Effects of direction and index of difficulty on aiming movements after stroke

Paola Ribeiro Coqueiro, Sandra Maria Sbeghen Ferreira de Freitas, Cassandra Mendes Assunção e Silva, Sandra Regina Alouche, Paola Ribeiro Coqueiro, Sandra Maria Sbeghen Ferreira de Freitas, Cassandra Mendes Assunção e Silva, Sandra Regina Alouche

Abstract

Background: Brain hemispheres play different roles in the control of aiming movements that are impaired after unilateral stroke. It is not clear whether those roles are influenced by the direction and the difficulty of the task.

Objective: To evaluate the influence of direction and index of difficulty (ID) of the task on performance of ipsilesional aiming movements after unilateral stroke.

Methods: Ten individuals with right hemisphere stroke, ten with left hemisphere stroke, and ten age- and gender-matched controls performed the aiming movements on a digitizing tablet as fast as possible. Stroke individuals used their ipsilesional arm. The direction (ipsilateral or contralateral), size (0.8 or 1.6 cm), and distance (9 or 18 cm) of the targets, presented on a monitor, were manipulated and determined to be of different ID (3.5, 4.5, and 5.5). Results. Individuals with right hemisphere lesion were more sensitive to ID of the task, affecting planning and final position accuracy. Left hemisphere lesion generated slower and less smooth movements and was more influenced by target distance. Contralateral movements and higher ID increased planning demands and hindered movement execution.

Conclusion: Right and left hemisphere damages are differentially influenced by task constraints which suggest their complementary roles in the control of aiming movements.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
(a) Participant's position. (b) Time course of each trial. (c) Targets position (gray circles) at a distance of 18 cm and 45° to the right and left of the initial position (IP, white circle).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Trajectory profile from a representative participant of each group with the right (R) and left (L) arms for the two targets (0.8 and 1.6 cm) and two distances from the initial position (9 and 18 cm) used in the experiment.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean reaction time (a) and mean time to peak of velocity (b) for the right (RC) and left (LC) arms of control group and ipsilesional arms of participants with right (RCVA) and left (LCVA) cerebrovascular accident for ipsilateral and contralateral movements for each index of difficulty (I = 3.5; II = 4.5; III = 4.5; IV = 5.5). Bars indicate standard error.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Mean values of movement time (a), peak of velocity (b), movement units (c), and variable error (d) for the right (RC) and left (LC) arms of control group and ipsilesional arms of participants with right (RCVA) and left (LCVA) cerebrovascular accident for ipsilateral and contralateral movements for each index of difficulty (I = 3.5; II = 4.5; III = 4.5; IV = 5.5). Bars indicate standard error.

