Characterization of phase transition in the thalamocortical system during anesthesia-induced loss of consciousness

Eunjin Hwang, Seunghwan Kim, Kyungreem Han, Jee Hyun Choi, Eunjin Hwang, Seunghwan Kim, Kyungreem Han, Jee Hyun Choi

Abstract

The thalamocortical system plays a key role in the breakdown or emergence of consciousness, providing bottom-up information delivery from sensory afferents and integrating top-down intracortical and thalamocortical reciprocal signaling. A fundamental and so far unanswered question for cognitive neuroscience remains whether the thalamocortical switch for consciousness works in a discontinuous manner or not. To unveil the nature of thalamocortical system phase transition in conjunction with consciousness transition, ketamine/xylazine was administered unobtrusively to ten mice under a forced working test with motion tracker, and field potentials in the sensory and motor-related cortex and thalamic nuclei were concomitantly collected. Sensory and motor-related thalamocortical networks were found to behave continuously at anesthesia induction and emergence, as evidenced by a sigmoidal response function with respect to anesthetic concentration. Hyperpolarizing and depolarizing susceptibility diverged, and a non-discrete change of transitional probability occurred at transitional regimes, which are hallmarks of continuous phase transition. The hyperpolarization curve as a function of anesthetic concentration demonstrated a hysteresis loop, with a significantly higher anesthetic level for transition to the down state compared to transition to the up state. Together, our findings concerning the nature of phase transition in the thalamocortical system during consciousness transition further elucidate the underlying basis for the ambiguous borderlines between conscious and unconscious brains. Moreover, our novel analysis method can be applied to systematic and quantitative handling of subjective concepts in cognitive neuroscience.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Verification of electrodes location.
Figure 1. Verification of electrodes location.
Electrode placements in ventral posteromedial (A) and ventral lateral (B) nuclei were histologically verified with light microscopy. Arrow point indicates the tip of electrode. fi, fimbria of the hippocampus; st, stria terminalis; ic, internal capsule; cp, cerebral peduncle (basal part); VPM, ventral posteromedial nucleus; VPL, central posterolateral nucleus; ZI, zona incerta; STh, subthalamic nucleus; Po, posterior thalamic nuclear group; VM, ventromedial thalamic nucleus; DG, dentate gyrus; CM, central medial nucleus; VL, ventral lateral nucleus; VP, ventral posteromedial and posterolateral nuclei; Rt, reticular thalamic nucleus; LV, lateral ventricle. Scale bar is 1 mm.
Figure 2. Dynamics of brain state in…
Figure 2. Dynamics of brain state in temporal domain.
A. Representative time series for an electroencephalogram (EEG) from primary motor cortex (M1), local field potentials from ventral lateral (VL) thalamus, and motion signal around the time of drug administration (ADM), loss of motion (LOM), deep anesthesia, recovery of motion (ROM), and resumption of walking. For the periods of anesthesia, recovery of motion and resumption of walking, the motion signal is exaggerated by five times for clearance. Dashed lines in the panels indicate corresponding events. B. Representative plot of δ band (1–4 Hz) phase synchronization for thalamocortical motor pair (M1 and VL) and sensory pair (primary somatosensory cortex and ventral posteromedial nucleus). Dashed lines indicate drug administration (tADM), loss of motion (tLOM), and recovery of motion (tROM), from left to right. C. Ensemble average (N = 10) of the order parameter (black solid) and its standard error mean (gray errorbars) as a function of rescaled time. The rescaled time is obtained by normalizing measurement time with respect to the blackout period. The moments of tLOM, and tROM correspond to 0 and 1, respectively. D. Ensemble-averaged absolute value of time derivative of order parameter (black solid) and its standard error mean (gray errorbars). The peak amplitude during anesthetic induction period was statistically significantly larger than the value at recovery (t-test, P<0.05). E. Probability of transition between up and down states at 10-s time intervals. Gray errorbars indicate standard error mean of the transition probability. For a given 10-s observational period, the number of epochs was approximately twenty during the transitional periods and fifteen during the deep anesthesia period.
Figure 3. Path-dependent change of brain state…
Figure 3. Path-dependent change of brain state and metastable fluctuation during transition.
A. Anesthetic concentration estimated with the three-compartment is plotted with respect to rescaled time, where the moments of tLOM, and tROM correspond to 0 and 1, respectively. Black solid line indicates averaged anesthetic concentration among ten subjects and gray errorbars indicate standard error mean. B. Ensemble average (N = 10) of the order parameter (greenish gray solid) and sigmoidal curve fitting as a function of anesthetic concentration c (black solid). Gray errorbars indicate standard error mean of the order parameter. Arrowheads show the passage of time. C. Ensemble-averaged time courses of hyperpolarizing (solid) and depolarizing (dashed) susceptibilities. Gray errorbars indicate standard error mean of the susceptibilities. The distributions of concentration for maximal susceptibility are shown as the embedded boxplots at the corresponding peaks. D. Representative plot of the recurrence probability of the order parameter for each bin of anesthetic concentration (bin size = 0.1/0.05 for the induction and emergence periods, respectively). Traces of the order parameter at the transitional moments at approximately 0.8 (during induction of anesthesia) and 0.2 (during recovery) are provided as inset figures. The anesthetic concentration of 0.0 at the left side of the axis indicates the moment of drug administration (ADM).

References

    1. Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND (2004) Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol 3: 537–546.
    1. Boveroux P, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Bruno MA, Noirhomme Q, Lauwick S, et al. (2010) Breakdown of within- and between-network resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging connectivity during propofol-induced loss of consciousness. Anesthesiology 113: 1038–1053.
    1. Ferrarelli F, Massimini M, Sarasso S, Casali A, Riedner BA, et al. (2010) Breakdown in cortical effective connectivity during midazolam-induced loss of consciousness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 2681–2686.
    1. Alkire MT, Hudetz AG, Tononi G (2008) Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 322: 876–880.
    1. Steyn-Ross ML, Steyn-Ross DA, Sleigh JW (2004) Modelling general anaesthesia as a first-order phase transition in the cortex. Prog Biophys Mol Biol 85: 369–385.
    1. Liley DT, Bojak I (2005) Understanding the transition to seizure by modeling the epileptiform activity of general anesthetic agents. J Clin Neurophysiol 22: 300–313.
    1. Molaee-Ardekani B, Senhadji L, Shamsollahi MB, Vosoughi-Vahdat B, Wodey E (2007) Brain activity modeling in general anesthesia: enhancing local mean-field models using a slow adaptive firing rate. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 76: 041911.
    1. Steriade M, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ (1993) Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science 262: 679–685.
    1. Llinas RR, Steriade M (2006) Bursting of thalamic neurons and states of vigilance. J Neurophysiol 95: 3297–3308.
    1. Edelman GM (2003) Naturalizing consciousness: a theoretical framework. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100: 5520–5524.
    1. Hwang E, Kim S, Shin HS, Choi JH (2010) The forced walking test: a novel test for pinpointing the anesthetic-induced transition in consciousness in mouse. J Neurosci Methods 188: 14–23.
    1. Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ (2001) The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
    1. Schuttler J, Stanski DR, White PF, Trevor AJ, Horai Y, et al. (1987) Pharmacodynamic modeling of the EEG effects of ketamine and its enantiomers in man. J Pharmacokinet Biopharm 15: 241–253.
    1. Friedman EB, Sun Y, Moore JT, Hung HT, Meng QC, et al. (2010) A conserved behavioral state barrier impedes transitions between anesthetic-induced unconsciousness and wakefulness: evidence for neural inertia. PLoS One 5: e11903.
    1. Schwender D, Daunderer M, Mulzer S, Klasing S, Finsterer U, et al. (1996) Spectral edge frequency of the electroencephalogram to monitor “depth” of anaesthesia with isoflurane or propofol. Br J Anaesth 77: 179–184.
    1. Billard V, Gambus PL, Chamoun N, Stanski DR, Shafer SL (1997) A comparison of spectral edge, delta power, and bispectral index as EEG measures of alfentanil, propofol, and midazolam drug effect. Clin Pharmacol Ther 61: 45–58.
    1. Sleigh J, Steyn-Ross M, Steyn-Ross A, Voss L, Wilson M (2010) Anesthesia-Induced State Transitions in Neuronal Populations. In: Hudetz A, Pearce R, editors. Suppressing the Mind: Anesthetic Modulation of Memory and Consciousness. New York: Humana Press. 139–160.
    1. Gandomani MJ, Ghashghaii A, Tamadon A, Attaran HR, Behzadi MA, et al. (2011) Comparison of Anaesthetic Effects of Ketamine -Xylazine and Ketamine- Diazepam Combination in Budgerigar. Vet Scan 6: 81.
    1. Fosse RT, Grong K, Stangeland L, Lekven J (1987) Anesthetic interaction in cardiovascular research models: effects of xylazine and pentobarbital in cats. Am J Vet Res 48: 211–218.
    1. Alkire MT (2010) Anesthesia and the Thalamocortical System. In: Hudetz A, Pearce R, editors. Suppressing the Mind: Anesthetic Modulation of Memory and Consciousness. New York: Humana Press. 127–138.
    1. Hudetz AG (2006) Suppressing consciousness: Mechanisms of general anesthesia. Seminars in Anesthesia, Perioperative Medicine and Pain 25: 196–204.
    1. Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, Huber R, Esser SK, Singh H, et al. (2005) Breakdown of cortical effective connectivity during sleep. Science 309: 2228–2232.

Source: PubMed

3
Tilaa