Grasp-specific motor resonance is influenced by the visibility of the observed actor

Karen L Bunday, Roger N Lemon, James M Kilner, Marco Davare, Guy A Orban, Karen L Bunday, Roger N Lemon, James M Kilner, Marco Davare, Guy A Orban

Abstract

Motor resonance is the modulation of M1 corticospinal excitability induced by observation of others' actions. Recent brain imaging studies have revealed that viewing videos of grasping actions led to a differential activation of the ventral premotor cortex depending on whether the entire person is viewed versus only their disembodied hand. Here we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to examine motor evoked potentials (MEPs) in the first dorsal interosseous (FDI) and abductor digiti minimi (ADM) during observation of videos or static images in which a whole person or merely the hand was seen reaching and grasping a peanut (precision grip) or an apple (whole hand grasp). Participants were presented with six visual conditions in which visual stimuli (video vs static image), view (whole person vs hand) and grasp (precision grip vs whole hand grasp) were varied in a 2 × 2 × 2 factorial design. Observing videos, but not static images, of a hand grasping different objects resulted in a grasp-specific interaction, such that FDI and ADM MEPs were differentially modulated depending on the type of grasp being observed (precision grip vs whole hand grasp). This interaction was present when observing the hand acting, but not when observing the whole person acting. Additional experiments revealed that these results were unlikely to be due to the relative size of the hand being observed. Our results suggest that observation of videos rather than static images is critical for motor resonance. Importantly, observing the whole person performing the action abolished the grasp-specific effect, which could be due to a variety of PMv inputs converging on M1.

Keywords: Action observation; F5c; MEPs; Motor resonance; Videos.

Copyright © 2016 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.. All rights reserved.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Observed actions and experimental design. A. Observed actions shown in the videos and static images (frame depicts first contact with the object); hand alone grasping an apple (whole hand grasp), hand alone grasping a peanut (precision grip), whole person grasping an apple (whole hand grasp) and whole person grasping a peanut (precision grip). B. Experimental design showing 4 blocks containing a baseline without visual stimuli (15 MEPs evoked during rest periods), each condition (5 MEPs per condition) and the inter-trial interval (ITI) (15 MEPs evoked during this interval), with a final rest baseline (15 MEPs). Thus, in total an experiment consisted of 75 rest MEPs, 20 MEP per observation condition (160 in total for each muscle) and 60 ITI MEPs. C. An ITI preceded the video and image presentations. After a 5 sec delay (or 7 sec if TMS is given) the video began; the object is presented, followed by reaching to the object, contact of the hand with the object and grasping and lift off. TMS was given at object contact (2500 msec). The video ended at 4500 msec. The static image was presented for 4500 msec. TMS was given at 2750 msec (the average between precision grip and whole hand grasp contact).
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Without visual stimuli and inter-trial-interval baseline MEPs. Group data showing the mean FDI (A) and ADM (B) amplitude of MEPs evoked in the baseline without visual stimuli (open circles) and inter-trial-interval (ITI) (closed triangles) periods, across each block. The baseline without visual stimuli MEPs were recorded just prior to each block, whilst ITI MEPs were recorded randomly within each observation block. The abscissa shows the block number (1, 2, 3, 4). The ordinate shows the mean MEP amplitude (mV). Note that the ITI MEPs are significantly larger than MEPs without visual stimuli. Error bars indicate SE. *p < .05.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Without visual stimuli and ITI Normalised MEPs during observation of videos and static images. Group data showing the mean FDI (closed circles) and ADM (open circles) MEP size during observation of precision grip and whole hand grasp when subjects viewed videos (A, C), or static images (B, D) of precision grip and whole hand grasp. The abscissa shows the type of grasp observed (precision grip: PG, whole hand grasp: WHG). The ordinate shows MEP size expressed as a ratio of the MEP recorded during ITI (A, B) or trials without visual stimuli (C, D), where a value of 1 indicates that the baseline and observation MEPs were of equal amplitude. Note that when normalised to ITI trials, MEPs are suppressed compared with baseline ITI MEPs, whereas when normalised to trials without visual stimuli MEPs are facilitated. Error bars indicate SE. *p < .05.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
ITI normalised MEPs during observation of hand alone and whole person. Group data showing the mean FDI (closed circles) and ADM (open circles) MEP size during observation when viewing only the hand (A) or the whole person (B; collapse across videos and images). The abscissa shows the type of grasp observed (precision grip: PG, whole hand grasp: WHG). The ordinate shows MEP size expressed as a ratio of the MEP recorded during ITI trials, where a value of 1 indicates that MEPs during the ITI and observation conditions were of equal amplitude. Note that during the hand alone videos, but not whole person videos, a significant grasp × muscle interaction is present. Error bars indicate SE. *p < .05.
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
ITI normalised MEPs during observation of hand and person videos. Group data showing the mean FDI (closed circles) and ADM (open circles) MEP size during observation of videos when viewing only the hand (A) or the person (B). The abscissa shows the type of grasp observed (precision grip: PG, whole hand grasp: WHG). The ordinate shows MEP size expressed as a ratio of the MEP recorded during ITI trials, where a value of 1 indicates that MEPs during the ITI and observation conditions were of equal amplitude. Note that during the hand videos, but not person videos, a significant grasp × muscle interaction is present. Error bars indicate SE. *p < .05.
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
ITI normalised MEPs during observation of hand alone and small hand alone videos. Group data showing the mean FDI (closed circles) and ADM (open circles) MEP size during observation of videos when viewing only the hand (A) or the person (B). The abscissa shows the type of grasp observed (precision grip: PG, whole hand grasp: WHG). The ordinate shows MEP size expressed as a ratio of the MEP recorded during ITI trials, where a value of 1 indicates that MEPs during the ITI and observation conditions were of equal amplitude. Note that during observation both the hand and small hand alone videos a significant grasp × muscle interaction is present. Error bars indicate SE. *p < .05.

