Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents

Kimberly G Noble, Suzanne M Houston, Natalie H Brito, Hauke Bartsch, Eric Kan, Joshua M Kuperman, Natacha Akshoomoff, David G Amaral, Cinnamon S Bloss, Ondrej Libiger, Nicholas J Schork, Sarah S Murray, B J Casey, Linda Chang, Thomas M Ernst, Jean A Frazier, Jeffrey R Gruen, David N Kennedy, Peter Van Zijl, Stewart Mostofsky, Walter E Kaufmann, Tal Kenet, Anders M Dale, Terry L Jernigan, Elizabeth R Sowell, Kimberly G Noble, Suzanne M Houston, Natalie H Brito, Hauke Bartsch, Eric Kan, Joshua M Kuperman, Natacha Akshoomoff, David G Amaral, Cinnamon S Bloss, Ondrej Libiger, Nicholas J Schork, Sarah S Murray, B J Casey, Linda Chang, Thomas M Ernst, Jean A Frazier, Jeffrey R Gruen, David N Kennedy, Peter Van Zijl, Stewart Mostofsky, Walter E Kaufmann, Tal Kenet, Anders M Dale, Terry L Jernigan, Elizabeth R Sowell

Abstract

Socioeconomic disparities are associated with differences in cognitive development. The extent to which this translates to disparities in brain structure is unclear. We investigated relationships between socioeconomic factors and brain morphometry, independently of genetic ancestry, among a cohort of 1,099 typically developing individuals between 3 and 20 years of age. Income was logarithmically associated with brain surface area. Among children from lower income families, small differences in income were associated with relatively large differences in surface area, whereas, among children from higher income families, similar income increments were associated with smaller differences in surface area. These relationships were most prominent in regions supporting language, reading, executive functions and spatial skills; surface area mediated socioeconomic differences in certain neurocognitive abilities. These data imply that income relates most strongly to brain structure among the most disadvantaged children.

Conflict of interest statement

We declare no conflicts of interest for any authors.

Figures

Figure 1. Parent Education is Linearly Associated…
Figure 1. Parent Education is Linearly Associated with Cortical Surface Area (N=1099)
A. Multiple regression showed that, when adjusting for age, age2, scanner, sex, and genetic ancestry, parental education was significantly associated with children’s cortical surface area in a number of regions. B. Left hemisphere regions included the left superior, middle, and inferior temporal gyri, inferior frontal gyrus, orbito-frontal gyrus and the precuneus. Right hemisphere regions included the middle temporal gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus, supramarginal gryus, middle frontal gyrus and superior frontal gyrus. Bilateral regions included the fusiform gyrus, temporal pole, insula, superior frontal gyrus, medial frontal gyrus, the cingulate cortex, inferior parietal cortex, lateral occipital cortex, and postcentral gyrus. Maps are thresholded at p <. 05 (FDR correction).
Figure 2. Family Income is Logarithmically Related…
Figure 2. Family Income is Logarithmically Related to Cortical Surface Area (N=1099)
A. Multiple regression showed that, when adjusting for age, age2, scanner, sex, and genetic ancestry, family income was significantly logarithmically associated with children’s total cortical surface area, such that the steepest gradient was present at the lower end of the income spectrum (β = −0.19; p = 0.004). Income data are presented here on the untransformed scale, fitted with a logarithmic curve, to enable visualization of this asymptotic relationship. This differential rate of change is visualized with the brain maps, where the steepest change in cortical surface area per unit income is visualized with warm colors and the shallowest change in cortical surface area per unit income is visualized with cool colors. B. When adjusting for age, age2, scanner, sex, and genetic ancestry, ln(family income) was significantly associated with surface area in widespread regions of children’s bilateral frontal, temporal and parietal lobes. Relationships were strongest in bilateral inferior temporal, insula and inferior frontal gyrus, and in the right occipital and medial prefrontal cortex. C. When adjusting for age, age2, scanner, sex, genetic ancestry, and parent education, ln(family income) was significantly associated with surface area in a smaller number of regions including bilateral inferior frontal, cingulate, insula, and inferior temporal regions and in the right superior frontal and precuneus cortex. Maps are thresholded at p < .05 (FDR correction). More stringent FDR correction thresholds of .01 and .001 are shown in Supplementary Fig. 1a–c.
Figure 3. Parental Education is Quadratically Associated…
Figure 3. Parental Education is Quadratically Associated with Left Hippocampal Volume (N=1099)
Multiple regression showed that, when adjusting for age, age2, scanner, sex, genetic ancestry, and whole brain volume, parental education was significantly quadratically associated with children’s left hippocampal volume, such that the steepest gradient was present at the lower end of the education spectrum (β = −0.494; p = 0.016).

References

    1. Rosenzweig MR. Effects of differential experience on the brain and behavior. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2003;24:523–540.
    1. Sowell ER, et al. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:309–315.
    1. McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist. 1998;53:185–204.
    1. Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. The effects of poverty on children. [Review] [63 refs] Future of Children. 1997;7:55–71.
    1. Hackman D, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13:65–73.
    1. Noble KG, Wolmetz ME, Ochs LG, Farah MJ, McCandliss BD. Brain-behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic factors. Developmental Science. 2006;9:642–654.
    1. Raizada RD, Richards TL, Meltzoff A, Kuhl PK. Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. Neuroimage. 2008;40:1392–1401.
    1. Stevens C, Lauinger B, Neville H. Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: An event-related brain potential study. Developmental Science. 2009;12:634–646.
    1. Sheridan MA, Sarsour K, Jutte D, D'Esposito M, Boyce WT. The impact of social disparity on prefontal function in childhood. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e35744.
    1. Tomalski P, et al. Socioeconomic status and functional brain development – associations in early infancy. Developmental Science. 2013 n/a-n/a.
    1. Kim P, et al. Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110:18442–18447.
    1. Sowell ER, et al. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:8223–8231.
    1. Noble KG, et al. Hippocampal volume varies with educational attainment across the life-span. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6
    1. Noble KG, Houston SM, Kan E, Sowell ER. Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Developmental Science. 2012;15:516–527.
    1. Noble KG, Korgaonkar MS, Grieve SM, Brickman AM. Higher Education is an Age-Independent Predictor of White Matter Integrity and Cognitive Control in Late Adolescence. Developmental Science. 2013
    1. Chiang M-C, et al. Genetics of white matter development: A DTI study of 705 twins and their siblings aged 12 to 29. NeuroImage Oct. 2010 No Pagination Specified.
    1. Hanson JL, Chandra A, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Association between income and the hippocampus. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e18712.
    1. Hanson JL, et al. Family Poverty Affects the Rate of Human Infant Brain Growth. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e80954.
    1. Jednoróg K, et al. The influence of socioeconomic status on children’s brain structure. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e42486.
    1. Lawson GM, Duda JT, Avants BB, Wu J, Farah MJ. Associations between children's socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Developmental Science. 2013
    1. Luby JL, et al. The Effects of Poverty on Childhood Brain Development: The Mediating Effect of Caregiving and Stressful Life Events. JAMA Pediatrics. 2013:E1–E8.
    1. Staff RT, et al. Childhood socioeconomic status and adult brain size: Childhood socioeconomic status influences adult hippocampal size. Annals of Neurology. 2012;71:653–660.
    1. Raznahan A, et al. How does your cortex grow? Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:7174–7177.
    1. Sowell ER, et al. Sex differences in cortical thickness mapped in 176 healthy individuals between 7 and 87 years of age. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:1550–1560.
    1. Schnack HG, et al. Changes in Thickness and Surface Area of the Human Cortex and Their Relationship with Intelligence. Cerebral cortex. 2014
    1. Shaw P, et al. Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature. 2006;440:676–679.
    1. Grieve SM, Korgaonkar MS, Clark CR, Williams LM. Regional heterogeneity in limbic maturational changes: Evidence from integrating cortical thickness, volumetric and diffusion tensor imaging measures. Neuroimage. 2011;55:868–879.
    1. Duncan GJ, Magnuson KA. Can family socioeconomic resources account for racial and ethnic test score gaps?[In volume School Readiness: Closing Racial and Ethnic Gaps] The Future of Children. 2005;15:35–54.
    1. Duncan GJ, Magnuson K. Socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning: Moving from correlation to causation. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 2012;3:377–386.
    1. Vannest J, Karunanayaka PR, Schmithorst VJ, Szaflarski JP, Holland SK. Language networks in children: Evidence from functional MRI studies. American Journal of Roentgenology. 2009
    1. McCandliss BD, Noble KG. The development of reading impairment: A cognitive neuroscience model. Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2003;9:196–204.
    1. Gianaros PJ, et al. Perigenual anterior cingulate morphology covaries with perceived social standing. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience. 2007;2:161–173.
    1. Casey B, et al. A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance on a go-no-go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1997;9:835–847.
    1. Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Increased brain activity in frontal and parietal cortex underlies the development of visuospatial working memory capacity during childhood. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:1–10.
    1. Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science. 2007;10:464–480.
    1. Noble KG, Norman MF, Farah MJ. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science. 2005;8:74–87.
    1. Evans GW. The environment of childhood poverty. American Psychologist. 2004;59:77–92.
    1. Luby J, et al. The Effects of Poverty on Childhood Brain Development: The Mediating Effect of Caregiving and Stressful Life Events. JAMA Pediatrics. 2013
    1. Lange N, Froimowitz MP, Bigler ED, Lainhart JE Brain Development Cooperative, G. Associations between IQ, total and regional brain volumes, and demography in a large normative sample of healthy children and adolescents. Developmental neuropsychology. 2010;35:296–317.
    1. Trzaskowski M, et al. Genetic influence on family socioeconomic status and children's intelligence. Intelligence. 2014;42:83–88.
    1. Raver CC, et al. CSRP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: Self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Development. 2011;82:362–378.
    1. Kitzman HJ, et al. Enduring effects of prenatal and infancy home visiting by nurses on children: follow-up of a randomized trial among children at age 12 years. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2010;164:412–418.
    1. Ziol-Guest KM, Duncan GJ, Kalil A, Boyce WT. Early childhood poverty, immune-mediated disease processes, and adult productivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:17289–17293.
    1. Akshoomoff N, et al. The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery: results from a large normative developmental sample (PING) Neuropsychology. 2014;28:1–10.
    1. Fjell AM, et al. Multimodal imaging of the self-regulating developing brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109:19620–19625.
    1. Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000;97:11050–11055.
    1. Bakken TE, et al. Association of common genetic variants in GPCPD1 with scaling of visual cortical surface area in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109:3985–3990.
    1. Alexander DH, Lange K. Enhancements to the ADMIXTURE algorithm for individual ancestry estimation. BMC Bioinformatics. 2011;12:246.
    1. Rueda MR, et al. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1029–1040.
    1. Tulsky DS, et al. V. NIH TOOLBOX COGNITION BATTERY (CB): MEASURING WORKING MEMORY. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2013;78:70–87.

Source: PubMed

3
Tilaa