First evidence of the feasibility of gaze-contingent attention training for school children with autism

Georgina Powell, Sam V Wass, Jonathan T Erichsen, Susan R Leekam, Georgina Powell, Sam V Wass, Jonathan T Erichsen, Susan R Leekam

Abstract

A number of authors have suggested that attention control may be a suitable target for cognitive training in children with autism spectrum disorder. This study provided the first evidence of the feasibility of such training using a battery of tasks intended to target visual attentional control in children with autism spectrum disorder within school-based settings. Twenty-seven children were recruited and randomly assigned to either training or an active control group. Of these, 19 completed the initial assessment, and 17 (9 trained and 8 control) completed all subsequent training sessions. Training of 120 min was administered per participant, spread over six sessions (on average). Compliance with the training tasks was generally high, and evidence of within-task training improvements was found. A number of untrained tasks to assess transfer of training effects were administered pre- and post-training. Changes in the trained group were assessed relative to an active control group. Following training, significant and selective changes in visual sustained attention were observed. Trend training effects were also noted on disengaging visual attention, but no convincing evidence of transfer was found to non-trained assessments of saccadic reaction time and anticipatory looking. Directions for future development and refinement of these new training techniques are discussed.

Keywords: attention; autism; cognitive training; eye movements.

© The Author(s) 2016.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Schematics showing the pre–post tests that were administered: (a) Examples of the ‘boring’ (top) and ‘interesting’ (bottom) stimuli used in the visual sustained attention task. (b) Illustration of the screen layout for the anticipation task (two-location condition). In the four-location condition, objects were presented in the four corners of the screen. (c) Illustration of screen layout for the overlap condition gap-overlap task. In the baseline condition, the central target disappeared as the lateral target was presented. (Colour version available online. DOI: 10.1177/1362361315617880.)
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Schematics of the four training tasks administered. Dashed rectangles and arrows, which were not visible in the original tasks, indicate objects that were moving on-screen: (a) Task 1 (Butterfly). The butterfly (indicated in red) scrolled from left to right as long as the child looked directly at it, with static and moving (indicated in blue) distractors presented in the child’s peripheral visual field. If the child looked to any of the distractors, they disappeared and the scrolling stopped. (b) Task 2 (FlyMe). The purple character (indicated red) scrolled upwards as long as the child looked directly at it. Static and moving (indicated blue) distractors appeared from the top and bottom of the screen. If the child looked to any of the distractors, the purple object sank towards the bottom of the screen and the distractors disappeared. (c) Task 3 (Stars). A target (indicated red) was presented on-screen along with a number of static and moving (indicated blue) distractors. If the child looked to the target within a time window, (s)he received a reward. Both target and distractors changed between trials. (d) Task 4 (Suspects). A target (indicated red) was presented along with a range of distractors. If the child looked to the target within a time window, they received a reward. Once per block of 12 trials, the target changed. Targets from the previous block (indicated yellow) were presented concurrently with the current target, as distractors. (Colour version available online. DOI: 10.1177/1362361315617880.)
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Data quality comparison based on data from the gap-overlap study: (a–c) data from this study, (d–f) data from a comparison study that used identical procedures, in laboratory settings, with typical infants (Wass et al., 2011). (a) and (d) Histograms showing the duration of usable fragment durations that were present in our data (calculated on a block-by-block basis). Markedly, longer usable fragment durations were obtained in the comparison study. (b) and (e) Histograms showing the precision of our data (calculated on block-by-block basis). Markedly, more precise data were obtained in the comparison study. (c) and (f) Gaze maps of usable gaze data obtained during the trial. (g) A schematic of how images were distributed on the screen during the trials. (Colour version available online. DOI: 10.1177/1362361315617880.)
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Scatterplots showing, participant by participant, how performance on the training tasks changed across the course of the training sessions. Best-fit linear regression lines have been superimposed onto the scatterplots.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Scatterplots showing change in performance in our participants. Individual dots represent individual children. In each case, pre-test performance has been drawn on the x-axis and post-test performance on the y-axis. A 1:1 equivalence line (indicating that performance at pre-test was identical to post-test) has been drawn on each figure. The direction of predicted change following training, based on previous research, has been shown using an arrow in the top-right corner of each graph. (a) Visual sustained attention – first look duration to ‘interesting’ targets. (b) Visual sustained attention – first look duration to ‘boring’ targets. (c) Disengagement latencies. (d) Average saccadic reaction time (gap-overlap task). (e) Anticipations (two-object task). (f) Anticipations (four-object task).

References

    1. Ames C, Fletcher-Watson S. (2010) A review of methods in the study of attention in autism. Developmental Review 30(1): 52–73.
    1. APA (American Psychiatric Association) (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fifth ed). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
    1. Ballieux H, Wass SV, Tomalski P, et al. (in press) Training attention control outside the lab: applying gaze-contingent training to infants from diverse SES backgrounds. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology.
    1. Bedford R, Elsabbagh M, Gliga T, et al. (2012) Precursors to social and communication difficulties in infants at-risk for autism: gaze following and attentional engagement. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42(1): 2208–2218.
    1. Benson V, Castelhano MS, Au-Yeung SK, et al. (2012) Eye movements reveal no immediate ‘WOW’ (‘which one’s weird’) effect in autism spectrum disorder. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 65(6): 1139–1150.
    1. Elison JT, Paterson SJ, Wolff JJ, et al. (2013) White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism. American Journal of Psychiatry 170(8): 899–908.
    1. Elliot CD, Smith P, McUlloch K. (1996) British Ability Scales II. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
    1. Elsabbagh M, Bedford R, Senju A, et al. (2013a) What you see is what you get: contextual modulation of face scanning in typical and atypical development. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9(4): 538–543.
    1. Elsabbagh M, Gliga T, Pickles A, et al. (2013b) The development of face orienting mechanisms in infants at-risk for autism. Behavioural Brain Research 251: 147–154.
    1. Elsabbagh M, Volein A, Holmboe K, et al. (2009) Visual orienting in the early broader autism phenotype: disengagement and facilitation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50(5): 637–642.
    1. Faja S, Aylward E, Bernier R, et al. (2008) Becoming a face expert: a computerized face-training program for high-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Neuropsychology 33(1): 1–24.
    1. Golan O, Baron-Cohen S. (2006) Systemizing empathy: teaching adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism to recognize complex emotions using interactive multimedia. Development and Psychopathology 18(2): 591–617.
    1. Johnson MH. (2010) Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 3rd ed. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
    1. Johnson MH. (2012) Executive function and developmental disorders: the flip side of the coin. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16(9): 454–457.
    1. Jones EJH, Gliga T, Bedford R, et al. (2014) Developmental pathways to autism: a review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 39: 1–33.
    1. Kannass KN, Oakes LM. (2008) The development of attention and its relations to language in infancy and toddlerhood. Journal of Cognition and Development 9(2): 222–246.
    1. Karmiloff-Smith A. (1992) Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.
    1. Karmiloff-Smith A. (2009) Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: rethinking the study of developmental disorders. Developmental Psychology 45(1): 56–63.
    1. Keehn B, Müller RA, Townsend J. (2013) Atypical attentional networks and the emergence of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37(2): 164–183.
    1. Kourkoulou A, Kuhn G, Findlay JM, et al. (2013) Eye movement difficulties in autism spectrum disorder: implications for implicit contextual learning. Autism Research 6(3): 177–189.
    1. Leekam SR, Lopez B, Moore C. (2000) Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology 36(2): 261–273.
    1. Leppänen J, Forssman L, Kaatiala J, et al. (2014) Widely applicable MATLAB routines for automated analysis of saccadic reaction times in infants. Behavior Research Methods 47(2): 538–548.
    1. Orekhova EV, Stroganova TA. (2014) Arousal and attention re-orienting in autism spectrum disorders: evidence from auditory event-related potentials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8: 34.
    1. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. (2007) Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology 58: 1–23.
    1. Razza RA, Martin A, Brooks-Gunn J. (2010) Associations among family environment, sustained attention, and school readiness for low-income children. Developmental Psychology 46(6): 1528–1542.
    1. Reichow B. (2012) Overview of meta-analyses on early intensive behavioral intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42(4): 512–520.
    1. Rose SA, Feldman JF, Jankowski JJ. (2009) A cognitive approach to the development of early language. Child Development 80(1): 134–150.
    1. Rose SA, Feldman JF, Jankowski JJ. (2011) Modeling a cascade of effects: the role of speed and executive functioning in preterm/full-term differences in academic achievement. Developmental Science 14(5): 1161–1175.
    1. Ruff HA, Rothbart MK. (1996) Attention in Early Development: Themes and Variations. New York: Oxford University Press.
    1. Scerif G. (2010) Attention trajectories, mechanisms and outcomes: at the interface between developing cognition and environment. Developmental Science 13(6): 805–812.
    1. Soderqvist S, Nutley SB, Ottersen J, et al. (2012) Computerized training of non-verbal reasoning and working memory in children with intellectual disability. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 271.
    1. Steele A, Karmiloff-Smith A, Cornish K, et al. (2012) The multiple subfunctions of attention: differential developmental gateways to literacy and numeracy. Child Development 83(6): 2028–2041.
    1. Swettenham J. (1996) Can children with autism be taught to understand false belief using computers? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 37(2): 157–165.
    1. Swettenham J, Baron-Cohen S, Charman T, et al. (1998) The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 39(5): 747–753.
    1. Tanaka JW, Wolf JM, Klaiman C, et al. (2012) Using computerized games to teach face recognition skills to children with autism spectrum disorder: the Let’s Face It! program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51(8): 944–952.
    1. Wallace KS, Rogers SJ. (2010) Intervening in infancy: implications for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51(12): 1300–1320.
    1. Wass S, Porayska-Pomsta K, Johnson MH. (2011) Training attentional control in infancy. Current Biology 21(18): 1543–1547.
    1. Wass SV. (2014) Applying cognitive training to target executive functions during early development. Child Neuropsychology 21(2): 150–166.
    1. Wass SV, Porayska-Pomsta K. (2013) The uses of cognitive training technologies in the treatment of autism spectrum disorders. Autism 18(3): 1–21.
    1. Wass SV, Forssman L, Leppänen J. (2014) Robustness and precision: how data quality may influence key dependent variables in infant eye-tracker analyses. Infancy 19(5): 427–460.
    1. Wass SV, Jones EJH, Gliga T, et al. (2015) Shorter spontaneous fixation durations in infants with later emerging autism. Nature Scientific Reports 5(8): 1–8.
    1. Wass SV, Scerif G, Johnson MH. (2012) Training attentional control and working memory: is younger, better? Developmental Review 32(4): 360–387.
    1. Welsh JA, Nix RL, Blair C, et al. (2010) The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology 102(1): 43–53.
    1. Young GS, Merin N, Rogers SJ, et al. (2009) Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental Science 12: 798–814.

Source: PubMed

3
Tilaa