Cognitive development in children with chronic protein energy malnutrition

Bhoomika R Kar, Shobini L Rao, B A Chandramouli, Bhoomika R Kar, Shobini L Rao, B A Chandramouli

Abstract

Background: Malnutrition is associated with both structural and functional pathology of the brain. A wide range of cognitive deficits has been reported in malnourished children. Effect of chronic protein energy malnutrition (PEM) causing stunting and wasting in children could also affect the ongoing development of higher cognitive processes during childhood (>5 years of age). The present study examined the effect of stunted growth on the rate of development of cognitive processes using neuropsychological measures.

Methods: Twenty children identified as malnourished and twenty as adequately nourished in the age groups of 5-7 years and 8-10 years were examined. NIMHANS neuropsychological battery for children sensitive to the effects of brain dysfunction and age related improvement was employed. The battery consisted of tests of motor speed, attention, visuospatial ability, executive functions, comprehension and learning and memory

Results: Development of cognitive processes appeared to be governed by both age and nutritional status. Malnourished children performed poor on tests of attention, working memory, learning and memory and visuospatial ability except on the test of motor speed and coordination. Age related improvement was not observed on tests of design fluency, working memory, visual construction, learning and memory in malnourished children. However, age related improvement was observed on tests of attention, visual perception, and verbal comprehension in malnourished children even though the performance was deficient as compared to the performance level of adequately nourished children.

Conclusion: Chronic protein energy malnutrition (stunting) affects the ongoing development of higher cognitive processes during childhood years rather than merely showing a generalized cognitive impairment. Stunting could result in slowing in the age related improvement in certain and not all higher order cognitive processes and may also result in long lasting cognitive impairments.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Age related comparisons between adequately nourished and malnourished children on motor speed (right and left hand). Note: MS (R): motor speed (right hand); MS (L): motor speed (left hand); AN: adequately nourished; MN: malnourished.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Age related comparisons between adequately nourished and malnourished children on selective attention (color cancellation test). Note: SA: selective attention; AN: adequately nourished; MN: malnourished.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Age related comparisons between adequately nourished and malnourished children on executive functions. Note: VF: verbal fluency; DF: design fluency; WM: working memory; AN: adequately nourished; MN: malnourished.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Age related comparisons between adequately nourished and malnourished children on visuospatial functions. Note: PC: picture completion; BD: block design; MVP: motorfree visual perception; AN: adequately nourished; MN: malnourished.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Age related comparisons between adequately nourished and malnourished children on verbal comprehension and verbal learning. Note: VC: verbal comprehension; VL: verbal learning; AN: adequately nourished; MN: malnourished.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Age related comparisons between adequately nourished and malnourished children on verbal and visual memory. Note: VM: verbal memory; VisM: visual memory; AN: adequately nourished; MN: malnourished

References

    1. Udani PM. Brain and various facets of child development. Indian J Pediatr. 1992;59:165–186. doi: 10.1007/BF02759978.
    1. Upadhyaya SK, Saran A, Agarwal DK, Singh MP, Agarwal KN. Growth and behaviour development in rural infants in relation to malnutrition and environment. Indian Pediatr. 1992;29:595–606.
    1. Leenstra T, Petersen LT, Kariuki SK, Oloo AJ, Kager PA, ter Kuile FO. Prevalence and severity of malnutrition and age at menarche; cross-sectional studies in adolescent schoolgirls in western Kenya. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2005;59:41–48. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602031.
    1. Levitsky DA, Strupp BJ. Malnutrition and the brain: undernutrition and behavioural development in children. J Nutr. 1995;125:2212S–2220S.
    1. Upadhyaya SK, Agarwal KN, Agarwal DK. Influence of malnutrition on social maturity, visual motor coordination and memory in rural school children. Indian J Med Res. 1989;90:320–327.
    1. Upadhyay SK, Agarwal DK, Agarwal KN. Influence of malnutrition on intellectual development. Indian J Med Res. 1989;90:430–441.
    1. Mendez MA, Adair LS. Severity and timing of stunting in the first two years of life affect performance on cognitive tests in late childhood. J Nutr. 1999;129:1555–1562.
    1. Posner MI, Petersen SE, Fox PT, Raichle ME. Localization of cognitive functions in the human brain. Science. 1988;240:1627–1631. doi: 10.1126/science.3289116.
    1. Strupp BJ, Levitsky DA. Enduring cognitive effects of early malnutrition: A theoretical Appraisal. J Nutr. 1995;125:2221S–2232S.
    1. Das JP, Pivato E. Malnutrition and cognitive functioning. In: Ellis NR, editor. International review of research in mental retardation. 1976. p. 8.
    1. Measuring changes in Nutritional Status. WHO, Geneva; 1983.
    1. Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH. Colored progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press; 1998.
    1. Kar BR, Rao SL, Chandramouli BA, Thennarasu K. NIMHANS neuropsychological battery for children-manual. Bangalore: NIMHANS publication division; 2004.
    1. Rutter MA. Children's behaviour questionnaire for completion by teachers: preliminary findings. J Child Psychol Psyc. 1967;8:1–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1967.tb02175.x.
    1. Reitan RM. Sensorimotor functions, intelligence and cognition, and emotional status in subjects with cerebral lesions. Percept Motor Skill. 1970;31:275–284.
    1. Bornstein RA. Normative data on selected neuropsychological measures from a nonclinical sample. J Clin Psychol. 1985;41:651–659. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198509)41:5<651::AID-JCLP2270410511>;2-C.
    1. Korkman M, Kemp SL, Kirk U. Effects of age on neurocognitive measures of children ages 5–12 years: A cross-sectional study on 800 children from the United States. Dev Neuropsychol. 2001;20:325–330. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_1.
    1. D' Elia LF, Satz P, Uchiyama CL, White T. Color trails test. Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1996.
    1. Williams J, Rickert V, Hogan J, Zolten AJ, Satz P, D'Elia LF, Asarnow RF, Zaucha K, Light R. Children's colour trails. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 1995;10:211–223. doi: 10.1016/0887-6177(94)00041-N.
    1. Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests. 2. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.
    1. Kapur M. Measurement of organic brain dysfunction. PhD thesis submitted to Bangalore University. 1974.
    1. Benton AL. Right-left discrimination. Pediatr Clin North Am. 1968;15:747–58.
    1. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995.
    1. Jones-Gotman M, Milner B. Design fluency: the invention of nonsense drawings after focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1977;15:653–674. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(77)90070-7.
    1. Jones-Gotman M. Localization of lesions by neuropsychological testing. Epilepsia. 1991;32:S41–S52.
    1. Kown H, Reiss AL, Menon V. Neural basis of protracted developmental changes in visuospatial working memory. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:13336–13341. doi: 10.1073/pnas.162486399.
    1. Kar BR. PhD thesis. National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, Department of Clinical Psychology; 2003. Standardization of a neuropsychological battery for children.
    1. Khetrapal N, Kar BR, Srinivasan N. Development of visuospatial working memory: Role of load and task difficulty. Psychological Studies. 2008;53:170–174.
    1. Collarrusso RP, Hammill DD. Motor-free visual perception test. Finland: Academic Therapy Publications; 1972.
    1. Malin AJ. Malin's intelligence scale for Indian children (MISIC) Indian Journal of Mental Retardation. 1969;4:15–25.
    1. De Renzi E, Vignolo L. The token test: A sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain. 1962;85:665–678. doi: 10.1093/brain/85.4.665.
    1. Maj M, D' Elia L, Satz P, Janssen R. Evaluation of two new neuropsychological tests designed to minimize cultural bias in the assessment of HIV-1 seropositive persons: A WHO study. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 1993;8:123–135. doi: 10.1016/0887-6177(93)90030-5.
    1. Vakil E, Blachstein H, Sheinman M, Rey AVLT. Development of norms for children and sensitivity of different measures to age. Child Neuropsychol. 1998;4:161–177.
    1. Jones-Gotman M. Memory for designs: the hippocampal contribution. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24:193–203. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90052-7.
    1. Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosur Ps. 1954;20:11–21.
    1. Agarwal KN, Agarwal DK, Upadhyay SK. Impact of chronic undernutrition on higher mental functions in Indian boys aged 10–12 years. Acta Paediatr. 1995;84:1357–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1995.tb13568.x.
    1. Gorman KS. Malnutrition and cognitive development: evidence from experimental/quasi experimental studies among the mild-moderately malnourished. J Nutr. 1995;125:2239S–2244S.
    1. Grantham-McGregor S. A review of studies of the effect of severe malnutrition on mental development. J Nutr. 1995;125:2333S–2238S.
    1. Chang SM, Walker SP, Grantham-McGregor S, Powell CA. Early childhood stunting and later behaviour and school achievement. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2002;43:775–783. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00088.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe