Observation of limb movements reduces phantom limb pain in bilateral amputees

Monica L Tung, Ian C Murphy, Sarah C Griffin, Aimee L Alphonso, Lindsey Hussey-Anderson, Katie E Hughes, Sharon R Weeks, Victoria Merritt, Joseph M Yetto, Paul F Pasquina, Jack W Tsao, Monica L Tung, Ian C Murphy, Sarah C Griffin, Aimee L Alphonso, Lindsey Hussey-Anderson, Katie E Hughes, Sharon R Weeks, Victoria Merritt, Joseph M Yetto, Paul F Pasquina, Jack W Tsao

Abstract

Background: Mirror therapy has been demonstrated to reduce phantom limb pain (PLP) experienced by unilateral limb amputees. Research suggests that the visual feedback of observing a limb moving in the mirror is critical for therapeutic efficacy.

Objective: Since mirror therapy is not an option for bilateral lower limb amputees, the purpose of this study was to determine if direct observation of another person's limbs could be used to relieve PLP.

Methods: We randomly assigned 20 bilateral lower limb amputees with PLP to visual observation (n = 11) or mental visualization (n = 9) treatment. Treatment consisted of seven discrete movements which were mimicked by the amputee's phantom limbs moving while visually observing the experimenter's limbs moving, or closing the eyes while visualizing and attempting the movements with their phantom limbs, respectively. Participants performed movements for 20 min daily for 1 month. Response to therapy was measured using a 100-mm visual analog scale (VAS) and the McGill Short-Form Pain Questionnaire (SF-MPQ).

Results: Direct visual observation significantly reduced PLP in both legs (P < 0.05). Amputees assigned to the mental visualization condition did not show a significant reduction in PLP.

Interpretation: Direct visual observation therapy is an inexpensive and effective treatment for PLP that is accessible to bilateral lower limb amputees.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Visual analog scale (VAS) scores reported by subjects assigned to either the direct observation (DO) or mental visualization (MV) treatment groups pre- and posttherapeutic intervention. VAS scores range from 0 to 100 with greater numbers indicating higher reported pain levels. Data are presented as the mean (SD) for each group at two time points - pre- and post-treatment after 20 sessions. ***P < 0.01.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Cumulative Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire scores reported by subjects assigned to either the direct visual observation (DO) or mental visualization (MV) treatment groups pre- and posttherapeutic intervention. The McGill short form is scored out of a total of 45, with greater scores indicating more severe phantom limb pain symptoms. Data are presented as the mean (SD) for each group at two time points - pre- and post-treatment after 20 sessions. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01.

References

    1. Manchikanti L, Singh V. Managing phantom pain. Pain Physician. 2004;7:365–376.
    1. Flor H, Elbert T, Knecht S, et al. Phantom-limb pain as a perceptual correlate of cortical reorganization following arm amputation. Nature. 1995;375:482–484.
    1. Roux FE, Lotterie JA, Cassol E, et al. Cortical areas involved in virtual movement of phantom limbs: comparison with normal subjects. Neurosurgery. 2003;53:1342–1353.
    1. Giraux P, Sirigu A. Illusory movements of the paralyzed limb restore motor cortex activity. Neuroimage. 2003;20:S107–S111.
    1. Grüsser SM, Winter C, Schaefer M, et al. Perceptual phenomena after unilateral arm amputation: a pre-post-surgical comparison. Neurosci Lett. 2001;302:13–16.
    1. Lotze M, Montoya P, Erb M, et al. Activation of cortical and cerebellar motor areas during executed and imagined hand movements: an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 1999;11:491–501.
    1. Flor H, Nikolajsen L, Jensen TS. Phantom limb pain: a case of maladaptive CNS plasticity? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:873–881.
    1. Ramachandran VS, Stewart M, Rogers-Ramachandran D. Perceptual correlates of massive cortical reorganization. NeuroReport. 1992;3:583–586.
    1. Makin TR, Scholz J, Filippini N, et al. Phantom pain is associated with preserved structure and function in the former hand area. Nat Commun. 2013;4:1570.
    1. Anderson-Barnes VC, McAuliffe C, Swanberg KM, Tsao JW. Phantom limb pain – a phenomenon of proprioceptive memory? Med Hypotheses. 2009;73:555–558.
    1. Ramachandran VS, Hirstein W. The perception of phantom limbs. The DO Hebb lecture. Brain. 1998;121:1603–1630.
    1. Ramachandran V, Altschuler EL. The use of visual feedback, in particular mirror visual feedback, in restoring brain function. Brain. 2009;132:1693–1710.
    1. McCormick Z, Chang-Chien G, Marshall B, et al. Phantom limb pain: a systematic neuroanatomical-based review of pharmacologic treatment. Pain Med. 2013;15:292–305.
    1. Rothgangel AS, Braun SM, Beurskens AJ, et al. The clinical aspects of mirror therapy in rehabilitation: a systematic review of the literature. Int J Rehabil Res. 2011;34:1–13.
    1. Ramachandran VS, Rogers-Ramachandran D. Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;263:377–386.
    1. Chan BL, Witt R, Charrow AP, et al. Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain. N Engl J Med. 2007;357:2206–2207.
    1. Penn-Barwell JG. Outcomes in lower limb amputation following trauma: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Injury. 2011;42:1474–1479.
    1. Ziegler-Graham K, MacKenzie EJ, Ephraim PL, et al. Estimating the prevalence of limb loss in the United States: 2005 to 2050. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89:422–429.
    1. Su PF, Gard SA, Lipschutz RD, Kuiken TA. Gait characteristics of persons with bilateral transtibial amputations. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2007;44:491–501.
    1. Ramachandran VS, Brang D. Sensations evoked in patients with amputation from watching an individual whose corresponding intact limb is being touched. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:1281.
    1. Tombaugh TN. The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact and cognitively impaired individuals. Psychol Assess. 1997;9:260.
    1. Melzack R. The short-form McGill pain questionnaire. Pain. 1987;30:191–197.
    1. Reilly KT, Sirigu A. The motor cortex and its role in phantom limb phenomena. Neuroscientist. 2008;14:195–202.
    1. Marconi B, Koch G, Pecchioli C, et al. Breakdown of inhibitory effects induced by foot motor imagery on hand motor area in lower-limb amputees. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;118:2468–2478.
    1. MacIver K, Lloyd D, Kelly S, et al. Phantom limb pain, cortical reorganization and the therapeutic effect of mental imagery. Brain. 2008;131:2181–2191.
    1. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:169–192.
    1. Fitzgibbon BM, Enticott PG, Rich AN, et al. High incidence of ‘synaesthesia for pain’in amputees. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:3675–3678.
    1. Weeks SR, Tsao JW. Incorporation of another person’s limb into body image relieves phantom limb pain: a case study. Neurocase. 2010;16:461–465.
    1. Moseley GL. Graded motor imagery for pathologic pain A randomized controlled trial. Neurology. 2006;67:2129–2134.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonner