Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study

Meghan L Meyer, Robert P Spunt, Elliot T Berkman, Shelley E Taylor, Matthew D Lieberman, Meghan L Meyer, Robert P Spunt, Elliot T Berkman, Shelley E Taylor, Matthew D Lieberman

Abstract

Keeping track of various amounts of social cognitive information, including people's mental states, traits, and relationships, is fundamental to navigating social interactions. However, to date, no research has examined which brain regions support variable amounts of social information processing ("social load"). We developed a social working memory paradigm to examine the brain networks sensitive to social load. Two networks showed linear increases in activation as a function of increasing social load: the medial frontoparietal regions implicated in social cognition and the lateral frontoparietal system implicated in nonsocial forms of working memory. Of these networks, only load-dependent medial frontoparietal activity was associated with individual differences in social cognitive ability (trait perspective-taking). Although past studies of nonsocial load have uniformly found medial frontoparietal activity decreases with increasing task demands, the current study demonstrates these regions do support increasing mental effort when such effort engages social cognition. Implications for the etiology of clinical disorders that implicate social functioning and potential interventions are discussed.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Social working memory task.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Parametric increases in the mentalizing and canonical working memory regions during the delay period as a function of social load level.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Parametric increases in the mentalizing and canonical working memory regions during the probe response period as a function of social load level.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
Regions showing social load dependent increases during the delay period of social working memory trials that correlate with trait-level perspective-taking ability. The lateral view of the brain shows that none of the regions in the frontoparietal canonical working memory network showed parametric activation correlating with perspective-taking scores. The medial view of the brain shows regions in the mentalizing and default-mode network whose parametric activation correlates with perspective-taking scores. This correlation is plotted for MPFC parametric increases by load as a function of perspective-taking scores.

References

    1. Dunbar R. The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol. 1998;6:178–190.
    1. Byrne R, Whiten A. Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. UK: Clarendon Press; 1988.
    1. Humphrey NK. The Social Function of Intellect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1976.
    1. D'Esposito M, Postle BR, Ballard D, Lease J. Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: An event-related fMRI study. Brain Cogn. 1999;41:66–86.
    1. Rypma B, Prabhakaran V, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD. Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory. Neuroimage. 1999;9:216–226.
    1. Wager TD, Smith EE. Neuroimaging studies of working memory: A meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2003;3:255–274.
    1. Kampe KK, Frith CD, Frith U. “Hey John”: Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. J Neurosci. 2003;23:5258–5263.
    1. Mitchell JP, Neil Macrae C, Banaji MR. Forming impressions of people versus inanimate objects: Social-cognitive processing in the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2005;26:251–257.
    1. Saxe R, Wexler A. Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1391–1399.
    1. Greicius MD, Menon V. Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: Uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16:1484–1492.
    1. McKiernan KA, Kaufman JN, Kucera-Thompson J, Binder JR. A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15:394–408.
    1. Metzak P, et al. Constrained principal component analysis reveals functionally connected load-dependent networks involved in multiple stages of working memory. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32:856–871.
    1. Raichle M, Snyder A. A default mode of brain function: A brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1083–1090.
    1. Raichle ME, et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:676–682.
    1. Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim AS. The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:489–510.
    1. Davis M. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44:113–126.
    1. Conway AR, Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:547–552.
    1. Gray JR, Chabris CF, Braver TS. Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:316–322.
    1. Rypma B, D'Esposito M. The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: Effects of memory load and individual differences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999;96:6558–6563.
    1. Mitchell JP, Heatherton TF, Macrae CN. Distinct neural systems subserve person and object knowledge. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:15238–15243.
    1. Underwood B, Moore B. Perspective-taking and altruism. Psychol Bull. 1982;91:143–173.
    1. Chaiken S, Trope Y. Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology. New York: Guilford Press; 1999.
    1. Evans JS. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:255–278.
    1. Smith E, DeCoster J. Dual process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2000;4:108–131.
    1. Lieberman MD, Gaunt R, Gilbert D, Trope Y. Reflection and reflexion: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2002;34:199–249.
    1. Strack F, Deutsch R. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2004;8:220–247.
    1. Brunet E, Sarfati Y, Hardy-Baylé MC, Decety J. A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. Neuroimage. 2000;11:157–166.
    1. Fletcher PC, et al. Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition. 1995;57:109–128.
    1. Walter H, et al. Understanding intentions in social interaction: The role of the anterior paracingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16:1854–1863.
    1. Wimmer H, Perner J. Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition. 1983;13:103–128.
    1. Sommer M, et al. Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning. Neuroimage. 2007;35:1378–1384.
    1. Kelley WM, et al. Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:785–794.
    1. Semendeferi K, Armstrong E, Schleicher A, Zilles K, Van Hoesen GW. Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: A comparative study of area 10. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2001;114:224–241.
    1. Powell JL, Lewis PA, Dunbar RI, García-Fiñana M, Roberts N. Orbital prefrontal cortex volume correlates with social cognitive competence. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:3554–3562.
    1. Lewis PA, Rezaie R, Brown R, Roberts N, Dunbar RI. Ventromedial prefrontal volume predicts understanding of others and social network size. Neuroimage. 2011;57:1624–1629.
    1. Couture SM, Penn DL, Roberts DL. The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: A review. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(Suppl 1):S44–S63.
    1. Goldman-Rakic PS. Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6:348–357.
    1. Pickup GJ, Frith CD. Theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia: Symptomatology, severity and specificity. Psychol Med. 2001;31:207–220.
    1. Amir N, Bomyea J. Working memory capacity in generalized social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120:504–509.
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition. 1985;21:37–46.
    1. Dawson G, Fernald M. Perspective-taking ability and its relationship to the social behavior of autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord. 1987;17:487–498.
    1. Bennetto L, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Dev. 1996;67:1816–1835.
    1. Ozonoff S, Strayer DL. Further evidence of intact working memory in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:257–263.
    1. Russell J, Jarrold C, Henry L. Working memory in children with autism and with moderate learning difficulties. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996;37:673–686.
    1. Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ. The profile of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:21–29.
    1. Daneman M, Carpenter P. Individual differences in working memory and reading. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav. 1980;19:450–466.
    1. Geary DC, Hoard MK, Byrd-Craven J, DeSoto MC. Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;88:121–151.
    1. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:6829–6833.
    1. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Shah P. Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:10081–10086.
    1. Klingberg T, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:177–186.
    1. Veltman DJ, Rombouts SA, Dolan RJ. Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: An fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2003;18:247–256.
    1. Bower G. Cognitive psychology: An introduction. In: Estes W, editor. Handbook of Learning and Cognitive Processes. Vol 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1975. pp. 25–80.
    1. Dumas J, Johnson M, Lynch A. Likableness, familiarity, and frequency of 844 person-descriptive words. Pers Individ Dif. 2002;32:523–531.
    1. Wager TD, Nichols TE. Optimization of experimental design in fMRI: A general framework using a genetic algorithm. Neuroimage. 2003;18:293–309.
    1. Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;17:143–155.
    1. Funahashi S, Bruce S, Goldman-Rakic P. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1989;61:1–19.
    1. Fuster JM. Unit activity in prefrontal cortex during delayed-response performance: Neuronal correlates of transient memory. J Neurophysiol. 1973;36:61–78.
    1. Huijbers W, Pennartz CM, Rubin DC, Daselaar SM. Imagery and retrieval of auditory and visual information: Neural correlates of successful and unsuccessful performance. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1730–1740.
    1. Pessoa L, Gutierrez E, Bandettini P, Ungerleider L. Neural correlates of visual working memory: fMRI amplitude predicts task performance. Neuron. 2002;35:975–987.
    1. Rosenkilde CE, Bauer RH, Fuster JM. Single cell activity in ventral prefrontal cortex of behaving monkeys. Brain Res. 1981;209:375–394.
    1. Watanabe M. Prefrontal unit activity during delayed conditional Go/No-Go discrimination in the monkey. I. Relation to the stimulus. Brain Res. 1986;382:1–14.
    1. Cox RW. AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res. 1996;29:162–173.

Source: PubMed

3
Tilaa