Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder

Alice P Jones, Francesca G E Happé, Francesca Gilbert, Stephanie Burnett, Essi Viding, Alice P Jones, Francesca G E Happé, Francesca Gilbert, Stephanie Burnett, Essi Viding

Abstract

Background: Empathy dysfunction is one of the hallmarks of psychopathy, but it is also sometimes thought to characterise autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Individuals with either condition can appear uncaring towards others. This study set out to compare and contrast directly boys with psychopathic tendencies and boys with ASD on tasks assessing aspects of affective empathy and cognitive perspective taking. The main aim of the study was to assess whether a distinct profile of empathy deficits would emerge for boys with psychopathic tendencies and ASD, and whether empathy deficits would be associated with conduct problems in general, rather than psychopathic tendencies or ASD specifically.

Methods: Four groups of boys aged between 9 and 16 years (N = 96) were compared: 1) psychopathic tendencies, 2) ASD, 3) conduct problems and 4) comparison. Tasks were included to probe attribution of emotions to self, empathy for victims of aggression and cognitive perspective-taking ability.

Results: Boys with psychopathic tendencies had a profile consistent with dysfunctional affective empathy. They reported experiencing less fear and less empathy for victims of aggression than comparison boys. Their cognitive perspective-taking abilities were not statistically significantly different from those of comparison boys. In contrast, boys with ASD had difficulties with tasks requiring cognitive perspective taking, but reported emotional experiences and victim empathy that were in line with comparison boys. Boys with conduct problems did not differ from comparison boys, suggesting that the affective empathy deficit seen in boys with psychopathic tendencies was specific to that group, rather than common to all boys with conduct problems.

Conclusions: Although both groups can appear uncaring, our findings suggest that the affective/information processing correlates of psychopathic tendencies and ASD are quite different. Psychopathic tendencies are associated with difficulties in resonating with other people's distress, whereas ASD is characterised by difficulties in knowing what other people think.

© 2010 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry © 2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Bar chart of total scores for outcome values: a) vignettes of reactive aggression, b) vignettes of instrumental aggression (* indicates significant differences between groups, p < .05 that remain after co-varying for age and IQ). PT = psychopathic tendencies, CP = conduct problems, C = comparison, ASD = autism spectrum disorder

References

    1. Abell F, Happé F, Frith U, Frith C. Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Journal of Cognitive Development. 2000;15:1–20.
    1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th edn. Washington, DC: Author; 1994.
    1. Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous X, Warden D. Cognitive and affective perspective-taking in conduct-disordered children high and low on callous-unemotional traits. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health. 2008;2:16.
    1. Aniskiewicz A. Autonomic components of vicarious conditioning and psychopathy. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1979;35:60–67.
    1. Asperger H. Die ‘Autistischen Psychopathen’ im Kindesalter [Autistic Psychopathy of Childhood, trans. U. Frith (1991)] In: Frith U, editor. Autism and Asperger syndrome. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1944. pp. 37–92.
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright C. The Empathy Quotient (EQ). An investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2004;34:163–175.
    1. Blair R. Psychophysiological responsiveness to the distress of others in children with autism. Personality & Individual Differences. 1999;26:477–485.
    1. Blair RJ. Fine cuts of empathy and the amygdala: Dissociable deficits in psychopathy and autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2008;61:157–170.
    1. Blair R, Colledge E, Murray L, Mitchell D. A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001;29:491–498.
    1. Blair RJ, Jones L, Clark F, Smith M. The psychopathic individual: A lack of responsiveness to distress cues? Psychophysiology. 1997;34:192–198.
    1. Blair RJR, Sellars C, Strickland I, Clark F, Williams AO, Smith M, et al. Emotion attributions in the psychopath. Personality and Individual Differences. 1995;19:431–437.
    1. Blair RJR, Sellars C, Strickland I, Clark F, Williams A, Smith M, et al. Theory of mind in the psychopath. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry. 1996;7:15–25.
    1. Blair RJR, Viding E. Psychopathy. In: Rutter M, Bishop D, Pine D, Scott S, Stevenson J, Taylor E, Thapar A, editors. Rutter’s child and adolescent psychiatry. 5th edn. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 2008. pp. 852–863. Chapter 51.
    1. Boldizar JP, Perry DG, Perry LC. Outcome values and aggression. Child Development. 1989;60:571–579.
    1. Bowler DM. ‘Theory of mind’ in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1992;33:877–893.
    1. Burnett S, Bird G, Moll J, Frith C, Blakemore SJ. Development during adolescence of the neural processing of social emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:1736–1750.
    1. Campbell R, Lawrence K, Mandy W, Mitra C, Jeyakuma L, Skuse D. Meanings in motion and faces: Developmental associations between the processing of intention from geometrical animations and gaze detection accuracy. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:99–118.
    1. Castelli F, Frith C, Happé F, Frith U. Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain. 2002;125:1839–1849.
    1. Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, Frost AD, Vassallo S, Bunn P, Hunter K, et al. Learning to ‘talk the talk: The relationship of psychopathic traits to deficits in empathy across childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:599–606.
    1. Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;44:113–126.
    1. De Vignemont F, Singer T. The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:435–441.
    1. Dolan M, Fullam R. Theory of mind and mentalizing ability in antisocial personality disorders with and without psychopathy. Psychological Medicine. 2004;34:1093–1102.
    1. Dolan M, Fullam R. Face affect recognition deficits in personality-disordered offenders: Association with psychopathy. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36:1563–1539.
    1. Essau CA, Sasagawa S, Frick PJ. Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment. 2006;13:454–469.
    1. Frick PJ. The Inventory of Callous/Unemotional traits: An unpublished rating scale. New Orleans: The University of New Orleans; 2003.
    1. Frick PJ, Hare RD. Antisocial Process Screening Device. Toronto: Multi Health Systems; 2001.
    1. Frith U. Autism and Asperger syndrome. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1991.
    1. Gadow KD, Sprafkin J. Adolescent Symptom Inventory IV. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus; 1997.
    1. Gadow KD, Sprafkin J. Child Symptom Inventory IV. 4th edn. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus; 2002.
    1. Hall JA, Herzberger SD, Skrowronski KJ. Outcome expectancies and outcome values as predictors of children’s aggression. Aggressive Behavior. 1998;24:439–454.
    1. Hare RD. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist – Revised. 2nd edn. Toronto, Ontario: Multi-Health Systems; 2003.
    1. Hughes C, Adalm A, Happé F, Jackson J, Taylor A, Caspi A. Good test–retest reliability for standard and advanced false-belief tasks across a wide range of abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41:483–440.
    1. Jones AP, Larsson H, Ronald A, Rjisdijk F, Busfield P, McMillan A, et al. Phenotypic and aetiological relationships between psychopathic tendencies, autistic traits, and emotion attribution. Criminal Justice and Behavior. 2009;36:1198–1212.
    1. Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Skeem JL, Marsee MA, Cruise K, Munoz LC, et al. Assessing callous-unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: Validation of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 2008;31:241–252.
    1. Knight RA, Silverstein SM. A process-oriented approach for averting confounds resulting from general performance deficiencies in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:31–39.
    1. Miller GA, Chapman JP. Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:40–48.
    1. Pardini DA, Lochman JE, Frick PJ. Callous/unemotional traits and social-cognitive processes in adjudicated youths. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:364–371.
    1. Richell R, Mitchell D, Newman C, Leonard A, Baron-Cohen S, Blair R. Theory of mind and psychopathy: Can psychopathic individuals read the ‘language of the eyes’? Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:523–526.
    1. Rogers J, Viding E, Blair RJ, Frith U, Happé F. Autism spectrum disorder and psychopathy: Shared cognitive underpinnings or double hit? Psychological Medicine. 2006;36:1789–1798.
    1. Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A. Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37:709–715.
    1. Sigman M, Dissanayake C, Corona R, Espinosa M. Social and cardiac responses of young children with autism. Autism. 2003;7:205–216.
    1. Stevens D, Charman T, Blair R. Recognition of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 2001;162:201–211.
    1. Sullivan K, Zaitchik D, Tager-Flusberg H. Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30:395–402.
    1. Viding E, Simmonds E, Petrides KV, Frederickson N. The contribution of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems to bullying in early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:471–481.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 1999.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonner