Cognitive and Psychosocial Outcomes of Self-Guided Executive Function Training and Low-Intensity Aerobic Exercise in Healthy Older Adults

Lixia Yang, Sara N Gallant, Leanne Karyn Wilkins, Ben Dyson, Lixia Yang, Sara N Gallant, Leanne Karyn Wilkins, Ben Dyson

Abstract

Objectives: Prior work has demonstrated that executive function training or physical exercise can improve older adults' cognition. The current study takes an exploratory approach to compare the feasibility and efficacy of online executive function training and low-intensity aerobic exercise for improving cognitive and psychosocial functioning in healthy older adults.

Method: Following a standard pretest-training-posttest protocol, 40 older adults (aged 65 and above) were randomly assigned to an executive function or a physical training group. A battery of cognitive and psychosocial outcome measures were administered before and after training. During the 10 weeks of self-guided training at home (25-30 min/day, 4 days/week), the executive function training group practiced a set of adaptive online executive function tasks designed by Lumos Labs, whereas the physical training group completed an adaptive Digital Video Disc (DVD)-based low-intensity aerobic exercise program.

Results: Training transfer effects were limited. Relative to low-intensity aerobic exercise, executive function training yielded cognitive improvement on the 64-card Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST-64), a general executive function measure. Depression and stress levels dropped following both training programs, but this could be driven by decreased stress or excitement in performing the tasks over time.

Discussion: The results revealed limited cognitive benefits of the online executive function training program, specifically to a near transfer test of general executive control. Importantly, the current study supports the feasibility of home-based self-guided executive function and low-intensity physical training with healthy older adults.

Keywords: Lumosity; aerobic exercise; aging; executive function training; executive functions; psychosocial functions.

Copyright © 2020 Yang, Gallant, Wilkins and Dyson.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
The proportional training gain scores in the 64-card Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST-64). Positive values mean the proportional score increases at posttest relative to pretest, with higher values meaning larger gains. Error bars refer to the standard errors of the means. Trials (first category) = trials to complete the first category.

References

    1. Angevaren M., Vanhees L., Wendel-Vos W., Verhaar H. J. J., Aufdemkampe G., Aleman A., et al. (2007). Intensity, but not duration, of physical activities is related to cognitive function. Eur. J. Cardiovasc. Prev. Rehabil 14 825–830. 10.1097/HJR.0b013e3282ef995b
    1. Ansai J. H., Rebelatto J. R. (2015). Effect of two physical exercise protocols on cognition and depressive symptoms in oldest-old people: a randomized controlled trial. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 15 1127–1134. 10.1111/ggi.12411
    1. Ball K., Berch D. B., Helmers K. F., Jobe J. B., Leveck M. D., Marsiske M., et al. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288 2271–2281. 10.1001/jama.288.18.2271
    1. Ballesteros S., Prieto A., Mayas J., Toril P., Pita C., Ponce de León L., et al. (2015). Corrigendum: brain training with non-action video games enhances aspects of cognition in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:82. 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00082
    1. Benedict H. B. R., Hopkins B. J. (2020). Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised. Available online at: (accessed September 2, 2020).
    1. Berryman N., Bherer L., Nadeau S., Lauzière S., Lehr L., Bobeuf F., et al. (2014). Multiple roads lead to Rome: combined high-intensity aerobic and strength training vs. gross motor activities leads to equivalent improvement in executive functions in a cohort of healthy older adults. AGE 36:9710. 10.1007/s11357-014-9710-8
    1. Bridle C., Spanjers K., Patel S., Atherton N. M., Lamb S. E. (2012). Effect of exercise on depression severity in older people: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Br. J. Psychiatry. 201 180–185. 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.095174
    1. CSTF (2020). Fitness Canada. Canadian Standardized Test of Fitness (CSTF) : Operations Manual. Fitness and Amateur Sport; 1986, 3 Edn Available online at: (accessed September 2, 2020).
    1. Edwards J. D., Fausto B. A., Tetlow A. M., Corona R. T., Valdés E. G. (2018). Systematic review and meta-analyses of useful field of view cognitive training. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84 72–91. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.004
    1. Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A.-G., Buchner A. (2007). G∗Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39 175–191. 10.3758/BF03193146
    1. Folstein M. F., Folstein S. E., McHugh P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatric Res. 12 189–198. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
    1. Grady C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13 491–505. 10.1038/nrn3256
    1. Green C. S., Bavelier D., Kramer A. F., Vinogradov S., Ansorge U., Ball K., et al. (2019). Improving methodological standards in behavioral interventions for cognitive enhancement. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 3 2–29. 10.1007/s41465-018-0115-y
    1. Hardy J. L., Drescher D., Sarker K., Kellett G., Scanlon M. (2011). Enhancing visual attention and working memory with a Web-based cognitive training program. Mensa Res. J. 2011 709–744.
    1. Hillman C. H., Erickson K. I., Kramer A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9 58–65. 10.1038/nrn2298
    1. Hindin S. B., Zelinski E. M. (2012). Extended practice and aerobic exercise interventions benefit untrained cognitive outcomes in older adults: a meta-analysis. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60 136–141. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03761.x
    1. Jaeggi S. M., Studer-Luethi B., Buschkuehl M., Su Y.-F., Jonides J., Perrig W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning −– implications for training and transfer. Intelligence 38 625–635. 10.1016/j.intell.2010.09.001
    1. Jost K., Bryck R. L., Vogel E. K., Mayr U. (2011). Are old adults just like low working memory young adults? Filtering efficiency and age differences in visual working memory. Cereb Cortex. 21 1147–1154. 10.1093/cercor/bhq185
    1. Kable J. W., Caulfield M. K., Falcone M., McConnell M., Bernardo L., Parthasarathi T., et al. (2017). No effect of commercial cognitive training on brain activity, choice behavior, or cognitive performance. J. Neurosci. 37 7390–7402. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2832-16.2017
    1. Karr J. E., Areshenkoff C. N., Rast P., Garcia-Barrera M. A. (2014). An empirical comparison of the therapeutic benefits of physical exercise and cognitive training on the executive functions of older adults: a meta-analysis of controlled trials. Neuropsychology 28 829–845. 10.1037/neu0000101
    1. Kelly M. E., Loughrey D., Lawlor B. A., Robertson I. H., Walsh C., Brennan S. (2014). The impact of cognitive training and mental stimulation on cognitive and everyday functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 15 28–43. 10.1016/j.arr.2014.02.004
    1. Kobayashi-Cuya K. E., Sakurai R., Sakuma N., Sakuma N., Suzuki H., Yasunaga M., et al. (2018). Hand dexterity, not handgrip strength, is associated with executive function in Japanese community-dwelling older adults: a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatr. 18:192. 10.1186/s12877-018-0880-6
    1. Kongs K. S., Thompson L. L., Iverson G. L., Heaton R. K. (2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test 64 Card Version (WCST-64). Psychological Assessment Research. Available online at: (accessed September 2, 2020).
    1. Kramer A. F., Colcombe S., Erickson K., Belopolsky A., McAuley E., Cohen N. J., et al. (2002). Effects of aerobic fitness training on human cortical function. J. Mol. Neurosci. 19 227–231. 10.1007/s12031-002-0038-y
    1. Kriska A. M., Pereira M., Fitzgerald S., Gregg S. (1997). Modifiable activity questionnaire in: a collection of physical activity questionnaires for health-related research. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 29(Suppl. 6) 73–78.
    1. Krucoff C. (1990). WHY JANE’S FONDA EXERCISE. Washington Post. Available online at: (accessed September 2, 2020).
    1. Lampit A., Hallock H., Valenzuela M. (2014). Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med. 11:1001756. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756
    1. Lawton M. P., Brody E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist 9 179–186.
    1. Lovibond P. F., Lovibond S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav. Res. Therapy 33 335–343. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U
    1. Lustig C., Hasher L., Zacks R. T. (2007). “Inhibitory deficit theory: recent developments in a “new view”,” in Inhibition in Cognition, eds Gorfein D. S., MacLeod C. M. (Worcester, MA: American Psychological Association; ), 145–162. 10.1037/11587-008
    1. Melby-Lervåg M., Redick T. S., Hulme C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer”: evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol. Sci. 11 512–534. 10.1177/1745691616635612
    1. Navon D. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9 353–383. 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
    1. Nielsen T.-T., Møller T. K., Andersen L. L., Zebis M. K., Hansen P. R., Krustrup P. (2019). Feasibility and health effects of a 15-week combined exercise programme for sedentary elderly: a randomised controlled trial. BioMed Res. Int. 2019:3081029.
    1. Northey J. M., Cherbuin N., Pumpa K. L., Smee D. J., Rattray B. (2018). Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 52 154–160. 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096587
    1. Osman A., Wong J. L., Bagge C. L., Freedenthal S., Gutierrez P. M., Lozano G. (2012). The depression anxiety stress scales—21 (DASS-21): further examination of dimensions, scale reliability, and correlates. J. Clin. Psychol. 68 1322–1338. 10.1002/jclp.21908
    1. Owen A. M., Hampshire A., Grahn J. A., Stenton R., Dajani S., Burns A. S., et al. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature 465 775–778. 10.1038/nature09042
    1. Park D. C., Lodi-Smith J., Drew L., Haber S., Hebrank A., Bischof G. N., et al. (2014). The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive function in older adults: the synapse project. Psychol. Sci. 25 103–112. 10.1177/0956797613499592
    1. Rebok G. W., Ball K., Guey L. T., Jones R. N., Kim H.-Y., King J. W., et al. (2014). Ten-year effects of the advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly cognitive training trial on cognition and everyday functioning in older adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 62 16–24. 10.1111/jgs.12607
    1. Simons D. J., Boot W. R., Charness N., Gathercole S. E., Chabris C. F., Hambrick D. Z., et al. (2016). Do “Brain-Training” programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 17 103–186. 10.1177/1529100616661983
    1. Smith P. J., Blumenthal J. A., Hoffman B. M., Cooper H., Strauman T. A., Welsh-Bohmer K., et al. (2010). Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom Med. 72 239–252. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d14633
    1. Sprague B. N., Freed S. A., Webb C. E., Phillips C. B., Hyun J., Ross L. A. (2019). The impact of behavioral interventions on cognitive function in healthy older adults: a systematic review. Ageing Res. Rev. 52 32–52. 10.1016/j.arr.2019.04.002
    1. Stine-Morrow E. A. L., Parisi J. M., Morrow D. G., Park D. C. (2008). The effects of an engaged lifestyle on cognitive vitality: a field experiment. Psychol. Aging 23 778–786. 10.1037/a0014341
    1. Stroop J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18 643–662.
    1. Swain R. A., Berggren K. L., Kerr A. L., Patel A., Peplinski C., Sikorski A. M. (2012). On Aerobic exercise and behavioral and neural plasticity. Brain Sci. 2 709–744. 10.3390/brainsci2040709
    1. Tse A. C. Y., Wong T. W. L., Lee P. H. (2015). Effect of low-intensity exercise on physical and cognitive health in older adults: a systematic review. Sports Med. Open 1:34. 10.1186/s40798-015-0034-8
    1. Van Doorn J., van den Bergh D., Bohm U., Dablander F., Derks K., Draws T., et al. (2019). The JASP guidelines for conducting and reporting a bayesian analysis. PsyArXiv. [Preprint]. 10.31234/
    1. Verhaeghen P. (2011). Aging and executive control: reports of a demise greatly exaggerated. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20 174–180. 10.1177/0963721411408772
    1. Warburton D. E. R., Bredin S. S. D., Jamnik V. K., Gledhill N. (2011). Validation of the PAR-Q+ and ePARmed-X+. Health Fit. J. Can. 4 38–46. 10.14288/hfjc.v4i2.151
    1. Webb S. L., Loh V., Lampit A., Bateman J. E., Birney D. P. (2018). Meta-analysis of the effects of computerized cognitive training on executive functions: a cross-disciplinary taxonomy for classifying outcome cognitive factors. Neuropsychol Rev. 28 232–250. 10.1007/s11065-018-9374-8
    1. Wechsler D. (1981). The psychometric tradition: developing the wechsler adult intelligence scale. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 6, 82–85. 10.1016/0361-476X(81)90035-7
    1. Wilkinson A. J., Yang L. (2012). Plasticity of inhibition in older adults: Retest practice and transfer effects. Psychol. Aging 27 606–615. 10.1037/a0025926
    1. Wilkinson A. J., Yang L. (2015). Cognitivie Plasticity. The Encyclopedia of Adulthood and Aging. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc, 1–5.
    1. Wilkinson A. J., Yang L. (2016a). Inhibition plasticity in older adults: practice and transfer effects using a multiple task approach. Neural Plasticity 2016 1–12.
    1. Wilkinson A. J., Yang L. (2016b). Long-term maintenance of inhibition training effects in older adults: 1- and 3-year follow-up. J. Gerontol Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 71 622–629.
    1. Wolinsky F. D., Mahncke H. W., Weg M. W. V., Martin R., Unverzagt F. W., Ball K. K., et al. (2009). The ACTIVE cognitive training interventions and the onset of and recovery from suspected clinical depression. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 64B 577–585. 10.1093/geronb/gbp061
    1. Yang L. (2011). Practice-oriented retest learning as the basic form of cognitive plasticity of the aging brain. J. Aging Res. 2001:407074. 10.4061/2011/407074
    1. Yang L., Krampe R. T., Baltes P. B. (2006). Basic forms of cognitive plasticity extended into the oldest-old: retest learning, age, and cognitive functioning. Psychol. Aging 21 372–378. 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.372
    1. Yang L., Reed M., Russo F. A., Wilkinson A. A. (2009). New look at retest learning in older adults: learning in the absence of item-specific effects. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 64B 470–473. 10.1093/geronb/gbp040
    1. Younan B. (2018). Cognitive functioning differences between physically active and sedentary older adults. J. Alzheimers Dis. Rep. 2 93–101. 10.3233/ADR-180053
    1. Zelinski E. M. (2009). Far transfer in cognitive training of older adults. Restorat. Neurol. Neurosci. 27 455–471. 10.3233/RNN-2009-0495

Source: PubMed

3
Abonner