Training attentional control in infancy

Sam Wass, Kaska Porayska-Pomsta, Mark H Johnson, Sam Wass, Kaska Porayska-Pomsta, Mark H Johnson

Abstract

Several recent studies have reported that cognitive training in adults does not lead to generalized performance improvements [1, 2], whereas many studies with younger participants (children 4 years and older) have reported distal transfer [3, 4]. This is consistent with convergent evidence [5-8] for greater neural and behavioral plasticity earlier in development. We used gaze-contingent paradigms to train 11-month-old infants on a battery of attentional control tasks. Relative to an active control group, and following only a relatively short training period, posttraining assessments revealed improvements in cognitive control and sustained attention, reduced saccadic reaction times, and reduced latencies to disengage visual attention. Trend changes were also observed in spontaneous looking behavior during free play, but no change was found in working memory. The amount of training correlated with the degree of improvement on some measures. These findings are to our knowledge the first demonstration of distal transfer following attentional control training in infancy. Given the longitudinal relationships identified between early attentional control and learning in academic settings [9, 10], and the causal role that impaired control of attention may play in disrupting learning in several disorders [11-14], the current results open a number of avenues for future work.

Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Results from Training (A) Training: average difficulty level. The longest unbroken instance of each training task per session was identified, and the average difficulty level of each task was calculated (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). Average difficulty level at visit 4 was normalized to 1, to allow comparison between the degrees of improvement at the different tasks. Error bars represent standard errors. (B) Training: training time. Gray lines show per-session training times for individual participants; the thick black line shows the average. The large change between visit 1 and visits 2–4 is because visit 1 was conducted immediately following the pretest assessment battery, so infants had already conducted circa 90 min of testing.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Results of Pre-post Assessments All plots show Δ, the change in performance (post − pre, baseline corrected) in the trained and control groups. Error bars represent standard errors. Asterisks indicate significance of ANCOVA analyses as described in Results: ∗p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01; (∗)p < 0.10. (A) Cognitive control. Graph shows proportion of correct anticipatory looks in the preswitch (initial rule learning) and postswitch (unlearning one rule and learning another) phases. (B) Gap overlap. “Average RT” is the average of the three conditions that we administered (gap, baseline, and overlap). “Facilitation” shows the facilitation effect, and “disengagement” shows disengagement latencies. Because the valence of the observed changes in the task was negative, −Δ values are presented for ease of comparison. (C) Sustained attention. “Mixed dynamic/static” shows the results of experiment 1, which measured looking behavior toward a mixture of dynamic and static stimuli. “Interesting static” and “boring static” show the results of experiment 2, which measured looking behavior toward “interesting” and “boring” static images. (D) Working memory. Graph shows median delay length for trials followed by a correct response. (E) Structured free play. “Looks to object” shows number of separate looks to the target objects. “Shifts from object to person” shows number of attention shifts from looking at the objects to looking at either the experimenter or caregiver. “Average duration of looks to object” shows the average duration of looks toward the target objects.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Results of Pre-post Assessments: Selected Scatter Plots from the Trained Group Showing Amount of Training Time against Change in Performance at Posttesting The y axes show the amount of total training time (in minutes) that each participant received. The x axes show trained infants' change in performance (post − pre) on three measures; in (B), −Δ values are presented for ease of comparison. For all three graphs, a position to the right of the y axis indicates improved performance posttraining. The regression lines indicate the significant bivariate correlations (see Results) observed between training time and outcome measures in (A) and (C).

