gross W.L., Conant L.L., Binder J.R. (2015). Predicting brain activation patterns associated with individual lexical concepts based on five sensory-motor attributes. Neuropsychologia, 76, 17–26.
    1. Fields H. (2004). State-dependent opioid control of pain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(7), 565–75.
    1. Fields H.L., Margolis E.B. (2015). Understanding opioid reward. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(4), 217–25.
    1. Finlay B.L., Uchiyama R. (2015). Developmental mechanisms channeling cortical evolution. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(2), 69–76.
    1. Firestein S. (2012). Ignorance: How It Drives Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    1. Freddolino P.L., Tavazoie S. (2012). Beyond homeostasis: a predictive-dynamic framework for understanding cellular behavior. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology, 28, 363–84.
    1. Friston K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory?. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 127–38.
    1. Furlong T.M., Cole S., Hamlin A.S., McNally G.P. (2010). The role of prefrontal cortex in predictive fear learning. Behavioral Neuroscience 124(5), 574.
    1. Gallivan J.P., Logan L., Wolpert D.M., Flanagan J.R. (2016). Parallel specification of competing sensorimotor control policies for alternative action options. Nature Neuroscience, 19(2), 320–6.
    1. Gelman S.A., Rhodes M. (2012). Two-thousand years of stasis. How psychological essentialism impedes evolutionary understanding. In: Rosengren K.S., Brem S., Evans E.M., Sinatra G., editors. Evolution Challenges: Integrating Research and Practice in Teaching and Learning About Evolution, pp. 3–21. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    1. Gendron M., Roberson D., van der Vyver J.M., Barrett L.F. (2014a). Cultural relativity in perceiving emotion from vocalizations. Psychological Science, 25(4), 911–20.
    1. Gendron M., Roberson D., van der Vyver J.M., Barrett L.F. (2014b). Perceptions of emotion from facial expressions are not culturally universal: evidence from a remote culture. Emotion, 14(2), 251–62.
    1. Gendron M., Roberson D., Barrett L.F. (2015). Cultural variaton in emotion perception is real: a response to Sauter et al. Psychological Science, 26, 357–9.
    1. Ghashghaei H., Hilgetag C., Barbas H. (2007). Sequence of information processing for emotions based on the anatomic dialogue between prefrontal cortex and amygdala. NeuroImage, 34(3), 905–23.
    1. Gilbert D.T. (1998). Ordinary personology In: Fiske S.T., Gardner L., editors. The Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. 1 and 2, pp. 89–150. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
    1. Goodkind M., Eickhoff S.B., Oathes D.J., et al. (2015). Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(4), 305–15.
    1. Gross J.J., Barrett L.F. (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regulation: one or two depends on your point of view. Emotion Review, 3(1), 8–16.
    1. Gross C.T., Canteras N.S. (2012). The many paths to fear. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(9), 651–8.
    1. Gross J.J. (2015). Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1–26.
    1. Guillory S.A., Bujarski K.A. (2014). Exploring emotions using invasive methods: review of 60 years of human intracranial electrophysiology. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(12), 1880–9.
    1. Guitart-Masip M., Duzel E., Dolan R., Dayan P. (2014). Action vserus valence in decision making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 194–202.
    1. Haber S.N., Behrens T.E. (2014). The neural network underlying incentive-based learning: implications for interpreting circuit disruptions in psychiatric disorders. Neuron, 83(5), 1019–39.
    1. Hampton A.N., Adolphs R., Tyszka J.M., O’Doherty J.P. (2007). Contributions of the amygdala to reward expectancy and choice signals in human prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 55(4), 545–55.
    1. Harris J.J., Jolivet R., Attwell D. (2012). Synaptic energy use and supply. Neuron, 75(5), 762–77.
    1. Hassabis D., Maguire E.A. (2009). The construction system of the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1263–71.
    1. Hasson U., Chen J., Honey C.J. (2015). Hierarchical process memory: memory as an integral component of information processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(6), 304–13.
    1. Herry C., Johansen J.P. (2014). Encoding of fear learning and memory in distributed neuronal circuits. Nature Neuroscience, 17(12), 1644–54.
    1. Hodgkin A.L., Huxley A.F. (1952). A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. Journal of Physiology, 117(4), 500–44.
    1. Hohwy J. (2013). The Predictive Mind. Oxford: OUP Oxford.
    1. Hunter R.G., McEwen B.S. (2013). Stress and anxiety across the lifespan: structural plasticity and epigenetic regulation. Epigenomics, 5(2),177–94.