References

    1. Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Ipsilesional motor deficits following stroke reflect hemispheric specializations for movement control. Brain. 2007;130(8):2146–2158.
    1. Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Hemispheric specialization and functional impact of ipsilesional deficits in movement coordination and accuracy. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(13):2953–2966.
    1. Sainburg RL, Schaefer SY. Interlimb differences in control of movement extent. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;92(3):1374–1383.
    1. Ketcham CJ, Rodriguez TM, Zihlman KA. Targeted aiming movements are compromised in nonaffected limb of persons with stroke. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2007;21(5):388–397.
    1. Winstein CJ, Pohl PS. Effects of unilateral brain damage on the control of goal-directed hand movements. Experimental Brain Research. 1995;105(1):163–174.
    1. Harrington DL, Haaland KY. Hemispheric specialization for motor sequencing: abnormalities in levels of programming. Neuropsychologia. 1991;29(2):147–163.
    1. Haaland KY, Harrington DL. Hemispheric asymmetry of movement. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1996;6(6):796–800.
    1. Henry FM, Rogers DE. Increased response latency for complicated movements and a “memory drum” theory of neuromotor reaction. Research Quartely. 1960;31:448–458.
    1. Alouche SR, Sant'anna GN, Biagioni G, Ribeiro-do-Valle LE. Influence of cueing on the preparation and execution of untrained and trained complex motor responses. Brazilian Journal Of Medical and Biological Research. 2012;45(5):425–435.
    1. Munro H, Plumb MS, Wilson AD, Williams JHG, Mon-Williams M. The effect of distance on reaction time in aiming movements. Experimental Brain Research. 2007;183(2):249–257.
    1. Lajoie JM, Franks IM. Response programming as a function of accuracy and complexity: evidence from latency and kinematic measures. Human Movement Science. 1997;16(4):485–505.
    1. Fitts PM. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1954;47(6):381–391.
    1. Christe B, Burkhard PR, Pegna AJ, Mayer E, Hauert CA. Clinical assessment of motor function: a processes oriented instrument based on a speed-accuracy trade-off paradigm. Behavioural Neurology. 2007;18(1):19–29.
    1. Fowler B, Duck T, Mosher M, Mathieson B. The coordination of bimanual aiming movements: evidence for progressive desynchronization. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A. 1991;43(2):205–221.
    1. Smits-Engelsman B, van Galen G, Duysens J. The breakdown of Fitts’ law in rapid, reciprocal aiming movements. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;145(2):222–230.
    1. Duarte M, Freitas SM. Speed-accuracy trade-off in voluntary postural movements. Motor Control. 2005;9(2):180–196.
    1. Dounskaia N. The internal model and the leading joint hypothesis: implications for control of multi-joint movements. Experimental Brain Research. 2005;166(1):1–16.
    1. Goble JA, Zhang Y, Shimansky Y, Sharma S, Dounskaia NV. Directional biases reveal utilization of arm’s biomechanical properties for optimization of motor behavior. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98(3):1240–1252.
    1. Dounskaia N, Ketcham C, Stelmach G. Commonalities and differences in control of various drawing movements. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;146(1):11–25.
    1. Dounskaia N, Wisleder D, Johnson T. Influence of biomechanical factors on substructure of pointing movements. Experimental Brain Research. 2005;164(4):505–516.
    1. Haaland KY, Schaefer SY, Knight RT, et al. Ipsilesional trajectory control is related to contralesional arm paralysis after left hemisphere damage. Experimental Brain Research. 2009;196(2):195–204.
    1. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113.
    1. Brucki S, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bertolucci PHF, Okamoto IH. Sugestões para o uso do mini-exame do estado mental no Brasil. Arquivo de Neuropisiquiatria. 2003;61(3):777–781.
    1. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. ‘Mini mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12(3):189–198.
    1. Maki T, Quagliato EMAB, Cacho EWA, et al. Estudo de Confiabilidade da Aplicação da Escala de Fugl-Meyer no Brasil. The Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy. 2006;10:177–183.
    1. Fugl-Meyer AR, Jaasko L, Leyman I. The post stroke hemiplegic patient. I. A method for evaluation of physical performance. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine. 1975;7(1):13–31.
    1. Carson RG, Chua R, Goodman D, Byblow WD, Elliott D. The preparation of aiming movements. Brain and Cognition. 1995;28(2):133–154.
    1. Barthelemy S, Boulinguez P. Manual reaction time asymmetries in human subjects: the role of movement planning and attention. Neuroscience Letters. 2001;315(1-2):41–44.
    1. Adam JJ, Müskens R, Hoonhorst S, Pratt J, Fischer MH. Left hand, but not right hand, reaching is sensitive to visual context. Experimental Brain Research. 2010;203(1):227–232.
    1. Haaland KY, Prestopnik JL, Knight RT, Lee RR. Hemispheric asymmetries for kinematic and positional aspects of reaching. Brain. 2004;127(5):1145–1158.
    1. Haaland KY, Harrington D. The role of the hemispheres in closed loop movements. Brain and Cognition. 1989;9(2):158–180.
    1. Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Dissociation of initial trajectory and final position errors during visuomotor adaptation following unilateral stroke. Brain Research. 2009;1298:78–91.
    1. Hore J, Watts S, Tweed D. Prediction and compensation by an internal model for back forces during finger opening in an overarm throw. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;82(3):1187–1197.
    1. Sainburg RL. Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;142(2):241–258.
    1. Rhodes BJ, Bullock D, Verwey WB, Averbeck BB, Page MP. Learning and production of movement sequences: behavioral, neurophysiological, and modeling perspectives. Human Movement Science. 2004;23(5):699–746.
    1. Klapp ST. Motor response programming during simple and choice reaction time: the role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1995;21(5):1015–1027.
    1. Dounskaia N. Control of human limb movements: the leading joint hypothesis and its practical applications. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews. 2010;38(4):201–208.
    1. Mutha PK, Sainburg RL. Control of velocity and position in single joint movements. Human Movement Science. 2007;26(6):808–823.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonner