Attention to irrelevant cues is related to positive symptoms in schizophrenia

Richard Morris, Oren Griffiths, Michael E Le Pelley, Thomas W Weickert, Richard Morris, Oren Griffiths, Michael E Le Pelley, Thomas W Weickert

Abstract

Many modern learning theories assume that the amount of attention to a cue depends on how well that cue predicted important events in the past. Schizophrenia is associated with deficits in attention and recent theories of psychosis have argued that positive symptoms such as delusions and hallucinations are related to a failure of selective attention. However, evidence demonstrating that attention to irrelevant cues is related to positive symptoms in schizophrenia is lacking. We used a novel method of measuring attention to nonpredictive (and thus irrelevant) cues in a causal learning test (Le Pelley ME, McLaren IP. Learned associability and associative change in human causal learning. Q J Exp Psychol B. 2003;56:68-79) to assess whether healthy adults and people with schizophrenia discriminate previously predictive and nonpredictive cues. In a series of experiments with independent samples, we demonstrated: (1) when people with schizophrenia who had severe positive symptoms successfully distinguished between predictive and nonpredictive cues during training, they failed to discriminate between predictive and nonpredictive cues relative to healthy adults during subsequent testing and (2) learning about nonpredictive cues was correlated with more severe positive symptoms scores in schizophrenia. These results suggest that positive symptoms of schizophrenia are related to increased attention to nonpredictive cues during causal learning. This deficit in selective attention results in learning irrelevant causal associations and may be the basis of positive symptoms in schizophrenia.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Mean learning scores of the stage 2 test for each group (A) and each subgroup of learners (B). Error bars show SEM. *Significant between-group difference (P < .05).
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Correlation of Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale positive symptom scores with predictive (A) and nonpredictive cue performance (B); and mean learning scores of the stage 2 test for the high- and low-positive symptom subgroup and the healthy adults (C). Error bars show SEM. **P < .01.

References

    1. Kruschke JK. Toward a unified model of attention in associative learning. J Math Psychol. 2001;45:812–863.
    1. Le Pelley ME. The role of associative history in models of associative learning: a selective review and a hybrid model. Q J Exp Psychol. 2004;57B:193–243.
    1. Mackintosh NJ. A theory of attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 1975;82:276–298.
    1. Braff DL, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR. Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;156:234–258.
    1. Jazbec S, Pantelis C, Robbins T, Weickert T, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE. Intra-dimensional/extra-dimensional set-shifting performance in schizophrenia: impact of distractors. Schizophr Res. 2007;89:339–349.
    1. Barch DM, Carter CS, Hachten PC, Usher M, Cohen JD. The “benefits” of distractibility: mechanisms underlying increased Stroop effects in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1999;25:749–762.
    1. Posner MI, Early TS, Reiman E, Pardo PJ, Dhawan M. Asymmetries in hemispheric control of attention in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45:814–821.
    1. Strauss ME, Alphs L, Boekamp J. Disengagement of attention in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1992;43:87–92.
    1. Servan-Schreiber D, Cohen JD, Steingard S. Schizophrenic deficits in the processing of context. A test of a theoretical model. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:1105–1112.
    1. Lubow RE, Moore AU. Latent inhibition: the effect of nonreinforced pre-exposure to the conditional stimulus. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1959;52:415–419.
    1. Kamin LJ. Selective association and conditioning. In: Mackintosh NJ, Honig WK, editors. Fundamental Issues in Instrumental Learning. Halifax, Canada: Dalhousie University Press; 1969. pp. 42–64.
    1. Jones SH, Gray JA, Hemsley DR. Loss of the Kamin blocking effect in acute but not chronic schizophrenics. Biol Psychiatry. 1992;32:739–755.
    1. Jones SH, Hemsley D, Ball S, Serra A. Disruption of the Kamin blocking effect in schizophrenia and in normal subjects following amphetamine. Behav Brain Res. 1997;88:103–114.
    1. Cheng PW, Novick LR. A probabilistic contrast model of causal induction. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;58:545–567.
    1. De Houwer J, Beckers T, Vandorpe S. Evidence for the role of higher order reasoning processes in cue competition and other learning phenomena. Learn Behav. 2005;33:239–249.
    1. De Houwer J, Beckers T, Glautier S. Outcome and cue properties modulate blocking. Q J Exp Psychol A. 2002;55:965–985.
    1. Weickert TW, Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Bigelow LB, Egan MF, Weinberger DR. Cognitive impairments in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:907–913.
    1. Weickert TW, Terrazas A, Bigelow LB, et al. Habit and skill learning in schizophrenia: evidence of normal striatal processing with abnormal cortical input. Learn Mem. 2002;9:430–442.
    1. Le Pelley ME, McLaren IP. Learned associability and associative change in human causal learning. Q J Exp Psychol B. 2003;56:68–79.
    1. Beesley T, Le Pelley ME, Griffiths O. Overt attention and predictive validity in human associative learning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2011;37:220–229.
    1. Corlett PR, Honey GD, Fletcher PC. From prediction error to psychosis: ketamine as a pharmacological model of delusions. J Psychopharmacol. 2007;21:238–252.
    1. Gray JA. Integrating schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1998;24:249–266.
    1. Corlett PR, Murray GK, Honey GD, et al. Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: evidence for an associative account of delusions. Brain. 2007;130(pt 9):2387–2400.
    1. McGowan S, Lawrence AD, Sales T, Quested D, Grasby P. Presynaptic dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomographic [18F]fluorodopa study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:134–142.
    1. Martins Serra A, Jones SH, Toone B, Gray JA. Impaired associative learning in chronic schizophrenics and their first-degree relatives: a study of latent inhibition and the Kamin blocking effect. Schizophr Res. 2001;48:273–289.
    1. Moran PM, Owen L, Crookes AE, Al-Uzri MM, Reveley MA. Abnormal prediction error is associated with negative and depressive symptoms in schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2008;32:116–123.
    1. Oades RD, Zimmermann B, Eggers C. Conditioned blocking in patients with paranoid, non-paranoid psychosis or obsessive compulsive disorder: associations with symptoms, personality and monoamine metabolism. J Psychiat Res. 1996;30:369–390.
    1. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders—Patient Edition. 1st ed, revised. New York, NY: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2007.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2001.
    1. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276.
    1. Le Pelley ME, Schmidt-Hansen M, Harris NJ, Lunter CM, Morris CS. Disentangling the attentional deficit in schizophrenia: pointers from schizotypy. Psychiatry Res. 2010;176:143–149.
    1. Lindenmayer JP, Bernstein-Hyman R, Grochowski S. A new five factor model of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Q. 1994;65:299–322.
    1. Bollini P, Pampallona S, Nieddu S, Bianco M, Tibaldi G, Munizza C. Indicators of conformance with guidelines of schizophrenia treatment in mental health services. Psychiatr Serv. 2008;59:782–791.
    1. Le Pelley ME. Attention and human associative learning. In: Mitchell CJ, Le Pelley ME, editors. Attention and Associative Learning: From Brain to Behaviour. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2010. pp. 187–215.
    1. Mulholland CC, O'Donoghue D, Meenagh C, Rushe TM. Errorless learning and memory performance in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2008;159:180–188.
    1. O'Carroll RE, Russell HH, Lawrie SM, Johnstone EC. Errorless learning and the cognitive rehabilitation of memory-impaired schizophrenic patients. Psychol Med. 1999;29:105–112.
    1. Baruch I, Hemsley DR, Gray JA. Differential performance of acute and chronic schizophrenics in a latent inhibition task. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1988;176:598–606.
    1. Rascle C, Mazas O, Vaiva G, et al. Clinical features of latent inhibition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2001;51:149–161.
    1. Roiser JP, Stephan KE, den Ouden HE, Barnes TR, Friston KJ, Joyce EM. Do patients with schizophrenia exhibit aberrant salience? Psychol Med. 2009;39:199–209.
    1. Fletcher PC, Anderson JM, Shanks DR, et al. Responses of human frontal cortex to surprising events are predicted by formal associative learning theory. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:1043–1048.
    1. Morris RW, Vercammen A, Lenroot R, et al. Disambiguating ventral striatum fMRI related BOLD signal during reward prediction in schizophrenia [published online ahead of print June 28, 2011] Mol Psychiatry. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.75.
    1. Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:1–27.

Source: PubMed

3
Tilaa