    1. Hurlemann R., Wagner M., Hawellek B., et al. (2007). Amygdala control of emotion-induced forgetting and remembering: evidence from Urbach-Wiethe disease. Neuropsychologia, 45(5),877–84.
    1. Iwata J., LeDoux J.E. (1988). Dissociation of associative and nonassociative concomitants of classical fear conditioning in the freely behaving rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102(1),66–76.
    1. James W. (1890/2007). The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 1 New York: Dover.
    1. John Y.J., Zikopoulos B., Bullock D., Barbas H. (2016). The emotional gatekeeper: A computational model of attention selection and suppression through the pathway from the amygdala to the inhibitory thalamic reticular nucleus. PLOS Computational Biology, 12(2),e1004722.
    1. Karmiloff-Smith A. (2009). Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: rethinking the study of developmental disorders. Developmental Psychology, 45(1),56–63.
    1. Kassam K.S., Markey A.R., Cherkassky V.L., Loewenstein G., Just M.A. (2013). Identifying emotions on the basis of neural activation. PLoS One, 8(6), e66032.
    1. Kleckner I.R., Zhang J., Touroutoglou A., et al. (in press). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nature Human Behavior. doi: 10.1101/098970.
    1. Kober H., Barrett L.F., Joseph J., Bliss-Moreau E., Lindquist K., Wager T.D. (2008). Functional grouping and cortical-subcortical interactions in emotion: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage, 42(2), 998–1031.
    1. Kok P., Bains L.J., van Mourik T., Norris D.G., de Lange F.P. (2016). Selective activation of the deep layers of the human primary visual cortex by top-down feedback. Current Biology, 26(3), 371–6.
    1. Kragel P.A., LaBar K.S. (2013). Multivariate pattern classification reveals autonomic and experiential representations of discrete emotions. Emotion, 13(4), 681–90.
    1. Kragel P.A., LaBar K.S. (2015). Multivariate neural biomarkers of emotional states are categorically distinct. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(11), 1437–48.
    1. Kragel P.A., LaBar K.S. (2016). Decoding the nature of emotion in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(6),444–55.
    1. Kuppens P., Tuerlinckx F., Russell J.A., Barrett L.F. (2013). The relation between valence and arousal in subjective experience. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 917–40.
    1. Laxminarayan S., Tadmor G., Diamond S.G., Miller E., Franceschini M.A., Brooks D.H. (2011). Modeling habituation in rat EEG-evoked responses via a neural mass model with feedback. Biological Cybernetics, 105(5–6), 371–97.
    1. Lazarus R.S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    1. LeDoux J.E. (2012). Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron, 73(4),653–76.
    1. Li S.S.Y., McNally G.P. (2014). The conditions that promote fear learning: prediction error and Pavlovian fear conditioning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 108, 14–21.
    1. Liang M., Mouraux A., Hu L., Iannetti G. (2013). Primary sensory cortices contain distinguishable spatial patterns of activity for each sense. Nature Communications, 4
    1. Lindquist K.A., Wager T.D., Kober H., Bliss-Moreau E., Barrett L.F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3),121–43.
    1. Lochmann T., Deneve S. (2011). Neural processing as causal inference. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 21(5),774–81.
    1. Makeig S., Debener S., Onton J., Delorme A. (2004). Mining event-related brain dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(5),204–10.
    1. Makeig S., Westerfield M., Jung T.P., et al. (2002). Dynamic brain sources of visual evoked responses. Science, 295(5555), 690–4.
    1. Marder E., Taylor A.L. (2011). Multiple models to capture the variability in biological neurons and networks. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 133–8.
    1. Mareschal D., Johnson M.H., Sirois S., Spratling M., Thomas M.S., Westermann G. (2007). Neuroconstructivism-I: How the Brain Constructs Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    1. Mason W.A., Capitanio J.P., Machado C.J., Mendoza S.P., Amaral D.G. (2006). Amygdalectomy and responsiveness to novelty in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): generality and individual consistency of effects. Emotion, 6(1), 73.
    1. Mayr E. (2004). What Makes Biology Unique?: Considerations on the Autonomy of a Scientific Discipline. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    1. Mazaheri A., Jensen O. (2010). Rhythmic pulsing: linking ongoing brain activity with evoked responses. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 177.
    1. McEwen B.S., Bowles N.P., Gray J.D., et al. (2015). Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 18(10), 1353–63.
    1. McGaugh J.L. (2016). Consolidating memories. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 1–24.
    1. McIntosh A.R. (2004). Contexts and catalysts: a resolution of the localization and integration of function in the brain. Neuroinformatics, 2(2), 175–82.
    1. McNally G.P., Johansen J.P., Blair H.T. (2011). Placing prediction into the fear circuit. Trends in Neurosciences, 34(6), 283–92.
    1. McNorgan C. (2012). A meta-analytic review of multisensory imagery identifies the neural correlates of modality-specific and modality-general imagery. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, Article 285.
    1. Mesulam M.M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain, 121(Pt 6), 1013–52.
    1. Mesulam M.M. (2002). The human frontal lobes: Transcending the default mode through contingent encoding In: Stuss D.T., Knight R.T., editors. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, pp. 8–30. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    1. Meyer K., Damasio A. (2009). Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 32, 376–82.
    1. Mihov Y., Kendrick K.M., Becker B., et al. (2013). Mirroring fear in the absence of a functional amygdala. Biological Psychiatry, 73(7),e9–11.
    1. Moran R.J., Campo P., Symmonds M., Stephan K.E., Dolan R.J., Friston K.J. (2013). Free energy, precision and learning: the role of cholinergic neuromodulation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(19),8227–36.
    1. Murphy G. (2002). The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
    1. Ongur D., Ferry A.T., Price J.L. (2003). Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 460(3), 425–49.
    1. Oosterwijk S., Lindquist K.A., Adebayo M., Barrett L.F. (2015). The neural representation of typical and atypical experiences of negative images: comparing fear, disgust and morbid fascination. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 11–22.
    1. Oosterwijk S., Lindquist K.A., Anderson E., Dautoff R., Moriguchi Y., Barrett L.F. (2012). States of mind: Emotions, body feelings, and thoughts share distributed neural networks. NeuroImage, 62(3), 2110–28.
    1. Oosterwijk S., Mackey S., Wilson-Mendenhall C., Winkielman P., Paulus M.P. (2015). Concepts in context: processing mental state concepts with internal or external focus involves different neural systems. Social Neuroscience, 10(3), 294–307.
    1. Pavlenko A. (2014). The Bilingual Mind: And What It Tells Us about Language and Thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    1. Peelen M.V., Atkinson A.P., Vuilleumier P. (2010). Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(30), 10127–34.
    1. Pezzulo G., Rigoli F., Friston K. (2015). Active Inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control. Progress in Neurobiology, 134, 17–35.
    1. Posner M.I., Keele S.W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77(3p1), 353–63.
    1. Power J.D., Cohen A.L., Nelson S.M., et al. (2011). Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron, 72(4),665–78.
    1. Pulvermüller F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9),458–70.
    1. Qian C., Di X. (2011). Phase or amplitude? The relationship between ongoing and evoked neural activity. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(29),10425–6.
    1. Raichle M.E. (2010). Two views of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(4),180–90.
    1. Rao R.P.N., Ballard D.H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 79–87.
    1. Raz G., Touroutoglou A., Gilam G., et al. (2016). Functional connectivity dynamics during film viewing reveal common networks for different emotional experiences. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 709–23.
    1. Rigotti M., Barak O., Warden M.R., et al. (2013). The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks. Nature, 497(7451),585–90.
    1. Robbins J., Rumsey A. (2008). Introduction: Cultural and linguistic anthropology and the opacity of other minds. Anthropological Quarterly, 81(2),407–20.
    1. Roseman I.J. (2011). Emotional behaviors, emotivational goals, emotion strategies: Multiple levels of organization integrate variable and consistent responses. Emotion Review, 3, 1–10.
    1. Russell J.A. (1991). Culture and the Categorization of Emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3),426–50.
    1. Saarimaki H., Gotsopoulos A., Jaaskelainen I.P., et al. (2016). Discrete Neural Signatures of Basic Emotions. Cerebral Cortex, 26(6),2563–73.
    1. Salamone J.D., Correa M. (2012). The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron, 76, 470–85.
    1. Sartorius N., Kaelber C.T., Cooper J.E., et al. (1993). Progress toward achieving a common language in psychiatry: results from the field trial of the clinical guidelines accompanying the WHO classification of mental and behavioral disorders in ICD-10. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50(2),115–24.
    1. Satpute A.B., Wilson-Mendenhall C.D., Kleckner I.R., Barrett L.F. (2015). Emotional experience In: Toga A.W., editor. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, pp. 65–72. Boston MA: Elsevier.
    1. Sayers B.M., Beagley H.A., Henshall W.R. (1974). The mechanism of auditory evoked EEG responses. Nature, 247, 481–3.
    1. Schacter D.L., Addis D.R., Buckner R.L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8(9), 657–61.
    1. Scheeringa R., Mazaheri A., Bojak I., Norris D.G., Kleinschmidt A. (2011). Modulation of visually evoked cortical FMRI responses by phase of ongoing occipital alpha oscillations. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(10), 3813–20.
    1. Scherer K.R. (2009). Emotions are emergent processes: they require a dynamic computational architecture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364 (1535), 3459–74.
    1. Schmahmann J.D. (2010). The role of the cerebellum in cognition and emotion: personal reflections since 1982 on the dysmetria of thought hypothesis, and its historical evolution from theory to therapy. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3),236–60.
    1. Schmahmann J.D., Pandya D.N. (1997). The cerebrocerebellar system. International Review of Neurobiology, 41, 31–60.
    1. Schultz W. (2016). Dopamine reward prediction-error signalling: a two-component response. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(3), 183–95.
    1. Searle J.R. (1992). The Rediscovery of the Mind. Cambridge: MIT press.
    1. Searle J.R. (2004). Mind: A Brief Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    1. Sepulcre J., Sabuncu M.R., Yeo T.B., Liu H., Johnson K.A. (2012). Stepwise connectivity of the modal cortex reveals the multimodal organization of the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(31),10649–61.
    1. Seth A.K. (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends in Cognitiive Sciences, 17(11), 565–73.
    1. Seth A.K., Suzuki K., Critchley H.D. (2012). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 395.
    1. Seth A. K., Friston K. J. (2016). Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0007.
    1. Sharpe M.J., Killcross S. (2015). The prelimbic cortex directs attention toward predictive cues during fear learning. Learning and Memory, 22(6), 289–93.
    1. Shipp S., Adams R.A., Friston K.J. (2013). Reflections on agranular architecture: predictive coding in the motor cortex. Trends in Neurosciences, 36(12), 706–16.
    1. Si M., Marsella S., Pynadath D. (2010). Modeling appraisal in Theory of Mind Reasoning. Journal of Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems, 20, 14–31.
    1. Skerry A.E., Saxe R. (2015). Neural representations of emotion are organized around abstract event features. Current Biology, 25(15), 1945–54.
    1. Sloan E.K., Capitanio J.P., Cole S.W. (2008). Stress-induced remodeling of lymphoid innervation. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 22(1), 15–21.
    1. Sloan E.K., Capitanio J.P., Tarara R.P., Mendoza S.P., Mason W.A., Cole S.W. (2007). Social stress enhances sympathetic innervation of primate lymph nodes: mechanisms and implications for viral pathogenesis. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(33), 8857–65.
    1. Spillmann L., Dresp-Langley B., Tseng C.H. (2015). Beyond the classical receptive field: The effect of contextual stimuli. Journal Vision, 15(9), 7.
    1. Sporns O. (2011). Networks of the Brain. Cambridge: MIT press.
    1. Stephens C.L., Christie I.C., Friedman B.H. (2010). Autonomic specificity of basic emotions: evidence from pattern classification and cluster analysis. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 463–73.
    1. Sterling P. (2012). Allostasis: a model of predictive regulation. Physiology and Behavior, 106(1), 5–15.
    1. Sterling P., Laughlin S. (2015). Principles of Neural Design. Cambridge: MIT Press.
    1. Stokes M.G., Kusunoki M., Sigala N., Nili H., Gaffan D., Duncan J. (2013). Dynamic coding for cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 78(2), 364–75.
    1. Strick P.L., Dum R.P., Fiez J.A. (2009). Cerebellum and Nonmotor Function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 413–34.
    1. Swanson L.W. (2000). Cerebral hemisphere regulation of motivated behavior. Brain Research, 886(1–2), 113–64.
    1. Swanson L.W. (2012). Brain Architecture: Understanding the Basic Plan. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    1. Terburg D., Morgan B., Montoya E., et al. (2012). Hypervigilance for fear after basolateral amygdala damage in humans. Translational Psychiatry, 2(5), e115.
    1. Tononi G., Sporns O., Edelman G.M. (1999). Measures of degeneracy and redundancy in biological networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96(6), 3257–62.
    1. Touroutoglou A., Hollenbeck M., Dickerson B.C., Barrett L.F. (2012). Dissociable large-scale networks anchored in the right anterior insula subserve affective experience and attention. NeuroImage.
    1. Touroutoglou A., Lindquist K.A., Dickerson B.C., Barrett L.F. (2015). Intrinsic connectivity in the human brain does not reveal networks for ‘basic’ emotions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(9), 1257–65.
    1. Tovote P., Fadok J.P., Lüthi A. (2015). Neuronal circuits for fear and anxiety. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(6), 317–31.
    1. Tracy J.L., Randles D. (2011). Four models of basic emotions: a review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and Watt. Emotion Review, 3(4), 397–405.
    1. Tsuchiya N., Moradi F., Felsen C., Yamazaki M., Adolphs R. (2009). Intact rapid detection of fearful faces in the absence of the amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 12(10), 1224–5.
    1. Turkheimer E., Pettersson E., Horn E.E. (2014). A phenotypic null hypothesis for the genetics of personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 515–40.
    1. Uddin L.Q. (2015). Salience procesing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Neuron, 16, 55–61.
    1. Ullsperger M., Danielmeier C., Jocham G. (2014). Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior. Physiological Reviews, 94, 35–79.
    1. van den Heuvel M.P., Kahn R.S., Goni J., Sporns O. (2012). High-cost, high-capacity backbone for global brain communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(28), 11372–7.
    1. van den Heuvel M.P., Sporns O. (2011). Rich-club organization of the human connectome. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(44), 15775–86.
    1. van den Heuvel M.P., Sporns O. (2013). An anatomical substrate for integration among functional networks in human cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(36), 14489–500.
    1. van den Heuvel M.P., Sporns O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 683–96.
    1. Voorspoels W., Vanpaemel W., Storms G. (2011). A formal ideal-based account of typicality. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18(5), 1006–14.
    1. Vytal K., Hamann S. (2010). Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: a voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2864–85.
    1. Wager T.D., Kang J., Johnson T.D., Nichols T.E., Satpute A.B., Barrett L.F. (2015). A Bayesian model of category-specific emotional brain responses. PLOS Computational Biology, 11(4), e1004066.
    1. Westermann G., Mareschal D., Johnson M.H., Sirois S., Spratling M.W., Thomas M.S.C. (2007). Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science, 10(1), 75–83.
    1. Whalen P.J. (1998). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: Initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(6), 177–88.
    1. Whitacre J., Bender A. (2010). Degeneracy: a design principle for achieving robustness and evolvability. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 263(1), 143–53.
    1. Whitacre J.M. (2010). Degeneracy: a link between evolvability, robustness and complexity in biological systems. Theoretical Biology and Medical Modelling, 7(6), 1–17.
    1. Wierzbicka A. (2014). Imprisoned in English. The Hazards of English as a Default Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    1. Wilson C.R., Gaffan D., Browning P.G., Baxter M.G. (2010). Functional localization within the prefrontal cortex: missing the forest for the trees?. Trends in Neurosciences, 33(12), 533–40.
    1. Wilson-Mendenhall C., Barrett L.F., Barsalou L.W. (2013). Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychological Science, 24, 947–56.
    1. Wilson-Mendenhall C.D., Barrett L.F., Barsalou L.W. (2015). Variety in emotional life: within-category typicality of emotional experiences is associated with neural activity in large-scale brain networks. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(1),62–71. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu037
    1. Wilson-Mendenhall C.D., Barrett L.F., Simmons W.K., Barsalou L.W. (2011). Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1105–27.
    1. Woodworth R.S., Sherrington C.S. (1904). A pseudaffective reflex and its spinal path. Journal of Physiology, 31(3–4), 234–43.
    1. Wundt W. (1897). Outlines of Psychology In: Judd C.H. (Trans.). Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann.
    1. Xu F., Kushnir T. (2013). Infants are rational constructivist learners. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 28–32.
    1. Zikopoulos B., Barbas H. (2006). Prefrontal projections to the thalamic reticular nucleus form a unique circuit for attentional mechanisms. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(28), 7348–61.
    1. Zikopoulos B., Barbas H. (2012). Pathways for emotions and attention converge on the thalamic reticular nucleus in primates. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(15), 5338–50.

Source: PubMed

3
S'abonner