References

    1. Alaerts K., Heremans E., Swinnen S.P., Wenderoth N. How are observed actions mapped to the observer's motor system? Influence of posture and perspective. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(2):415–422.
    1. Alaerts K., Senot P., Swinnen S.P., Craighero L., Wenderoth N., Fadiga L. Force requirements of observed object lifting are encoded by the observer's motor system: A TMS study. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;31(6):1144–1153.
    1. Alaerts K., Swinnen S.P., Wenderoth N. Observing how others lift light or heavy objects: Which visual cues mediate the encoding of muscular force in the primary motor cortex? Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(7):2082–2090.
    1. Caggiano V., Fogassi L., Rizzolatti G., Thier P., Casile A. Mirror neurons differentially encode the peripersonal and extrapersonal space of monkeys. Science. 2009;324(5925):403–406.
    1. Catmur C., Mars R.B., Rushworth M.F., Heyes C. Making mirrors: Premotor cortex stimulation enhances mirror and counter-mirror motor facilitation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23(9):2352–2362.
    1. Catmur C., Walsh V., Heyes C. Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Current Biology. 2007;17(17):1527–1531.
    1. Cattaneo L., Caruana F., Jezzini A., Rizzolatti G. Representation of goal and movements without overt motor behavior in the human motor cortex: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(36):11134–11138.
    1. Cattaneo L., Voss M., Brochier T., Prabhu G., Wolpert D.M., Lemon R.N. A cortico-cortical mechanism mediating object-driven grasp in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102(3):898–903.
    1. Cavallo A., Becchio C., Sartori L., Bucchioni G., Castiello U. Grasping with tools: Corticospinal excitability reflects observed hand movements. Cerebral Cortex. 2012;22(3):710–716.
    1. Clark S., Tremblay F., Ste-Marie D. Differential modulation of corticospinal excitability during observation, mental imagery and imitation of hand actions. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(1):105–112.
    1. Davare M., Kraskov A., Rothwell J.C., Lemon R.N. Interactions between areas of the cortical grasping network. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2011;21(4):565–570.
    1. Davare M., Lemon R., Olivier E. Selective modulation of interactions between ventral premotor cortex and primary motor cortex during precision grasping in humans. The Journal of Physiology. 2008;586(Pt 11):2735–2742.
    1. Davare M., Montague K., Olivier E., Rothwell J.C., Lemon R.N. Ventral premotor to primary motor cortical interactions during object-driven grasp in humans. Cortex. 2009;45(9):1050–1057.
    1. Davare M., Rothwell J.C., Lemon R.N. Causal connectivity between the human anterior intraparietal area and premotor cortex during grasp. Current Biology. 2010;20(2):176–181.
    1. Fadiga L., Buccino G., Craighero L., Fogassi L., Gallese V., Pavesi G. Corticospinal excitability is specifically modulated by motor imagery: A magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37(2):147–158.
    1. Fadiga L., Craighero L., Olivier E. Human motor cortex excitability during the perception of others' action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2005;15(2):213–218.
    1. Fadiga L., Fogassi L., Pavesi G., Rizzolatti G. Motor facilitation during action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1995;73(6):2608–2611.
    1. Ferri S., Peeters R., Nelissen K., Vanduffel W., Rizzolatti G., Orban G.A. A human homologue of monkey F5c. NeuroImage. 2015;111:251–266.
    1. Gallese V., Fadiga L., Fogassi L., Rizzolatti G. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain. 1996;119(Pt 2):593–609.
    1. Gazzola V., van der Worp H., Mulder T., Wicker B., Rizzolatti G., Keysers C. Aplasics born without hands mirror the goal of hand actions with their feet. Current Biology. 2007;17(14):1235–1240.
    1. Janssen P., Scherberger H. Visual guidance in control of grasping. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2015;38:69–86.
    1. Jastorff J., Begliomini C., Fabbri-Destro M., Rizzolatti G., Orban G.A. Coding observed motor acts: Different organizational principles in the parietal and premotor cortex of humans. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;104(1):128–140.
    1. Jeannerod M., Arbib M.A., Rizzolatti G., Sakata H. Grasping objects: The cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences. 1995;18(7):314–320.
    1. Kassavetis P., Sadnicka A., Saifee T.A., Belvisi D., van den Bos M., Parees I. Motor 'surround inhibition' is not correlated with activity in surround muscles. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;40(3):2541–2547.
    1. Keel J.C., Smith M.J., Wassermann E.M. A safety screening questionnaire for transcranial magnetic stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2001;112(4):720.
    1. Kraskov A., Dancause N., Quallo M.M., Shepherd S., Lemon R.N. Corticospinal neurons in macaque ventral premotor cortex with mirror properties: A potential mechanism for action suppression? Neuron. 2009;64(6):922–930.
    1. Langton S.R., O'Donnell C., Riby D.M., Ballantyne C.J. Gaze cues influence the allocation of attention in natural scene viewing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove) 2006;59(12):2056–2064.
    1. Loporto M., McAllister C.J., Edwards M.G., Wright D.J., Holmes P.S. Prior action execution has no effect on corticospinal facilitation during action observation. Behavioural Brain Research. 2012;231(1):124–129.
    1. Maeda K., Ishida H., Nakajima K., Inase M., Murata A. Functional properties of parietal hand manipulation-related neurons and mirror neurons responding to vision of own hand action. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2015;27(3):560–572.
    1. Maeda F., Kleiner-Fisman G., Pascual-Leone A. Motor facilitation while observing hand actions: Specificity of the effect and role of observer's orientation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2002;87(3):1329–1335.
    1. McCabe S.I., Villalta J.I., Saunier G., Grafton S.T., Della-Maggiore V. The relative influence of goal and kinematics on corticospinal excitability depends on the information provided to the observer. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25(8):2229–2237.
    1. Mukamel R., Ekstrom A.D., Kaplan J., Iacoboni M., Fried I. Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology. 2010;20(8):750–756.
    1. Murata A., Gallese V., Luppino G., Kaseda M., Sakata H. Selectivity for the shape, size, and orientation of objects for grasping in neurons of monkey parietal area AIP. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;83(5):2580–2601.
    1. Nelissen K., Borra E., Gerbella M., Rozzi S., Luppino G., Vanduffel W. Action observation circuits in the macaque monkey cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(10):3743–3756.
    1. Nelissen K., Vanduffel W. Grasping-related functional magnetic resonance imaging brain responses in the macaque monkey. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(22):8220–8229.
    1. Orban G., Platonov A. Psychometric curves describe action discrimination in humans. Journal of Vision. 2015;15(12):561.
    1. Pani P., Theys T., Romero M.C., Janssen P. Grasping execution and grasping observation activity of single neurons in the macaque anterior intraparietal area. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014;26(10):2342–2355.
    1. Prabhu G., Voss M., Brochier T., Cattaneo L., Haggard P., Lemon R. Excitability of human motor cortex inputs prior to grasp. Journal of Physiology. 2007;581(Pt 1):189–201.
    1. Rossini P.M., Burke D., Chen R., Cohen L.G., Daskalakis Z., Di Iorio R. Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2015;126(6):1071–1107.
    1. Rothwell J.C., Hallett M., Berardelli A., Eisen A., Rossini P., Paulus W. Magnetic stimulation: Motor evoked potentials. The international federation of clinical neurophysiology. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology Suppl. 1999;52:97–103.
    1. Sartori L., Bucchioni G., Castiello U. Motor cortex excitability is tightly coupled to observed movements. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(9):2341–2347.
    1. Senot P., D'Ausilio A., Franca M., Caselli L., Craighero L., Fadiga L. Effect of weight-related labels on corticospinal excitability during observation of grasping: A TMS study. Experimental Brain Research. 2011;211(1):161–167.
    1. Sohn Y.H., Hallett M. Surround inhibition in human motor system. Experimental Brain Research. 2004;158(4):397–404.
    1. Strafella A.P., Paus T. Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. NeuroReport. 2000;11(10):2289–2292.
    1. Urgesi C., Candidi M., Fabbro F., Romani M., Aglioti S.M. Motor facilitation during action observation: Topographic mapping of the target muscle and influence of the onlooker's posture. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;23(9):2522–2530.
    1. Vigneswaran G., Philipp R., Lemon R.N., Kraskov A. M1 corticospinal mirror neurons and their role in movement suppression during action observation. Current Biology. 2013;23(3):236–243.

Source: PubMed

3
Tilaa