Prebiotic intake reduces the waking cortisol response and alters emotional bias in healthy volunteers

Kristin Schmidt, Philip J Cowen, Catherine J Harmer, George Tzortzis, Steven Errington, Philip W J Burnet, Kristin Schmidt, Philip J Cowen, Catherine J Harmer, George Tzortzis, Steven Errington, Philip W J Burnet

Abstract

Rationale: There is now compelling evidence for a link between enteric microbiota and brain function. The ingestion of probiotics modulates the processing of information that is strongly linked to anxiety and depression, and influences the neuroendocrine stress response. We have recently demonstrated that prebiotics (soluble fibres that augment the growth of indigenous microbiota) have significant neurobiological effects in rats, but their action in humans has not been reported.

Objectives: The present study explored the effects of two prebiotics on the secretion of the stress hormone, cortisol and emotional processing in healthy volunteers.

Methods: Forty-five healthy volunteers received one of two prebiotics (fructooligosaccharides, FOS, or Bimuno®-galactooligosaccharides, B-GOS) or a placebo (maltodextrin) daily for 3 weeks. The salivary cortisol awakening response was sampled before and after prebiotic/placebo administration. On the final day of treatment, participants completed a computerised task battery assessing the processing of emotionally salient information.

Results: The salivary cortisol awakening response was significantly lower after B-GOS intake compared with placebo. Participants also showed decreased attentional vigilance to negative versus positive information in a dot-probe task after B-GOS compared to placebo intake. No effects were found after the administration of FOS.

Conclusion: The suppression of the neuroendocrine stress response and the increase in the processing of positive versus negative attentional vigilance in subjects supplemented with B-GOS are consistent with previous findings of endocrine and anxiolytic effects of microbiota proliferation. Further studies are therefore needed to test the utility of B-GOS supplementation in the treatment of stress-related disorders.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Cortisol awakening response before and after administration of placebo, B-GOS or FOS. There were no differences in the salivary CAR pre-administration. Salivary cortisol awakening response was significantly lower after 3 weeks of B-GOS intake, but not FOS intake, compared with placebo
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Area under the curve (with respect to ground) of salivary cortisol awakening response pre- and post-prebiotic supplement/placebo intake. *p < 0.05
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Vigilance reaction times in the attentional dot-probe task. a Attentional vigilance did not differ between groups during masked trials of the attentional dot-probe task. b Participants showed decreased attentional vigilance to negative versus positive words in the unmasked condition of the dot-probe task after B-GOS but not FOS intake compared to placebo

References

    1. Abou Hachem M, Andersen JM, Barrangou R, et al. Recent insight into oligosaccharide uptake and metabolism in probiotic bacteria. Biocatal Biotransform. 2013;31:226–235. doi: 10.3109/10242422.2013.828048.
    1. Ait-Belgnaoui A, Durand H, Cartier C, et al. Prevention of gut leakiness by a probiotic treatment leads to attenuated HPA response to an acute psychological stress in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1885–1895. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.03.024.
    1. Anderson N. Likableness ratings of 555 personality trait words. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1968;9:272–279. doi: 10.1037/h0025907.
    1. Beck AT. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatr. 2008;165:969–977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721.
    1. Beck AT, Clark DA. An information processing model of anxiety: automatic and strategic processes. Behav Res Ther. 1997;35:49–58. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00069-1.
    1. Beck AT, Ward C, Mendelson M, et al. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004.
    1. Bhagwagar Z, Cowen PJ. ‘It’s not over when it’s over’: persistent neurobiological abnormalities in recovered depressed patients. Psychol Med. 2007
    1. Bond A, Lader M. The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med Psychol. 1974;47:211–218. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1974.tb02285.x.
    1. Bradley BP, Mogg K, Falla SJ, Hamilton LR. Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: manipulation of stimulus duration. Cogn Emot. 1998;12:737–753. doi: 10.1080/026999398379411.
    1. Bravo JA, Forsythe P, Chew MV, et al. Ingestion of Lactobacillus strain regulates emotional behavior and central GABA receptor expression in a mouse via the vagus nerve. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108:16050–16055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1102999108.
    1. Bravo JA, Julio-Pieper M, Forsythe P, et al. Communication between gastrointestinal bacteria and the nervous system. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2012;12(6):667–672. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2012.09.010.
    1. Brenner DM, Moeller MJ, Chey WD, Schoenfeld PS. The utility of probiotics in the treatment of irritable bowel syndrome: a systematic review. Am J Gastroenterol. 2009;104:1033–1049. doi: 10.1038/ajg.2009.25.
    1. Browning M, Reid C, Cowen PJ, et al. A single dose of citalopram increases fear recognition in healthy subjects. J Psychopharmacol. 2006;21:684–690. doi: 10.1177/0269881106074062.
    1. Browning M, Holmes EA, Harmer CJ. The modification of attentional bias to emotional information: a review of the techniques, mechanisms, and relevance to emotional disorders. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010;10:8–20. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.1.8.
    1. Chan SWY, Goodwin GM, Harmer CJ. Highly neurotic never-depressed students have negative biases in information processing. Psychol Med. 2007;37:1281–1291. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000669.
    1. Cisler JM, Koster EHW. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30:203–216. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003.
    1. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404.
    1. Craig AD. How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555.
    1. Crumeyrolle-Arias M, Jaglin M, Bruneau A, et al. Absence of the gut microbiota enhances anxiety-like behavior and neuroendocrine response to acute stress in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;42:207–217. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.01.014.
    1. Cryan JF, Dinan TG. Mind-altering microorganisms: the impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:701–712. doi: 10.1038/nrn3346.
    1. Cryan JF, O’Mahony SM. The microbiome-gut-brain axis: from bowel to behavior. Neurogastroenterol Motil. 2011;23:187–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2010.01664.x.
    1. Dinan TG, Cryan JF. Regulation of the stress response by the gut microbiota: implications for psychoneuroendocrinology. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1369–1378. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.03.007.
    1. Dinan TG, Quigley EMM, Ahmed SMM, et al. Hypothalamic-pituitary-gut axis dysregulation in irritable bowel syndrome: plasma cytokines as a potential biomarker? Gastroenterology. 2006;130:304–311. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2005.11.033.
    1. Ekman P, Friesen WV (1976) Pictures of Facial Affect. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto
    1. Elliott R, Zahn R, Deakin JFW, Anderson IM. Affective cognition and its disruption in mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacol Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;36:153–182. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.77.
    1. Eysenck H, Eysenck S. Manual of the Eysenck personality questionnaire. London: Hodder & Stoughton; 1975.
    1. Fehring RJ, Schneider M, Raviele K. Variability in the phases of the menstrual cycle. J Obstet Gynecol Neonat Nurs. 2006;35(3):376–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2006.00051.x.
    1. First M, Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams J (1997) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (SCID-I)
    1. Forsythe P, Kunze WA. Voices from within: gut microbes and the CNS. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2013;70:55–69. doi: 10.1007/s00018-012-1028-z.
    1. Fu CHY, Williams SC, Cleare AJ, et al. Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: a prospective, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:877–889. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.877.
    1. Gareau MG, Jury J, MacQueen G, et al. Probiotic treatment of rat pups normalises corticosterone release and ameliorates colonic dysfunction induced by maternal separation. Gut. 2007;56:1522–1528. doi: 10.1136/gut.2006.117176.
    1. Gareau MG, Sherman PM, Walker WA. Probiotics and the gut microbiota in intestinal health and disease. Nat Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2010;7:503–514. doi: 10.1038/nrgastro.2010.117.
    1. Godlewska BR, Norbury R, Selvaraj S, et al. Short-term SSRI treatment normalises amygdala hyperactivity in depressed patients. Psychol Med. 2012;42:2609–2617. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712000591.
    1. Harmer CJ. Antidepressant drug action: a neuropsychological perspective. Depress Anxiety. 2010;27:231–233. doi: 10.1002/da.20680.
    1. Harmer CJ, Bhagwagar Z, Perrett DI, et al. Acute SSRI administration affects the processing of social cues in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:148–152. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300004.
    1. Harmer CJ, Shelley NC, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Increased positive versus negative affective perception and memory in healthy volunteers following selective serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibition. Am J Psychiatr. 2004;161:1256–1263. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1256.
    1. Harmer CJ, Goodwin GM, Cowen PJ. Why do antidepressants take so long to work? A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. Br J Psychiatr J Ment Sci. 2009;195:102–108. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051193.
    1. Kennedy PJ, Clarke G, Quigley EMM, et al. Gut memories: towards a cognitive neurobiology of irritable bowel syndrome. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:310–340. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.001.
    1. Koster EHW, Crombez G, Verschuere B, De Houwer J. Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behav Res Ther. 2004;42:1183–1192. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001.
    1. Koster EHW, Verschuere B, Crombez G, Van Damme S. Time-course of attention for threatening pictures in high and low trait anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 2005;43:1087–1098. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.004.
    1. Kunz-Ebrecht SR, Kirschbaum C, Marmot M, Steptoe A. Differences in cortisol awakening response on work days and weekends in women and men from the Whitehall II cohort. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:516–528. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00072-6.
    1. Lyte M. Probiotics function mechanistically as delivery vehicles for neuroactive compounds: microbial endocrinology in the design and use of probiotics. Bioessays. 2011;33:574–581. doi: 10.1002/bies.201100024.
    1. Mannie ZN, Harmer CJ, Cowen PJ. Increased waking salivary cortisol levels in young people at familial risk of depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:617–621. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.617.
    1. Mayer EA. Gut feelings: the emerging biology of gut–brain communication. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:453–466. doi: 10.1038/nrn3071.
    1. Messaoudi M, Lalonde R, Violle N, et al. Assessment of psychotropic-like properties of a probiotic formulation (Lactobacillus helveticus R0052 and Bifidobacterium longum R0175) in rats and human subjects. Br J Nutr. 2011;105:755–764. doi: 10.1017/S0007114510004319.
    1. Mogg K, Bradley BP. A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 1998;36:809–848. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00063-1.
    1. Mogg K, Bradley BP, Hallowell N. Attentional bias to threat: roles of trait anxiety, stressful events, and awareness. Q J Exp Psychol Sect A. 1994;47:841–864. doi: 10.1080/14640749408401099.
    1. Murphy SE, Norbury R, O’Sullivan U, et al. Effect of a single dose of citalopram on amygdala response to emotional faces. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;194:535–540. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.056093.
    1. Murphy SE, Yiend J, Lester KJ, et al. Short-term serotonergic but not noradrenergic antidepressant administration reduces attentional vigilance to threat in healthy volunteers. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol Off Sci J Coll Int Neuropsychopharmacologicum (CINP) 2009;12:169–179.
    1. Nelson H. The National Adult Reading Test (NART): test manual. Windsor: NFER-Nelson; 1982.
    1. Pariante CM, Lightman SL. The HPA axis in major depression: classical theories and new developments. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:464–468. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.06.006.
    1. Pizzagalli DA. Frontocingulate dysfunction in depression: toward biomarkers of treatment response. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;36:183–206. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.166.
    1. Pruessner JC, Wolf OT, Hellhammer DH, et al. Free cortisol levels after awakening: a reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Sci. 1997;61:2539–2549. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3205(97)01008-4.
    1. Rao AV, Bested AC, Beaulne TM, et al. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled pilot study of a probiotic in emotional symptoms of chronic fatigue syndrome. Gut Pathog. 2009;1:6. doi: 10.1186/1757-4749-1-6.
    1. Rhee SH, Pothoulakis C, Mayer EA. Principles and clinical implications of the brain–gut–enteric microbiota axis. Nat Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2009;6:306–314. doi: 10.1038/nrgastro.2009.35.
    1. Savignac HM, Corona G, Mills H, et al. Prebiotic feeding elevates central brain derived neurotrophic factor, N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor subunits and d-serine. Neurochem Int. 2013;63:756–764. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2013.10.006.
    1. Schlotz W, Yim IS, Zoccola PM, et al. The perceived stress reactivity scale: measurement invariance, stability, and validity in three countries. Psychol Assess. 2011;23:80–94. doi: 10.1037/a0021148.
    1. Sheline YI, Barch DM, Donnelly JM, et al. Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:651–658. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01263-X.
    1. Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene R. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1970.
    1. Sudo N, Chida Y, Aiba Y, et al. Postnatal microbial colonization programs the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal system for stress response in mice. J Physiol. 2004;558:263–275. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.063388.
    1. Tillisch K, Labus J, Kilpatrick L, et al. Consumption of fermented milk product with probiotic modulates brain activity. Gastroenterology. 2013;144:1394–1401.e4. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2013.02.043.
    1. Vulevic J, Drakoularakou A, Yaqoob P, et al (2008) Modulation of the fecal microflora profile and immune function by a novel trans-galactooligosaccharide mixture (B-GOS) in healthy elderly volunteers. Am J Clin Nutr 88:1438–1446
    1. Watson D, Clark L, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54:1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.
    1. Wolfram M, Bellingrath S, Kudielka BM. The cortisol awakening response (CAR) across the female menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36(6):905–912. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.12.006.
    1. Young AW, Rowland D, Calder AJ, et al (1997) Facial expression megamix: tests of dimensional and category accounts of emotion recognition. Cognition 63:271–313

Source: PubMed

3
Abonner