References

    1. Owen A.M., Hampshire A., Grahn J.A., Stenton R., Dajani S., Burns A.S., Howard R.J., Ballard C.G. Putting brain training to the test. Nature. 2010;465:775–778.
    1. Dahlin E., Nyberg L., Bäckman L., Neely A.S. Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: Immediate training gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. Psychol. Aging. 2008;23:720–730.
    1. Rueda M.R., Rothbart M.K., McCandliss B.D., Saccomanno L., Posner M.I. Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:14931–14936.
    1. Thorell L.B., Lindqvist S., Bergman Nutley S., Bohlin G., Klingberg T. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 2009;12:106–113.
    1. Stiles J., Reilly J., Paul B., Moses P. Cognitive development following early brain injury: Evidence for neural adaptation. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2005;9:136–143.
    1. Huttenlocher P. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 2002. Neural Plasticity: The Effects of Environment on the Development of Cerebral Cortex.
    1. Thomas M.S.C., Johnson M.H. The computational modeling of sensitive periods. Dev. Psychobiol. 2006;48:337–344.
    1. Heckman J.J. Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science. 2006;312:1900–1902.
    1. Razza R.A., Martin A., Brooks-Gunn J. Associations among family environment, sustained attention, and school readiness for low-income children. Dev. Psychol. 2010;46:1528–1542.
    1. Welsh J.A., Nix R.L., Blair C., Bierman K.L., Nelson K.E. The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010;102:43–53.
    1. Cornish K., Scerif G., Karmiloff-Smith A. Tracing syndrome-specific trajectories of attention across the lifespan. Cortex. 2007;43:672–685.
    1. Elsabbagh M., Volein A., Holmboe K., Tucker L., Csibra G., Baron-Cohen S., Bolton P., Charman T., Baird G., Johnson M.H. Visual orienting in the early broader autism phenotype: Disengagement and facilitation. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2009;50:637–642.
    1. Cornish K., Sudhalter V., Turk J. Attention and language in fragile X. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2004;10:11–16.
    1. Karmiloff-Smith A. Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 1998;2:389–398.
    1. Dimitrov D.M., Rumrill P.D., Jr. Pretest-posttest designs and measurement of change. Work. 2003;20:159–165.
    1. Kovács A.M., Mehler J. Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:6556–6560.
    1. Shaddy D.J., Colombo J. Developmental changes in infant attention to dynamic and static stimuli. Infancy. 2004;5:355–365.
    1. Courage M.L., Reynolds G.D., Richards J.E. Infants' attention to patterned stimuli: Developmental change from 3 to 12 months of age. Child Dev. 2006;77:680–695.
    1. Gilmore R.O., Johnson M.H. Working memory in infancy: Six-month-olds' performance on two versions of the oculomotor delayed response task. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1995;59:397–418.
    1. Kannass K.N., Oakes L.M. The development of attention and its relations to language in infancy and toddlerhood. J. Cogn. Dev. 2008;9:222–246.
    1. Snyder H.R., Munakata Y. Becoming self-directed: Abstract representations support endogenous flexibility in children. Cognition. 2010;116:155–167.
    1. Tang Y.Y., Ma Y.H., Wang J., Fan Y.X., Feng S.G., Lu Q.L., Yu Q.B., Sui D., Rothbart M.K., Fan M., Posner M.I. Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:17152–17156.
    1. Michel F., Anderson M. Using the antisaccade task to investigate the relationship between the development of inhibition and the development of intelligence. Dev. Sci. 2009;12:272–288.
    1. Karbach J., Kray J. How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Dev. Sci. 2009;12:978–990.
    1. Klingberg T., Fernell E., Olesen P.J., Johnson M., Gustafsson P., Dahlström K., Gillberg C.G., Forssberg H., Westerberg H. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—a randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2005;44:177–186.
    1. Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2010;14:317–324.
    1. Mulder H., Pitchford N.J., Marlow N. Inattentive behaviour is associated with poor working memory and slow processing speed in very pre-term children in middle childhood. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2011;81:147–160.
    1. Rose S.A., Feldman J.F., Jankowski J.J. The structure of infant cognition at 1 year. Intelligence. 2005;33:231–250.
    1. Colombo J., Mitchell D.W. Infant visual habituation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2009;92:225–234.
    1. Noack H., Lövdén M., Schmiedek F., Lindenberger U. Cognitive plasticity in adulthood and old age: Gauging the generality of cognitive intervention effects. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2009;27:435–453.
    1. Berry A.S., Zanto T.P., Clapp W.C., Hardy J.L., Delahunt P.B., Mahncke H.W., Gazzaley A. The influence of perceptual training on working memory in older adults. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e11537.
    1. Jaeggi S.M., Buschkuehl M., Jonides J., Shah P. Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:10081–10086.
    1. Diamond A., Barnett W.S., Thomas J., Munro S. Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science. 2007;318:1387–1388.
    1. Chawarska K., Volkmar F., Klin A. Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2010;67:178–185.
    1. Lawson K.R., Ruff H.A. Early attention and negative emotionality predict later cognitive and behavioural function. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2004;28:157–165.
    1. Lawson K.R., Ruff H.A. Early focused attention predicts outcome for children born prematurely. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2004;25:399–406.
    1. van de Weijer-Bergsma E., Wijnroks L., Jongmans M.J. Attention development in infants and preschool children born preterm: A review. Infant Behav. Dev. 2008;31:333–351.
    1. Jankowski J.J., Rose S.A., Feldman J.F. Modifying the distribution of attention in infants. Child Dev. 2001;72:339–351.
    1. Johnson S.P., Amso D., Slemmer J.A. Development of object concepts in infancy: Evidence for early learning in an eye-tracking paradigm. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2003;100:10568–10573.
    1. Deligianni F., Senju A., Gergely G., Csibra G. Automated gaze-contingent objects elicit orientation following in 8-month-old infants. Dev. Psych. 2011 in press.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonner