Furthering the understanding of olfaction, prevalence of loss of smell and risk factors: a population-based survey (OLFACAT study)

Joaquim Mullol, Isam Alobid, Franklin Mariño-Sánchez, Llorenç Quintó, Josep de Haro, Manuel Bernal-Sprekelsen, Antonio Valero, Cèsar Picado, Concepció Marin, Joaquim Mullol, Isam Alobid, Franklin Mariño-Sánchez, Llorenç Quintó, Josep de Haro, Manuel Bernal-Sprekelsen, Antonio Valero, Cèsar Picado, Concepció Marin

Abstract

Objectives: To investigate olfaction in general population, prevalence of olfactory dysfunction and related risk factors.

Design: Cross-sectional population-based survey, distributing four microencapsulated odorants (rose, banana, musk and gas) and two self-administered questionnaires (odour description; epidemiology/health status).

Setting: The survey was distributed to general population through a bilingual (Catalan, Spanish) newspaper in Catalonia (Spain), on December 2003.

Participants: Newspaper readers of all ages and gender; 9348 surveys were analysed from the 10 783 returned.

Main outcome measures: Characteristics of surveyed population, olfaction by age and gender, smell self-perception and smell impairment risk factors. Terms normosmia, hyposmia and anosmia were used when participants detected, recognised or identified all four, one to three or none of the odours, respectively.

Results: Survey profile was a 43-year-old woman with medium-high educational level, living in a city. Olfaction was considered normal in 80.6% (detection), 56% (recognition/memory) and 50.7% (identification). Prevalence of smell dysfunction was 19.4% for detection (0.3% anosmia, 19.1% hyposmia), 43.5% for recognition (0.2% anosmia, 43.3% hyposmia) and 48.8% for identification (0.8% anosmia, 48% hyposmia). Olfaction was worse (p<0.0001) in men than in women through all ages. There was a significant age-related smell detection decline however smell recognition and identification increased up to fourth decade and declined after the sixth decade of life. Risk factors for anosmia were: male gender, loss of smell history and poor olfactory self-perception for detection; low educational level, poor self-perception and pregnancy for recognition; and older age, poor self-perception and history of head trauma and loss of smell for identification. Smoking and exposure to noxious substances were mild protective factors for smell recognition.

Conclusions: Sense of smell in women is better than in men suggesting a learning process during life with deterioration in older ages. Poor self-perception, history of smell loss, head trauma and pregnancy are potential risk factors for olfactory disorders.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Flow-chart of participants in the OLFACAT (Olfaction in Catalonia) survey.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Evolution of normosmia (smell of all four odours) during lifetime. Smell detection showed a progressive decrease during the lifespan, while smell recognition/memory and identification increased up to the fourth decade of life, continued to plateau throughout the fifth and sixth decades, and declined thereafter. For detection, recognition/memory or identification, normosmia was significantly higher (p

Figure 3

Evolution of hyposmia (smell of…

Figure 3

Evolution of hyposmia (smell of one to three odours) during lifetime. For detection,…

Figure 3
Evolution of hyposmia (smell of one to three odours) during lifetime. For detection, hyposmia showed a progressive increase during the life span, while for recognition/memory and identification hyposmia decreased up to the fourth decade of life, continued to plateau throughout the fifth and sixth decades and increased thereafter. For detection, recognition/memory or identification, hyposmia was significantly lower (p

Figure 4

Evolution of anosmia (smell of…

Figure 4

Evolution of anosmia (smell of none of the four odours) during lifetime. Anosmia…

Figure 4
Evolution of anosmia (smell of none of the four odours) during lifetime. Anosmia showed a progressive mild increase during the life span but being more significant after the sixth decade of life. For detection, recognition/memory or identification, anosmia was significantly lower (p
Similar articles
Cited by
References
    1. Santos DV, Reiter ER, DiNardo LJ, et al. Hazardous events associated with impaired olfactory function. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2004;130:317–19 - PubMed
    1. Wysocki CJ, Gilbert AN. National Geographic Smell Survey. Effects of age are heterogenous. Ann NY Acad Sci 1989;561:12–28 - PubMed
    1. Hoffman HJ, Ishii EK, Macturk RH. Age-related changes in the prevalence of smell/taste problems among the United States adult population. Ann NY Acad Sci 1998;855:716–22 - PubMed
    1. Larsson M, Finkel D, Pedersen NL. Odor identification: influences of age, gender, cognition, and personality. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2000;55:304–10 - PubMed
    1. Larsson M, Nilsson L, Olofsson J, et al. Demographic and cognitive predictors of cued odor identification: evidence from a population-based study. Chem Senses 2004;29:547–54 - PubMed
Show all 36 references
Full text links [x]
[x]
Cite
Copy Download .nbib
Format: AMA APA MLA NLM

NCBI Literature Resources

MeSH PMC Bookshelf Disclaimer

The PubMed wordmark and PubMed logo are registered trademarks of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (HHS). Unauthorized use of these marks is strictly prohibited.

Follow NCBI
Figure 3
Figure 3
Evolution of hyposmia (smell of one to three odours) during lifetime. For detection, hyposmia showed a progressive increase during the life span, while for recognition/memory and identification hyposmia decreased up to the fourth decade of life, continued to plateau throughout the fifth and sixth decades and increased thereafter. For detection, recognition/memory or identification, hyposmia was significantly lower (p

Figure 4

Evolution of anosmia (smell of…

Figure 4

Evolution of anosmia (smell of none of the four odours) during lifetime. Anosmia…

Figure 4
Evolution of anosmia (smell of none of the four odours) during lifetime. Anosmia showed a progressive mild increase during the life span but being more significant after the sixth decade of life. For detection, recognition/memory or identification, anosmia was significantly lower (p
Similar articles
Cited by
References
    1. Santos DV, Reiter ER, DiNardo LJ, et al. Hazardous events associated with impaired olfactory function. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2004;130:317–19 - PubMed
    1. Wysocki CJ, Gilbert AN. National Geographic Smell Survey. Effects of age are heterogenous. Ann NY Acad Sci 1989;561:12–28 - PubMed
    1. Hoffman HJ, Ishii EK, Macturk RH. Age-related changes in the prevalence of smell/taste problems among the United States adult population. Ann NY Acad Sci 1998;855:716–22 - PubMed
    1. Larsson M, Finkel D, Pedersen NL. Odor identification: influences of age, gender, cognition, and personality. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2000;55:304–10 - PubMed
    1. Larsson M, Nilsson L, Olofsson J, et al. Demographic and cognitive predictors of cued odor identification: evidence from a population-based study. Chem Senses 2004;29:547–54 - PubMed
Show all 36 references
Full text links [x]
[x]
Cite
Copy Download .nbib
Format: AMA APA MLA NLM
Figure 4
Figure 4
Evolution of anosmia (smell of none of the four odours) during lifetime. Anosmia showed a progressive mild increase during the life span but being more significant after the sixth decade of life. For detection, recognition/memory or identification, anosmia was significantly lower (p

