Understanding the effects of stress and alcohol cues on motivation for alcohol via behavioral economics

Michael Amlung, James MacKillop, Michael Amlung, James MacKillop

Abstract

Background: Psychological stress and alcohol cues are common antecedents of both ongoing drinking and relapse. One candidate mechanism of risk from these factors is acute increases in craving, but experimental support for this hypothesis is mixed. Furthermore, the combination of stress and cues has been largely unstudied. The current study employed a behavioral economic approach to investigate the combined roles of psychosocial stress and alcohol cues on motivation for alcohol.

Methods: In a sample of 84 adult heavy drinkers, we examined the effects of an acute laboratory stress induction and an alcohol cue exposure on subjective craving and stress, arousal, and behavioral economic decision making. Primary dependent measures included an intertemporal cross-commodity multiple-choice procedure (ICCMCP), incorporating both price and delay elements, an alcohol purchase task (APT), measuring alcohol demand, and a monetary delay discounting task, measuring intertemporal choice.

Results: The stress induction significantly increased stress, craving, and the incentive value of alcohol on the ICCMCP and APT. Stress-related increases in value on the ICCMCP were mediated by increased alcohol demand. Exposure to alcohol cues only significantly affected craving, APT breakpoint, and arousal. Delay discounting was not affected by either stress or cues.

Conclusions: These results reveal unique behavioral economic dimensions of motivation for alcohol following acute stress and an alcohol cue exposure. More broadly, as the first application of this approach to understanding the role of stress in drug motivation, these findings support its utility and potential in future applications.

Keywords: Alcohol; Behavioral Economics; Craving; Incentive Value; Stress.

Copyright © 2014 by the Research Society on Alcoholism.

Figures

Figure 1. Relationship between behavioral economic measures
Figure 1. Relationship between behavioral economic measures
The primary behavioral economic measure—the intertemporal cross-commodity multiple choice procedure (ICCMP)—combined elements of cost and delay into a single assessment. Effects on the ICCMP were further disentangled using the two secondary measures. The alcohol purchase task (APT) and delay discounting task (DDT) examined the influence of alcohol demand and intertemporal choice, respectively. Sample items from each measure are provided.
Figure 2. Schematic diagram of laboratory session…
Figure 2. Schematic diagram of laboratory session components
Assessments are listed in top row, exposures and other key events are listed in middle row, and corresponding time points relative to the start of the session are provided in the bottom row. Random selection and provision of ICCMCP outcome (alcohol or money) occurred following Post-Cues assessment.
Figure 3. Significant effects of acute stress…
Figure 3. Significant effects of acute stress on affect, arousal, craving, and behavioral economic indices
Panel A depicts composite stress ratings; Panel B depicts subjective craving for alcohol; Panels C–D depict physiological arousal (MAP: Mean Arterial Pressure); Panel E depicts crossover point on the intertemporal cross-commodity multiple choice procedure (ICCMP); Panels F–H depict Intensity, Breakpoint and Omax from the alcohol purchase task. Bars reflect mean (+/− standard error). *p<.05; **p<.01.
Figure 4. Effects of alcohol cues on…
Figure 4. Effects of alcohol cues on arousal, craving, and alcohol demand
Panel A depicts change in alcohol craving; Panel B depicts change in heart rate; Panel C depicts change in Breakpoint on the alcohol purchase task. Note, in Panel C, the two groups were significantly different at the Post-Stress time point (p = .04). In each panel, solid lines reflect the alcohol cues group; dashed lines reflect the neutral cues group. Data points reflect mean (+/− standard error). **p<.01; ***p<.001.

References

    1. Amlung M, Acker J, Stojek M, Murphy JG, MacKillop J. Is talk ‘cheap’? An initial investigation of the equivalence of alcohol purchase task performance for hypothetical and actual rewards. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2012;36:716–724.
    1. Benson TA, Little CS, Henslee AM, Correia CJ. Effects of reinforcer magnitude and alternative reinforcer delay on preference for alcohol during a multiple-choice procedure. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;100:161–163.
    1. Bickel WK, Landes RD, Christensen DR, Jackson L, Jones BA, Kurth-Nelson Z, Redish AD. Single- and cross-commodity discounting among cocaine addicts: the commodity and its temporal location determine discounting rate. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;217:177–187.
    1. Bickel WK, Marsch LA. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. Addiction. 2001;96:73–86.
    1. Bujarski S, MacKillop J, Ray LA. Understanding naltrexone mechanism of action and pharmacogenetics in Asian Americans via behavioral economics: a preliminary study. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2012;20:181–190.
    1. Carter BL, Tiffany ST. Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction. 1999;94:327–340.
    1. Coffey SF, Saladin ME, Drobes DJ, Brady KT, Dansky BS, Kilpatrick DG. Trauma and substance cue reactivity in individuals with comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder and cocaine or alcohol dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002;65:115–127.
    1. Cooney NL, Litt MD, Morse PA, Bauer LO, Gaupp L. Alcohol cue reactivity, negative-mood reactivity, and relapse in treated alcoholic men. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106:243–250.
    1. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:355–391.
    1. Dixon MR, Jacobs EA, Sanders S. Contextual control of delay discounting by pathological gamblers. J Appl Behav Anal. 2006;39:413–422.
    1. Field M, Powell H. Stress increases attentional bias for alcohol cues in social drinkers who drink to cope. Alcohol Alcohol. 2007;42:560–566.
    1. Field M, Rush M, Cole J, Goudie A. The smoking Stroop and delay discounting in smokers: effects of environmental smoking cues. J Psychopharmacol. 2007;21:603–610.
    1. Fox HC, Bergquist KL, Hong KI, Sinha R. Stress-induced and alcohol cue-induced craving in recently abstinent alcohol-dependent individuals. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:395–403.
    1. Gray JC, Amlung M, Acker J, Sweet LH, Mackillop J. Item-based analysis of delayed reward discounting decision making. Behav Processes 2014
    1. Hursh SR, Silberberg A. Economic demand and essential value. Psychological Review. 2008;115:186–198.
    1. Judd CM, Kenny DA, McClelland GH. Estimating and testing mediation and moderation in within-subject designs. Psychol Methods. 2001;6:115–134.
    1. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1999;128:78–87.
    1. Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’--a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28:76–81.
    1. Koffarnus MN, Bickel WK. A 5-trial adjusting delay discounting task: Accurate discount rates in less than 60 seconds. Experimental & Clinical Psychopharmacology in press.
    1. Lempert KM, Porcelli AJ, Delgado MR, Tricomi E. Individual differences in delay discounting under acute stress: the role of trait perceived stress. Frontiers in psychology. 2012;3:251.
    1. Liu X, Weiss F. Additive effect of stress and drug cues on reinstatement of ethanol seeking: exacerbation by history of dependence and role of concurrent activation of corticotropin-releasing factor and opioid mechanisms. J Neurosci. 2002;22:7856–7861.
    1. MacKillop J, Amlung M, Murphy CM, Acker J, Ray LA. In: A behavioral economic approach to health behavior, in Theory and Practice for a New Public Health, Theory and Practice for a New Public Health. DICLEMENTE R, SALAZAR LF, CROSBY RA, editors. Jones and Bartlett Publishers; Sudbury, MA: 2011a. pp. 131–162.
    1. MacKillop J, Amlung MT, Few LR, Ray LA, Sweet LH, Munafo MR. Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011b;216:305–321.
    1. MacKillop J, Brown CL, Stojek MK, Murphy CM, Sweet L, Niaura RS. Behavioral economic analysis of withdrawal- and cue-elicited craving for tobacco: an initial investigation. Nicotine Tob Res. 2012;14:1426–1434.
    1. MacKillop J, Miranda R, Jr, Monti PM, Ray LA, Murphy JG, Rohsenow DJ, McGeary JE, Swift RM, Tidey JW, Gwaltney CJ. Alcohol demand, delayed reward discounting, and craving in relation to drinking and alcohol use disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010a;119:106–114.
    1. MacKillop J, Murphy JG. A behavioral economic measure of demand for alcohol predicts brief intervention outcomes. Drug & Alcohol Dependence. 2007;89:227–233.
    1. MacKillop J, O’Hagen S, Lisman SA, Murphy JG, Ray LA, Tidey JW, McGeary JE, Monti PM. Behavioral economic analysis of cue-elicited craving for alcohol. Addiction. 2010b;105:1599–1607.
    1. McClure EA, Vandrey RG, Johnson MW, Stitzer ML. Effects of varenicline on abstinence and smoking reward following a programmed lapse. Nicotine Tob Res. 2013;15:139–148.
    1. McRae-Clark AL, Carter RE, Price KL, Baker NL, Thomas S, Saladin ME, Giarla K, Nicholas K, Brady KT. Stress- and cue-elicited craving and reactivity in marijuana-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;218:49–58.
    1. Mitchell JM, Fields HL, D’Esposito M, Boettiger CA. Impulsive responding in alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:2158–2169.
    1. Mokdad AH, Marks JS, Stroup DF, Grerberding JL. Actual causes of death in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2004;291:1238–1245.
    1. Monti PM, Binkoff JA, Abrams DB, Zwick WR, Nirenberg TD, Liepman MR. Reactivity of alcoholics and nonalcoholics to drinking cues. J Abnorm Psychol. 1987;96:122–126.
    1. Murphy JG, MacKillop J. Relative reinforcing efficacy of alcohol among college student drinkers. Experimental & Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2006;14:219–227.
    1. NIAAA. National Advisory Council on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism - Recommended Council Guidelines on Ethyl Alcohol Administration in Human Experimentation. Revised May 2005 Available at: .
    1. NIAAA. Rethinking Drinking: Alcohol and Your Health. National Institutes of Health; Bethesda, MD: 2010. NIH Publication No. 10-3770, in Series Rethinking Drinking: Alcohol and Your Health; NIH Publication No. 10-3770.
    1. Odum AL, Madden GJ, Bickel WK. Discounting of delayed health gains and losses by current, never- and ex-smokers of cigarettes. Nicotine Tob Res. 2002;4:295–303.
    1. Petry NM. Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;154:243–250.
    1. Pratt WM, Davidson D. Role of the HPA Axis and the A118G Polymorphism of the μ-Opioid Receptor in Stress-Induced Drinking Behavior. Alcohol Alcohol 2009
    1. Ramo DE, Brown SA. Classes of substance abuse relapse situations: a comparison of adolescents and adults. Psychol Addict Behav. 2008;22:372–379.
    1. Ray LA. Stress-induced and cue-induced craving for alcohol in heavy drinkers: Preliminary evidence of genetic moderation by the OPRM1 and CRH-BP genes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:166–174.
    1. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Incentive-sensitization and addiction. Addiction. 2001;96:103–114.
    1. Rousseau GS, Irons JG, Correia CJ. The reinforcing value of alcohol in a drinking to cope paradigm. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 2011
    1. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption. II. Addiction. 1993;88:791–804.
    1. Sobell LC, Sobell MB. Timeline follow-back: A technique for assessing self-reported alcohol consumption. In: LITTEN RZ, ALLEN JP, editors. Measuring alcohol consumption: Psychosocial and biochemical methods., Measuring alcohol consumption: Psychosocial and biochemical methods. Humana Press; Totowa, NJ: 1992. pp. 41–72.
    1. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 4. Allyn & Bacon; Needham Heights, MA: 2001.
    1. Thomas SE, Randall PK, Brady K, See RE, Drobes DJ. An acute psychosocial stressor does not potentiate alcohol cue reactivity in non-treatment-seeking alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:464–473.
    1. Tice DM, Bratslavsky E, Baumeister RF. Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:53–67.
    1. Vuchinich RE, Heather N. Series Choice, Behavioural Economics and Addiction. Elsevier; Amsterdam: 2003. Choice, Behavioural Economics and Addiction.
    1. White MJ, Morris CP, Lawford BR, Young RM. Behavioral phenotypes of impulsivity related to the ANKK1 gene are independent of an acute stressor. Behav Brain Funct. 2008;4:54.
    1. Zwaan RA, Stanfield RA, Madden CJ. How persistent is the effect of smoking urges on cognitive performance? Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2000;8:518–523.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonner