Altered auditory and multisensory temporal processing in autism spectrum disorders

Leslie D Kwakye, Jennifer H Foss-Feig, Carissa J Cascio, Wendy L Stone, Mark T Wallace, Leslie D Kwakye, Jennifer H Foss-Feig, Carissa J Cascio, Wendy L Stone, Mark T Wallace

Abstract

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are characterized by deficits in social reciprocity and communication, as well as by repetitive behaviors and restricted interests. Unusual responses to sensory input and disruptions in the processing of both unisensory and multisensory stimuli also have been reported frequently. However, the specific aspects of sensory processing that are disrupted in ASD have yet to be fully elucidated. Recent published work has shown that children with ASD can integrate low-level audiovisual stimuli, but do so over an extended range of time when compared with typically developing (TD) children. However, the possible contributions of altered unisensory temporal processes to the demonstrated changes in multisensory function are yet unknown. In the current study, unisensory temporal acuity was measured by determining individual thresholds on visual and auditory temporal order judgment (TOJ) tasks, and multisensory temporal function was assessed through a cross-modal version of the TOJ task. Whereas no differences in thresholds for the visual TOJ task were seen between children with ASD and TD, thresholds were higher in ASD on the auditory TOJ task, providing preliminary evidence for impairment in auditory temporal processing. On the multisensory TOJ task, children with ASD showed performance improvements over a wider range of temporal intervals than TD children, reinforcing prior work showing an extended temporal window of multisensory integration in ASD. These findings contribute to a better understanding of basic sensory processing differences, which may be critical for understanding more complex social and cognitive deficits in ASD, and ultimately may contribute to more effective diagnostic and interventional strategies.

Keywords: audiovisual; autism; cross-modal integration; multisensory; sensory processing; temporal binding.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Task design. (A) Visual TOJ task. (B) Auditory TOJ task. (C) Multisensory TOJ task. In multisensory conditions, two circles are presented sequentially above and below the central fixation point. One beep is always presented simultaneously with the first circle, whereas the second beep is presented with a varied delay (0–500 ms) from the onset of the second circle. See text for additional detail.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Threshold values for visual and auditory temporal order judgment tasks. Children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) show similar thresholds to typically developing children for the visual TOJ task (A). However, children with ASD show significantly larger thresholds for the auditory TOJ task (B). (*p < 0.05). Note that each of these threshold values was confirmed via a validation procedure (see text for additional detail). Error bars represent standard error of the mean (SEM).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Performance accuracy (A) and response time (B) for simultaneous multisensory trials (i. e., multisensory delay = 0) compared to visual-only trials. Error bars represent standard error of the mean (SEM).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Percent improvement in accuracy relative to visual-only performance as a function of multisensory delay. Whereas typically developing children show improvements for short delays (i.e., 50–150 ms), children with ASD show improvements for both moderate and short delays (i.e., 0–300 ms). The solid line indicates continuous significant (p < 0.05) differences from zero (i.e., the multisensory temporal window). Error bars represent standard error of the mean (SEM).
Figure 5
Figure 5
Improvement in response times relative to visual-only performance as a function of multisensory delay. Whereas typically developing children show improvements for short delays (i.e., 0–200 ms), children with ASD show improvements for both moderate and short delays (i.e., 0–300 ms). The solid line indicates continuous significant (p < 0.05) differences from zero. Error bars represent standard error of the mean (SEM).

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV-TR. Washington, DC: APA
    1. Ashwin E., Ashwin C., Rhydderch D., Howells J., Baron-Cohen S. (2009). Eagle-eyed visual acuity: an Experimental Investigation of Enhanced Perception in Autism. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 17–21
    1. Bebko J., Weiss J., Demark J., Gomez P. (2006). Discrimination of temporal synchrony in intermodal events by children with autism and children with developmental disabilities without autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 88–98
    1. Bertone A., Mottron L., Jelenic P., Faubert J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain 128, 2430–244010.1093/brain/awh561
    1. Blake R., Turner L., Smoski M., Pozdol S., Stone W. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychol. Sci. 14, 151–157
    1. Bonnel A., Mottron L., Peretz I., Trudel M., Gallun E., Bonnel A. (2003). Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: a signal detection analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 226–235
    1. Brock J., Brown C., Boucher J., Rippon G. (2002). The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 14, 209–224
    1. Cesaroni L., Garber M. (1991). Exploring the experience of autism through firsthand accounts. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 21, 303–313
    1. Dakin S., Frith U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron 48, 497–50710.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.018
    1. Escalante-Mead P., Minshew N., Sweeney J. (2003). Abnormal brain lateralization in high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33, 539–543
    1. Foss-Feig J., Kwakye L., Cascio C., Burnette C., Kadivar H., Stone W., Wallace M. (2010). An extended multisensory temporal binding window in autism spectrum disorders. Exp. Brain Res. 203, 381–38910.1007/s00221-010-2240-4
    1. Ghazanfar A., Schroeder C. (2006). Is neocortex essentially multisensory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 278–285
    1. Gomot M., Bernard F., Davis M., Belmonte M., Ashwin C., Bullmore E., Baron-Cohen S. (2006). Change detection in children with autism: an auditory event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 29, 475–48410.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.027
    1. Hairston W., Burdette J., Flowers D., Wood F., Wallace M. (2005). Altered temporal profile of visual-auditory multisensory interactions in dyslexia. Exp. Brain Res. 166, 474–48010.1007/s00221-005-2387-6
    1. Hairston W., Hodges D., Burdette J., Wallace M. (2006). Auditory enhancement of visual temporal order judgment. Neuroreport 17, 791–79510.1097/01.wnr.0000220141.29413.b4
    1. Herbert M. R., Ziegler D., Deutsch C., O'Brien L., Kennedy D., Filipek P., Bakardjiev A., Hodgson J., Takeoka M., Makris N., Caviness V. (2005). Brain asymmetries in autism and developmental language disorder: a nested whole-brain analysis. Brain 128, 213–22610.1093/brain/awh330
    1. Iarocci G., McDonald J. (2006). Sensory integration and the perceptual experience of persons with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 77–90
    1. Jolliffe T., Baron-Cohen S. (1997). Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the embedded figures test? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 38, 527–534
    1. Kanner L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child 2, 217–250
    1. Keetals M., Vroomen J. (2010). Sound affects the speed of visual processing. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.[Epub ahead of print].10.1037/a0020564
    1. Laasonen M., Service E., Visru V. (2001). Temporal order and processing acuity of visual, auditory, and tactile perception in developmentally dyslexic young adults. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 1, 394–410
    1. Lepisto T., Kujala T., Vanhala R., Alku P., Huotilainen M., Naatanen R. (2005). The discrimination of and orienting to speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism. Brain Res. 1066, 147–15710.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.052
    1. Lepisto T., Silokallio S., Nieminen-von Wendt T., Alku P., Naatanen R., Kujala T. (2006). Auditory perception and attention as reflected by the brain event-related potentials in children with Asperger syndrome. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 2161–217110.1016/j.clinph.2006.06.709
    1. Lord C., Risi S., Lambrecht L., Cook E., Jr., Leventhal B., DiLavore P., Pickles A., Rutter M. (2000). Autism diagnostic observation schedule – generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 30, 205–223
    1. Lord C., Rutter M., LeCouteur A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 659–685
    1. Mongillo E., Irwin J., Whalen D., Klaiman C., Carter A., Schultz R. (2008). Audiovisual processing in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 1349–1358
    1. Morein-Zamir S., Soto-Faraco S., Kingstonek A. (2003). Auditory capture of vision: examining temporal ventriloquism. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 17, 154–163
    1. Nicolson R. I., Fawcett A. J. (2005). Developmental dyslexia, learning and the cerebellum. J. Neural Transm. Suppl. 69, 19–36
    1. O'Neill M., Jones R. (1997). Sensory-perceptual abnormalities in autism: a case for more research? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 27, 283–293
    1. O'Riordan M., Plaisted K., Driver J., Baron-Cohen S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 27, 719–730
    1. Oram-Cardy J., Flagg E., Roberts W., Roberts T. (2005). Delayed mismatch field for speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism. Neuroreport 16, 521–52510.1097/00001756-200504040-00021
    1. Plaisted K., O'Riordan M., Baron-Cohen S. (1998). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 39, 765–775
    1. Rippon G., Brock J., Brown C., Boucher J. (2007). Disordered connectivity in the autistic brain: challenges for the “new psychophysiology”. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 63, 164–172
    1. Roberts T., Khan S., Rey M., Monroe J., Cannon K., Blaskey L., Woldoff S., Qasmieh S., Gandal M., Schmidt G., Zarnow D., Levy S., Edgar J. (2010). MEG detection of delayed auditory evoked responses in autism spectrum disorders: towards an imaging biomarker for autism. Autism Res. 3, 8–18
    1. Rogers S., Ozonoff S. (2005). Annotation: what do we know about sensory dysfunction in autism? A critical review of the empirical evidence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 1255–1268
    1. Rubenstein J., Merzenich M. (2003). Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav. 2, 255–26710.1034/j.1601-183X.2003.00037.x
    1. Russo N., Zecker S., Trommer B., Chen J., Kraus N. (2009). Effects of background noise on cortical encoding of speech in autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 1185–1196
    1. Rutter M., Bailey A., Lord C. (2003). SCQ: Social Communication Questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services
    1. Samson F., Mottron L., Jemel B., Belin P., Ciocca V. (2006). Can spectro-temporal complexity explain the autistic pattern of performance on auditory tasks? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 65–76
    1. Shah A., Frith U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: a research note. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 24, 613–620
    1. Sigman M., Capps L. (1997). Children with Autism: A Developmental Perspective. Cambride, MA: Harvard University Press
    1. Simmons D., Robertson A., McKay L., Toal E., McAleer P., Pollick F. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Res. 49, 2705–273910.1016/j.visres.2009.08.005
    1. Smith E., Bennetto L. (2007). Audiovisual speech integration and lipreading in autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 813–821
    1. Spence C., Squire S. (2003). Multisensory integration: maintaining the perception of synchrony. Curr. Biol. 13, R519–R521
    1. Stroganova T., Nygren G., Tsetlin M., Posikera I., Gillberg C., Elam M., Orekhova E. (2007). Abnormal EEG lateralization in boys with autism. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 1842–1854
    1. Szelag E., Kowalska J., Galkowski T., Poppel E. (2004). Temporal processing deficits in high-functioning children with autism. Br. J. Psychol. 95, 269–282
    1. Talay-Ongan A., Wood K. (2000). Unusual sensory sensitivities in autism: a possible crossroads. Int. J. Disabil. Dev. Educ. 47, 201–212
    1. Tecchio F., Benassi F., Zappasodi F., Gialloreti L., Palermo M., Seri S., Rossini P. (2003). Auditory sensory processing in autism: a magnetoencephalographic study. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 647–654
    1. Van der Smagt M., Van Engeland H., Kemner C. (2007). Brief report: can you see what is not there? Low-level auditory–visual integration in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 2014–2019
    1. Wechsler D. (1999). WASI: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc
    1. Williams E. L., Casanova M. F. (2010). Autism and dyslexia: a spectrum of cognitive styles as defined by minicolumnar morphometry. Med. Hypotheses 74, 59–6210.1016/j.mehy.2009.08.003
    1. Williams J., Massaro D., Peel N., Bosseler A., Suddendorf T. (2004). Visual-auditory integration during speech imitation in autism. Res. Dev. Disabil. 25, 559–575
    1. Woodcock R., McGrew K., Mather N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement, 3rd Edn. Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside Publishing

Source: PubMed

3
Tilaa