References

    1. Santos DV, Reiter ER, DiNardo LJ, et al. Hazardous events associated with impaired olfactory function. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2004;130:317–19
    1. Wysocki CJ, Gilbert AN. National Geographic Smell Survey. Effects of age are heterogenous. Ann NY Acad Sci 1989;561:12–28
    1. Hoffman HJ, Ishii EK, Macturk RH. Age-related changes in the prevalence of smell/taste problems among the United States adult population. Ann NY Acad Sci 1998;855:716–22
    1. Larsson M, Finkel D, Pedersen NL. Odor identification: influences of age, gender, cognition, and personality. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2000;55:304–10
    1. Larsson M, Nilsson L, Olofsson J, et al. Demographic and cognitive predictors of cued odor identification: evidence from a population-based study. Chem Senses 2004;29:547–54
    1. Brämerson A, Johansson L, Ek L, et al. Prevalence of olfactory dysfunction: the skovde population-based study. Laryngoscope 2004;114:733–7
    1. Ciofalo A, Filiaci F, Romeo R, et al. Epidemiological aspects of olfactory dysfunction. Rhinology 2006;44:78–82
    1. Murphy C, Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, et al. Prevalence of olfactory impairment in older adults. JAMA 2002;288:2307–12
    1. Landis BN, Konnerth CG, Hummel T. A study on the frequency of olfactory dysfunction. Laryngoscope 2004;114:1764–9
    1. Frye RE, Schwartz BS, Doty RL. Dose-related effects of cigarette smoking on olfactory function. JAMA 1990;263:1233–6
    1. Vennemann MM, Hummel T, Berger K. The association between smoking and smell and taste impairment in the general population. J Neurol 2008;255:1121–6
    1. Guilemany JM, Mariño-Sánchez FS, Angrill J, et al. The importance of smell in patients with bronchiectasis. Respir Med 2011;105:44–9
    1. Stata Statistical Software: Release 8.0. College Station, TX: Stata Corporation, 2003.
    1. Davis L. Practical aspects of nutrition of the elderly at home. In: Munro H, Schlierf G, eds. Nutrition in the Elderly. Nestle Nutrition Workshop Series, vol. 29 New York, NY: Raven Press, 1992:203–9
    1. Chalke HD, Dewhurst JR. Accidental coal-gas poisoning. BMJ 1957;2:915–17
    1. Doty RL. Studies of human olfaction from the University of Pennsylvania Smell and Taste. Chem Senses 1997;22:565–86
    1. Doty RL, Shaman P, Applebaum SL, et al. Smell identification ability: changes with age. Science 1984;226:1441–3
    1. Mariño-Sánchez FS, Alobid I, Cantellas S, et al. Smell training increases cognitive smell skills of wine tasters compared to the general healthy population. The WINECAT Study. Rhinology 2010;48:273–6
    1. Nakashima T, Kimmelman CP, Snow lB. Structure of human fetal and adult olfactory neuroepithelium. Arch Otolaryngol 1984;110:641–6
    1. Krmpotic-Nemanic J. Presbycusis, presbystasis, and presbyosmia as consequences of the analagous biological process. Acta Otolaryngol 1969;67:217–23
    1. Rombaux P, Mouraux A, Bertrand B, et al. Olfactory function and olfactory bulb volume in patients with postinfectious olfactory loss. Laryngoscope 2006;116:436–9
    1. Meisami E, Mikhail L, Baim D, et al. Human Olfactory bulb: aging of glomeruli and mitral cells and a search for the accessory olfactory bulb. Ann NY Acad Sci 1998;855:708–15
    1. Jernigan TL, Archibald SL, Fennema-Notestine C, et al. Effects of age on tissues and regions of the cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiol Aging 2001;22:581–94
    1. Cardesín A, Alobid I, Benítez P, et al. Barcelona Smell Test-24 (BAST-24): validation and smell characteristiques in the healthy Spanish population. Rhinology 2006;44:83–9
    1. Wohlgemuth C, Beinder E, Ochsenbein-Kölble N, et al. Changes in olfactory function with several pregnancies? Swiss Med Wkly 2008;138:466–9
    1. Nordin S, Broman DA, Olofsson JK, et al. A longitudinal descriptive study of self-reported abnormal smell and taste perception in pregnant women. Chem Senses 2004;29:391–402
    1. Welge-Lüssen A, Wolfensberger M. Olfactory disorders following upper respiratory tract infections. Adv Otorhinolaryngol 2006;63:125–32
    1. Sigurdardottir S, Jerstad T, Andelic N, et al. Olfactory dysfunction, gambling task performance and intracranial lesions after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 2010;24:504–13
    1. Hubert HB, Fabsitz RR, Feinleib M, et al. Olfactory sensitivity in human: genetic versus environmental control. Science 1980;9:607–9
    1. Haehner A, Boesveldt S, Berendse HW, et al. Prevalence of smell loss in Parkinson's disease—a multicenter study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2009;15:490–4
    1. Wirdefeldt K, Adami HO, Cole P, et al. Epidemiology and etiology of Parkinson's disease: a review of the evidence. Eur J Epidemiol 2011;26(Suppl 1):S1–58
    1. Rushforth SL, Allison C, Wonnacott S, et al. Subtype-selective nicotinic agonists enhance olfactory working memory in normal rats: a novel use of the odour span task. Neurosci Lett 2010;471:114–18
    1. Shimohama S. Nicotinic receptor-mediated neuroprotection in neurodegenerative disease models. Biol Pharm Bull 2009;32:332–6
    1. Larsson M, Bäckman L. Age-related differences in episodic odour recognition: the role of access to specific odour names. Memory 1997;5:361–78
    1. Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosselli M, et al. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:495–513
    1. Angel l, Fay S, Bouazzaoui B, et al. Protective role of educational level on episodic memory aging: an event-related potential study. Brain Cognit 2010;74:312–23

Source: PubMed

3
Abonner