The dual neural effects of oxytocin in autistic youth: results from a randomized trial

Adi Korisky, Abraham Goldstein, Ilanit Gordon, Adi Korisky, Abraham Goldstein, Ilanit Gordon

Abstract

Recent discoveries have highlighted the effects of oxytocin (OT) on social behavior and perception among autistic individuals. However, a gap persists in the literature regarding the potential effects of OT and the neural temporal dynamics due to OT administration. We explored the effect of OT on autistic individuals using magnetoencephalography (MEG), focusing on M100, M170, and M250, social perception-related components that tend to show atypical patterns in autistic individuals. Twenty-five autistic adolescents participated in this randomized, double-blind MEG study. Autistic individuals arrived at the lab twice and received an acute dose of intranasal OT or placebo in each session. During the scans, participants were asked to identify pictures of social and non-social stimuli. Additionally, 23 typically developing (TD) adolescents performed the same task in the MEG as a benchmark that allowed us to better characterize neural regions of interest and behavioral results for this age group in this task. A source-model beamformer analysis revealed that OT enhanced neural activity for social stimuli in frontal regions during M170. Additionally, in each of the preselected time windows, OT increased activation in the left hemisphere, regardless of the content of the presented stimuli. We suggest that OT increased the processing of social stimuli through two separate mechanisms. First, OT increased neural activity in a nonspecific manner, allowing increased allocation of attention toward the stimuli. Second, OT enhanced M170 activity in frontal regions only in response to social stimuli. These results reveal the temporal dynamics of the effects of OT on the early stages of social and non-social perception in autistic adolescents.Trial registration: This study was a part of a project registered as clinical trial October 27th, 2021. ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT05096676.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

© 2022. The Author(s).

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Behavioral paradigm—an example of two separate trials. The experimental procedure was adapted from the 'Reading the Mind in the Eyes' Task (RMET). It consisted of eight blocks—half contained pictures of faces (social condition, see example above the line) and the other half contained pictures of vehicles (non-social condition, see example below the line). The task lasted approximately 13 min in total. The order of the blocks and the order of the pictures inside each block were randomized and counterbalanced across participants. The three dots at the end of each row mark the continuity of the block.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Evoked responses locked to social and non-social stimuli. (A) Root mean square (RMS) responses to social and non-social stimuli averaged for all experimental groups. (B) Scalp topographies showing the differences between social and non-social neural responses in each component (M100 – left, M170 -middle, M250 -right). Black dots represent MEG sensors with a significant difference in the average neural response between social and non-social stimuli. (C) RMS responses to social and non-social stimuli for each study group separately: ASD-OT (top panel), ASD-PL (middle panel), and TD (bottom panel).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Source-level interaction between OT and PL sessions in the M170 time window. OT increased neural activation in medial frontal regions in response to social stimuli compared to non-social pictures. (A) Significant differences in neural activity between social and non-social stimuli were identified based on the t-values. (B) The average power of the significant cluster. Lines represent the average activity of the ASD group in each session. Dots represent individual data. (C) Source-level activity from a virtual sensor in the significant cluster. The horizontal black line represents the significant time window (p < .05, corrected for multiple comparisons). Straight lines represent neural activity in social trials, the dashed line represents activity in non-social trials. The color-coding is the same as in panel B.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Main effect of OT is source-level. In general, compared to PL, OT increased neural activation in frontal, temporal, and occipital regions, regardless of the content (social/non-social) of the presented stimuli. Top panel–significant differences in each of the preselected components of interest between OT and PL sessions. Significant differences were observed in the left hemisphere only. Bottom panel–average power in the significant clusters for each group separately. Vertical lines represent mean values ±1 SE.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Neural activity in typical social-related ROIs of the experimental groups. These ROIs were pooled from the M170 time windows of the TD group. In this period, higher neural activity was observed in response to social stimuli than non-social stimuli. The middle section shows posterior and bottom views of social-related ROIs in the TD study. Color coding represents t-values. These regions represent neural regions that were more active in social trials than in non-social trials. The left and right panels show the average neural activity in these clusters (left and right, respectively) in each group separately.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Behavioral results for accuracy rates and reaction times. After OT administration, the accuracy rates of the autistic individuals more closely resembled those of the TD study group. Analyses also revealed that all individuals presented higher ACC and lower RTs in the non-social trials. Accuracy (top panel) and RT (bottom panel) in response to social (left side) and non-social (right side) stimuli in the ASD-OT, ASD-PL, and control groups. Black lines represent the mean values of the groups. Vertical lines represent 1 SEM. Dots represent the individual data in the group.

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (2013)
    1. Adolphs R, Sears L, Piven J. Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2001;13(2):232–240. doi: 10.1162/089892901564289.
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The ‘reading the mind in the eyes’ test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2001;42(2):241–251. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715.
    1. Lozier LM, Vanmeter JW, Marsh AA. Impairments in facial affect recognition associated with autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Dev. Psychopathol. 2014 doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000479.
    1. Mitchell P, Sheppard E, Cassidy S. Autism and the double empathy problem: Implications for development and mental health. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2021;39(1):1–18. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12350.
    1. Crompton CJ, Ropar D, Evans-Williams CVM, Flynn EG, Fletcher-Watson S. Autistic peer-to-peer information transfer is highly effective. Autism. 2020;24(7):1704–1712. doi: 10.1177/1362361320919286.
    1. Bolis D, Balsters J, Wenderoth N, Becchio C, Schilbach L. Beyond autism: Introducing the dialectical misattunement hypothesis and a bayesian account of Intersubjectivity. Psychopathology. 2018;50(6):355–372. doi: 10.1159/000484353.
    1. Gimpl G, Fahrenholz F, Gene C. The oxytocin receptor system: Structure, function and regulation. Physiol. Rev. 2001;81(2):629–683. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.2.629.
    1. Gordon I, et al. Oxytocin enhances brain function in children with autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2013;110(52):20953–20958. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312857110.
    1. Kanat M, Heinrichs M, Schwarzwald R, Domes G. Oxytocin attenuates neural reactivity to masked threat cues from the eyes. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40183(10):287–295. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.183.
    1. Auyeung B, et al. Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time, naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. Transl. Psychiatry. 2015;5(2):e507. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.146.
    1. Andari E, Duhamel J-R, Zalla T, Herbrecht E, Leboyer M, Sirigua A. Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2010;107(9):4389–4394. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910249107.
    1. Domes G, Kumbier E, Heinrichs M, Herpertz SC. Oxytocin promotes facial emotion recognition and amygdala reactivity in adults with Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(3):698–706. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.254.
    1. Guastella AJ, et al. Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;67(7):692–694. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.020.
    1. Kruppa JA, et al. Neural modulation of social reinforcement learning by intranasal oxytocin in male adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: A randomized trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019 doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0258-7.
    1. Parker KJ, et al. Intranasal oxytocin treatment for social deficits and biomarkers of response in children with autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2017 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1705521114.
    1. Dadds MR, MacDonald E, Cauchi A, Williams K, Levy F, Brennan J. Nasal oxytocin for social deficits in childhood autism: A randomized controlled trial. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014;44(3):521–531. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1899-3.
    1. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Abu-Akel A. The Social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol. Psychiat. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020.
    1. Kosaka H, et al. Oxytocin efficacy is modulated by dosage and oxytocin receptor genotype in young adults with high-functioning autism: a 24-week randomized clinical trial. Transl. Psychiatry. 2016;6(8):e872. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.152.
    1. Andari E, Richard N, Leboyer M, Sirigu A. Adaptive coding of the value of social cues with oxytocin, an fMRI study in autism spectrum disorder. Cortex. 2016;76:79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.12.010.
    1. Aoki Y, et al. Oxytocin’s neurochemical effects in the medial prefrontal cortex underlie recovery of task-specific brain activity in autism: a randomized controlled trial. Mol. Psychiatry. 2015;20(4):447–453. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.74.
    1. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Kumbier E, Grossmann A, Hauenstein K, Herpertz SC. Effects of intranasal oxytocin on the neural basis of face processing in autism spectrum disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.007.
    1. Gordon I, et al. Intranasal oxytocin enhances connectivity in the neural circuitry supporting social motivation and social perception in children with autism. Sci. Rep. 2016 doi: 10.1038/srep35054.
    1. Groppe SE, et al. Oxytocin influences processing of socially relevant cues in the ventral tegmental area of the human brain. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.023.
    1. Tillman R, et al. Oxytocin enhances the neural efficiency of social perception. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00071.
    1. Bentin S, Allison T, Puce A, Perez E, McCarthy G. Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1996;8(6):551–565. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551.
    1. Van der Donck S, et al. Monitoring the effect of oxytocin on the neural sensitivity to emotional faces via frequency-tagging EEG: A double-blind, cross-over study. Psychophysiology. 2022 doi: 10.1111/psyp.14026.
    1. Rutherford HJV, Guo XM, Graber KM, Hayes NJ, Pelphrey KA, Mayes LC. Intranasal oxytocin and the neural correlates of infant face processing in non-parent women. Biol. Psychol. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.08.002.
    1. Huffmeijer R, et al. The impact of oxytocin administration and maternal love withdrawal on event-related potential (ERP) responses to emotional faces with performance feedback. Horm. Behav. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.008.
    1. Althaus M, et al. Do blood plasma levels of oxytocin moderate the effect of nasally administered oxytocin on social orienting in high-functioning male adults with autism spectrum disorder? Psychopharmacology. 2016 doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4339-1.
    1. Althaus M, Groen Y, Wijers AA, Noltes H, Tucha O, Hoekstra PJ. Oxytocin enhances orienting to social information in a selective group of high-functioning male adults with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2015;79:53–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.10.025.
    1. Liu J, Harris A, Kanwisher N. Stages of processing in face perception: An MEG study. Nat. Neurosci. 2002 doi: 10.1038/nn909.
    1. Kylliainen A, Braeutigam S, Hietanen JK, Swithenby SJ, Bailey AJ. Face-and gaze-sensitive neural responses in children with autism: A magnetoencephalographic study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006;24(9):2679–2690. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05132.x.
    1. Kovarski K, Mennella R, Wong SM, Dunkley BT, Taylor MJ, Batty M. Enhanced early visual responses during implicit emotional faces processing in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019 doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3787-3.
    1. Batty M, Meaux E, Wittemeyer K, Rogé B, Taylor MJ. Early processing of emotional faces in children with autism: An event-related potential study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2011 doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.001.
    1. McPartland JC, Wu J, Bailey CA, Mayes LC, Schultz RT, Klin A. Atypical neural specialization for social percepts in autism spectrum disorder. Soc. Neurosci. 2011 doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.586880.
    1. Hileman CM, Henderson H, Mundy P, Newell L, Jaime M. Developmental and individual differences on the P1 and N170 ERP components in children with and without autism. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2011 doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549870.
    1. Streit M, Wölwer W, Brinkmeyer J, Ihl R, Gaebel W. EEG-correlates of facial affect recognition and categorisation of blurred faces in schizophrenic patients and healthy volunteers. Schizophr. Res. 2001 doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00041-4.
    1. Schweinberger SR, Pickering EC, Jentzsch I, Burton AM, Kaufmann JM. Event-related brain potential evidence for a response of inferior temporal cortex to familiar face repetitions. Cogn. Brain Res. 2002 doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00142-8.
    1. Safar K, Vandewouw MM, Taylor MJ. Atypical development of emotional face processing networks in autism spectrum disorder from childhood through to adulthood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2021 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101003.
    1. Lord C, et al. Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS) J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2000;30(3):205–223. doi: 10.1007/BF02211841.
    1. D. Wechsler, WASI -II Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence—second edition. (2011)
    1. Webb SJ, Merkle K, Murias M, Richards T, Aylward E, Dawson G. ERP responses differentiate inverted but not upright face processing in adults with ASD. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2012;7(5):578–587. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp002.
    1. Churches O, Damiano C, Baron-Cohen S, Ring H. Getting to know you: The acquisition of new face representations in autism spectrum conditions. NeuroReport. 2012 doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283556658.
    1. Uzefovsky F, et al. The oxytocin receptor gene predicts brain activity during an emotion recognition task in autism. Mol. Autism. 2019 doi: 10.1186/s13229-019-0258-4.
    1. The jamovi project, “jamovi (Version 2.2) [Computer Software].” Retrieved from . (2021)
    1. Black MH, et al. Mechanisms of facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: Insights from eye tracking and electroencephalography. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.06.016.
    1. Hinojosa JA, Mercado F, Carretié L. N170 sensitivity to facial expression: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.002.
    1. Tal I, Abeles M. Cleaning MEG artifacts using external cues. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.04.002.
    1. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM. FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011 doi: 10.1155/2011/156869.
    1. Zeev-Wolf M, Dor-Ziderman Y, Goldstein A, Bonne O, Abramowitz EG. Oscillatory brain mechanisms of the hypnotically-induced out-of-body experience. Cortex. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.025.
    1. Van Veen BD, Van Drongelen W, Yuchtman M, Suzuki A. Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 1997 doi: 10.1109/10.623056.
    1. Evans AC. The NIH MRI study of normal brain development. Neuroimage. 2006;30(1):184–202. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.068.
    1. Kana RK, et al. Aberrant functioning of the theory-of-mind network in children and adolescents with autism. Mol. Autism. 2015 doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0052-x.
    1. Van Overwalle F. Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009 doi: 10.1002/hbm.20547.
    1. MacDonald K, MacDonald TM. The peptide that binds: A systematic review of oxytocin and its prosocial effects in humans. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry. 2010 doi: 10.3109/10673220903523615.
    1. Quintana DS, Guastella AJ. An allostatic theory of oxytocin. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.03.008.
    1. Kapetaniou GE, et al. The role of oxytocin in delay of gratification and flexibility in non-social decision making. Elife. 2021 doi: 10.7554/ELIFE.61844.
    1. Harari-Dahan O, Bernstein A. Oxytocin attenuates social and non-social avoidance: Re-thinking the social specificity of Oxytocin. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.04.005.
    1. Giel K, Zipfel S, Hallschmid M. Oxytocin and eating disorders: A narrative review on emerging findings and perspectives. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 2018 doi: 10.2174/1570159x15666171128143158.
    1. Miller MA, Bershad A, King AC, Lee R, De Wit H. Intranasal oxytocin dampens cue-elicited cigarette craving in daily smokers: A pilot study. Behav. Pharmacol. 2016 doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000260.
    1. Hansson AC, et al. Oxytocin reduces alcohol cue-reactivity in alcohol-dependent rats and humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018 doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.257.
    1. Geschwind DH. Oxytocin for autism spectrum disorder—Down, but not out. N. Engl. J. Med. 2021;385(16):1524–1525. doi: 10.1056/nejme2110158.
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. Reading the Mind in the Eyes; test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2001;42(2):241–251. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715.
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Cross P. Reading the Eyes: Evidence for the development of perception in the development of a theory of mind. Mind. Lang. 1992;7(1):173–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00203.x.
    1. Bukowski H, Dricot L, Hanseeuw B, Rossion B. Cerebral lateralization of face-sensitive areas in left-handers: Only the FFA does not get it right. Cortex. 2013;49(9):2583–2589. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.05.002.
    1. Frässle S, Krach S, Paulus FM, Jansen A. Handedness is related to neural mechanisms underlying hemispheric lateralization of face processing. Sci. Rep. 2016 doi: 10.1038/srep27153.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonner