Decision-Making Processes Related to Perseveration Are Indirectly Associated With Weight Status in Children Through Laboratory-Assessed Energy Intake

Bari A Fuchs, Nicole J Roberts, Shana Adise, Alaina L Pearce, Charles F Geier, Corey White, Zita Oravecz, Kathleen L Keller, Bari A Fuchs, Nicole J Roberts, Shana Adise, Alaina L Pearce, Charles F Geier, Corey White, Zita Oravecz, Kathleen L Keller

Abstract

Decision-making contributes to what and how much we consume, and deficits in decision-making have been associated with increased weight status in children. Nevertheless, the relationships between cognitive and affective processes underlying decision-making (i.e., decision-making processes) and laboratory food intake are unclear. We used data from a four-session, within-subjects laboratory study to investigate the relationships between decision-making processes, food intake, and weight status in 70 children 7-to-11-years-old. Decision-making was assessed with the Hungry Donkey Task (HDT), a child-friendly task where children make selections with unknown reward outcomes. Food intake was measured with three paradigms: (1) a standard ad libitum meal, (2) an eating in the absence of hunger (EAH) protocol, and (3) a palatable buffet meal. Individual differences related to decision-making processes during the HDT were quantified with a reinforcement learning model. Path analyses were used to test whether decision-making processes that contribute to children's (a) expected value of a choice and (b) tendency to perseverate (i.e., repeatedly make the same choice) were indirectly associated with weight status through their effects on intake (kcal). Results revealed that increases in the tendency to perseverate after a gain outcome were positively associated with intake at all three paradigms and indirectly associated with higher weight status through intake at both the standard and buffet meals. Increases in the tendency to perseverate after a loss outcome were positively associated with EAH, but only in children whose tendency to perseverate persistedacross trials. Results suggest that decision-making processes that shape children's tendencies to repeat a behavior (i.e., perseverate) are related to laboratory energy intake across multiple eating paradigms. Children who are more likely to repeat a choice after a positive outcome have a tendency to eat more at laboratory meals. If this generalizes to contexts outside the laboratory, these children may be susceptible to obesity. By using a reinforcement learning model not previously applied to the study of eating behaviors, this study elucidated potential determinants of excess energy intake in children, which may be useful for the development of childhood obesity interventions.

Keywords: Hungry Donkey Task; childhood obesity; children; decision-making; eating behavior.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Copyright © 2021 Fuchs, Roberts, Adise, Pearce, Geier, White, Oravecz and Keller.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Trays of food and drinks presented during the three eating paradigms. (A) Standard Meal: (left tray) water, angel food cake, grapes, (right tray) broccoli, garlic bread, cherry tomatoes, macaroni and cheese; (B) Eating in the Absence of Hunger protocol: (left tray) chocolate kisses, buttered popcorn, nacho-flavored tortilla chips, fruit-flavored candies, chocolate chip cookies, (right tray) pretzels, fudge brownies, potato chips, chocolate candies, cheese crackers; (C) Buffet meal: (left tray) fruit-flavored candies, potato chips, donut holes, chocolate chip cookies, cheese bagel bites, strawberry licorice twists, (middle tray) fruit punch, chocolate cupcake, chocolate milk, cheese pizza rolls, strawberry fruit leather, (right tray) mozzarella sticks, gummy candy, fudge brownies, chicken nuggets.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Hungry Donkey Task. During each trial of the task, children were presented with a selection screen (A). During the selection screen, children selected one door by using one of four keyboard keys (C, V, B, N) that corresponded to each door from left to right. Following a selection, children were presented with an outcome screen (B). The number of apples won and lost during that trial were displayed in the frame of the selected door as green and red apples, respectively, and numerically as “profit” and “loss” values under the vertical bar. The vertical bar provided global feedback about the ratio of apples won (green) and lost (red) in the game so far, and the net total amount of apples won in the game so far was indicated under the doors.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Final expected value model for the standard meal with pre-standard meal fullness covariate. Expected value models include VPP model parameters involved in computing expected value. For path analyses, VPP model parameters were normalized and intake (kcal) was scaled by a factor of 100. Pre-standard meal fullness was rated on a 150 mm visual analog scale prior to the eating paradigm. Arrows indicate paths tested in the final model and are labeled with the unstandardized coefficient (B) and standard error for that path. Dotted lines indicate paths did not reach statistical significance (p > 0.05). Solid lines indicate statistically significant paths (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001). Explained variance (R2) is reported for endogenous variables.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Final perseveration model for the (A) Standard Meal, (B) Eating in the Absence of Hunger (EAH) protocol, and (C) Buffet meal. Perseveration models contain VPP model parameters involved in computing perseveration strength. For path analyses, VPP model parameters were normalized and intake (kcal) was scaled by a factor of 100. Arrows indicate paths tested in the final model and are labeled with the unstandardized parameter estimate (B) and standard error for that path. Dotted lines indicate paths did not reach statistical significance (p > 0.05). Solid lines indicate statistically significant paths (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001). Explained variance (R2) is reported for endogenous variables.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
Relationship between the impact of gain on perseveration strength (i.e., εpos; x-axis) and intake (kcal; y-axis) during the (A) Standard meal, (B) Eating in the Absence of Hunger (EAH) protocol, and (C) Buffet meal. Blue lines reflect the best fit for the linear model between εpos and intake. Shaded gray regions reflects 95% confidence interval for the line of best fit.
FIGURE 6
FIGURE 6
Relationship between the impact of loss on perseveration strength (i.e., εneg) and intake (kcal) during the Eating in the Absence of Hunger (EAH) protocol at three levels of perseveration decay (i.e., k). (A) Three overlapping intervals of k that correspond to the three scatterplots in panel (B). (B) Scatterplots between εneg (x-axis) and EAH (y-axis). Normalized and raw values of εneg and k are presented. Left scatter plot: at the lower interval of k (normalized values: −2.15 to 0.03), the association between εneg and intake is negative. Middle scatter plot: at the middle interval of k (normalized values: −0.72 to 0.74), the association between εneg and intake is negative, although less negative than the lower interval. Right scatter plot: at the higher interval of k (normalized values: 0.08 to 2.39), the association between εneg and intake is positive.

References

    1. Adise S., Geier C. F., Roberts N. J., White C. N., Keller K. L. (2018). Is brain response to food rewards related to overeating? A test of the reward surfeit model of overeating in children. Appetite 128 167–179. 10.1016/j.appet.2018.06.014
    1. Adise S., Geier C. F., Roberts N. J., White C. N., Keller K. L. (2019). Food or money? Children’s brains respond differently to rewards regardless of weight status. Pediatric Obesity 14:e12469. 10.1111/ijpo.12469
    1. Adise S, White C. N., Roberts N. J., Geier C. F., Keller K. L. (2021). Children’s inhibitory control abilities in the presence of rewards are related to weight status and eating in the absence of hunger. Appetite 105610. (in press). 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105610
    1. Ahn W.-Y., Haines N., Zhang L. (2017). Revealing neurocomputational mechanisms of reinforcement learning and decision-making with the hbayesDM package. Comput. Psychiatry 1 24–57. 10.1162/CPSY_a_00002
    1. Anderson P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 8:71. 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724
    1. Bechara A., Damasio A. R., Damasio H., Anderson S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50 7–15. 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3
    1. Beitz K. M., Salthouse T. A., Davis H. P. (2014). Performance on the iowa gambling task: from 5 to 89 years of age. J. Exp. Psychol. General 143 1677–1689. 10.1037/a0035823
    1. Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Statist. Soc. Series B 57 289–300. 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
    1. Bentler P. M. (2006). EQS 6 Structural Equations Program Manual. Los Angeles, CA: University of California.
    1. Bentler P. M., Chou C.-P. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling: sociological. Methods Res. 16 78–117. 10.1177/0049124187016001004
    1. Bernstein H., Santos M. (2018). Definitions of hunger and fullness among youth enrolled in a pediatric weight management program. Clin. Nut. ESPEN 23 167–170. 10.1016/j.clnesp.2017.10.006
    1. Birch L. L. (1979). Dimensions of preschool children’s food preferences. J. Nutr. Educ. 11 77–80. 10.1016/S0022-3182(79)80089-8
    1. Brosseau-Liard P., Savalei V., Li L. (2012). An investigation of the sample performance of two nonnormality corrections for RMSEA. Multivariate Behav. Res. 47 904–930. 10.1080/00273171.2012.715252
    1. Brosseau-Liard P. E., Savalei V. (2014). Adjusting incremental fit indices for nonnormality. Multivariate Behav. Res. 49 460–470. 10.1080/00273171.2014.933697
    1. Busemeyer J. R., Stout J. C. (2002). A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task. Psychol. Asses. 14 253–262. 10.1037/1040-3590.14.3.253
    1. Buttelmann F., Karbach J. (2017). Development and plasticity of cognitive flexibility in early and middle childhood. Front. Psychol. 8:1040. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01040
    1. Carneiro I. P., Elliott S. A., Siervo M., Padwal R., Bertoli S., Battezzati A., et al. (2016). Is obesity associated with altered energy expenditure? Adv. Nut. 7 476–487. 10.3945/an.115.008755
    1. Casey B. J., Getz S., Galvan A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Dev. Rev. 28 62–77. 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003
    1. Cassotti M., Houdé O., Moutier S. (2011). Developmental changes of win-stay and loss-shift strategies in decision making. Child Neuropsychol. 17 400–411. 10.1080/09297049.2010.547463
    1. Catania A. C. (1979). Learning. Hoboken, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
    1. Cole T. J., Bellizzi M. C., Flegal K. M., Dietz W. H. (2000). Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: international survey. BMJ 320:1240. 10.1136/bmj.320.7244.1240
    1. Crone E. A., van der Molen M. W. (2004). Developmental changes in real life decision making: performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dev. Neuropsychol. 25 251–279. 10.1207/s15326942dn2503_2
    1. Cserjési R., Molnár D., Luminet O., Lénárd L. (2007). Is there any relationship between obesity and mental flexibility in children? Appetite 49 675–678. 10.1016/j.appet.2007.04.001
    1. Danner U. N., Ouwehand C., van Haastert N. L., Hornsveld H., de Ridder D. T. D. (2012). Decision-making impairments in women with binge eating disorder in comparison with obese and normal weight women. Eur. Eating Dis. Rev. J. Eating Dis. Assoc. 20 e56–e62. 10.1002/erv.1098
    1. Davidson M. C., Amso D., Anderson L. C., Diamond A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia 44 2037–2078. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006
    1. Dawe S., Loxton N. J. (2004). The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28 343–351. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.007
    1. De Decker A., Sioen I., Verbeken S., Braet C., Michels N., De Henauw S. (2016). Associations of reward sensitivity with food consumption, activity pattern, and BMI in children. Appetite 100 189–196. 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.028
    1. Eichen D. M., Matheson B. E., Liang J., Strong D. R., Rhee K., Boutelle K. N. (2018). The relationship between executive functioning and weight loss and maintenance in children and parents participating in family-based treatment for childhood obesity. Behav. Res. Ther. 105 10–16. 10.1016/j.brat.2018.03.010
    1. Epstein L. H., Carr K. A., Scheid J. L., Gebre E., O’Brien A., Paluch R. A., et al. (2015). Taste and food reinforcement in non-overweight youth. Appetite 91 226–232. 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.050
    1. Epstein L. H., Roemmich J. N., Raynor H. A. (2001). Behavioral therapy in the treatment of pediatric obesity. Pediatric Clin. North Am. 48 981–993. 10.1016/S0031-3955(05)70352-7
    1. Ernst M., Paulus M. P. (2005). Neurobiology of decision making: a selective review from a neurocognitive and clinical perspective. Biol. Psychiatry 58 597–604. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.004
    1. Farruggia M. C., Small D. M. (2019). Effects of adiposity and metabolic dysfunction on cognition: a review. Physiol. Behav. 208:112578. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112578
    1. Fisher J. O., Birch L. L. (1999). Restricting access to foods and children’s eating. Appetite 32 405–419. 10.1006/appe.1999.0231
    1. Grace J. B., Bollen K. A. (2005). Interpreting the results from multiple regression and structural equation models. Bull. Ecol. Soc. Am. 86 283–295. 10.1890/0012-9623(2005)86[283:itrfmr];2
    1. Graham A. L., Gluck M. E., Votruba S. B., Krakoff J., Thearle M. S. (2014). Perseveration augments the effects of cognitive restraint on ad libitum food intake in adults seeking weight loss. Appetite 82 78–84. 10.1016/j.appet.2014.07.008
    1. Groppe K., Elsner B. (2014). Executive function and food approach behavior in middle childhood. Front. Psychol. 5:447. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00447
    1. Groppe K., Elsner B. (2017). Executive function and weight status in children: a one-year longitudinal perspective. Child Neuropsychol. 23 129–147. 10.1080/09297049.2015.1089981
    1. Ha O.-R., Lim S.-L., Bruce A. S. (2020). Neural mechanisms of food decision-making in children. Curr. Nut. Rep. 9 236–250. 10.1007/s13668-020-00321-5
    1. Hartley C. A., Phelps E. A. (2012). Anxiety and decision-making. Biol. Psychiatry 72 113–118. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.027
    1. Hill C., Saxton J., Webber L., Blundell J., Wardle J. (2009). The relative reinforcing value of food predicts weight gain in a longitudinal study of 710-y-old children. Am. J. Clin. Nut. 90 276–281. 10.3945/ajcn.2009.27479
    1. Hooper D., Coughlan J., Mullen M. (2008). Structural equation modelling: guidelines for determining model fit. Electr. J. Bus. Res. Methods 6 53–60. 10.21427/D7CF7R
    1. Jelalian E. (1999). Empirically supported treatments in pediatric psychology: pediatric obesity. J. Pediatric Psychol. 24 223–248. 10.1093/jpepsy/24.3.223
    1. Jonker N. C., van Malderen E., Glashouwer K. A., Vervoort L., Braet C., Goossens L., et al. (2019). No differential reward responsivity and drive, punishment sensitivity or attention for cues signaling reward or punishment in adolescents with obesity. Front. Psychol. 10:2363. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02363
    1. Keller K. L., Assur S. A., Torres M., Lofink H. E., Thornton J. C., Faith M. S., et al. (2006). Potential of an analog scaling device for measuring fullness in children: development and preliminary testing. Appetite 47 233–243. 10.1016/j.appet.2006.04.004
    1. Keller K. L., English L. K., Fearnbach S. N., Lasschuijt M., Anderson K., Bermudez M., et al. (2018). Brain response to food cues varying in portion size is associated with individual differences in the portion size effect in children. Appetite 125 139–151. 10.1016/j.appet.2018.01.027
    1. Koritzky G., Rice C., Dieterle C., Bechara A. (2015). The biggest loser thinks long-term: recency as a predictor of success in weight management. Front. Psychol. 6:1864. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01864
    1. Lensing N., Elsner B. (2017). Overweight and normal-weight children’s decision-making in a child variant of the iowa gambling task. Child Dev. Res. 2017 1–9. 10.1155/2017/1285320
    1. Lim S.-L., Cherry J. B. C., Davis A. M., Balakrishnan S. N., Ha O.-R., Bruce J. M., et al. (2016). The child brain computes and utilizes internalized maternal choices. Nat. Commun. 7:11700. 10.1038/ncomms11700
    1. Macchi R., MacKew L., Davis C. (2017). Is decision-making ability related to food choice and facets of eating behaviour in adolescents? Appetite 116 442–455. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.031
    1. Masterson T. D., Bermudez M. A., Austen M., Lundquist E., Pearce A. L., Bruce A. S., et al. (2019). Food commercials do not affect energy intake in a laboratory meal but do alter brain responses to visual food cues in children. Appetite 132 154–165. 10.1016/j.appet.2018.10.010
    1. Mead E., Brown T., Rees K., Azevedo L. B., Whittaker V., Jones D., et al. (2017). Diet, physical activity and behavioural interventions for the treatment of overweight or obese children from the age of 6 to 11 years. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 6:CD012651. 10.1002/14651858.CD012651
    1. Michels N., Sioen I., Braet C., Eiben G., Hebestreit A., Huybrechts I., et al. (2012). Stress, emotional eating behaviour and dietary patterns in children. Appetite 59 762–769. 10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.010
    1. Moore C. F., Sabino V., Koob G. F., Cottone P. (2017). Pathological overeating: emerging evidence for a compulsivity construct. Neuropsychopharmacology 42 1375–1389. 10.1038/npp.2016.269
    1. Nazarboland N., Fath N. (2015). The role of BMI in predicting emotion-driven impulsivity and sensitivity to reward/punishment in over-obese adolescents. Biomed. Pharmacol. J. 8 729–737. 10.13005/bpj/776
    1. Nederkoorn C., Braet C., Van Eijs Y., Tanghe A., Jansen A. (2006). Why obese children cannot resist food: The role of impulsivity. Eating Behav. 7 315–322. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.11.005
    1. Ogden J., Roy-Stanley C. (2020). How do children make food choices? Using a think-aloud method to explore the role of internal and external factors on eating behaviour. Appetite 147:104551. 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104551
    1. Pearce A. L., Adise S., Roberts N. J., White C., Geier C. F., Keller K. L. (2020). Individual differences in the influence of taste and health impact successful dietary self-control: a mouse tracking food choice study in children. Physiol. Behav. 223:112990. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112990
    1. R Core Team (2021). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria; ). Available online at: .
    1. Rangel A. (2013). Regulation of dietary choice by the decision-making circuitry. Nat. Neurosci. 16 1717–1724. 10.1038/nn.3561
    1. Rangel A., Hare T. (2010). Neural computations associated with goal-directed choice. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20 262–270. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.001
    1. Reilly J. J. (2005). Descriptive epidemiology and health consequences of childhood obesity. Best Practice Res. Clin. Endocrinol. Meta. 19 327–341. 10.1016/j.beem.2005.04.002
    1. Rolls B. J., Van Duijvenvoorde P. M., Rolls E. T. (1984). Pleasantness changes and food intake in a varied four-course meal. Appetite 5 337–348. 10.1016/S0195-6663(84)80006-9
    1. Rosseel Y. (2012). lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling and More Version 0.5-12 (BETA).
    1. Satorra A., Bentler P. M. (1988). “Scaling corrections for chi-square statistics in covariance structure analysis,” in Proceeding of the American Statistical Association 1988 Proceedings of Business and Economics Sections, 308–313.
    1. Satorra A., Bentler P. M. (1994). “Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis,” in Latent Variables Analysis: Applications for Developmental Research, eds von Eye A., Clogg C. C. (Sage Publications, Inc; ), 399–419.
    1. Skinner A. C., Ravanbakht S. N., Skelton J. A., Perrin E. M., Armstrong S. C. (2018). Prevalence of obesity and severe obesity in US children, 1999–2016. J. Am. Acad. Pediatrics 141:e20173459. 10.1542/peds.2017-3459
    1. Smiciklas-Wright H., Mitchell D. C., Mickle S. J., Goldman J. D., Cook A. (2003). Foods commonly eaten in the United States, 1989-1991 and 1994-1996: Are portion sizes changing? J. Am. Dietetic Assoc. 103 41–47. 10.1053/jada.2003.50000
    1. Steinbeis N., Crone E. A. (2016). The link between cognitive control and decision-making across child and adolescent development. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 10 28–32. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.04.009
    1. Steinbeis N., Haushofer J., Fehr E., Singer T. (2016). Development of behavioral control and associated vmPFC–DLPFC connectivity explains children’s increased resistance to temptation in intertemporal choice. Cerebral Cortex 26 32–42. 10.1093/cercor/bhu167
    1. Streiner D. L. (2005). Finding our way: an introduction to path analysis. Canad. J. Psychiatry 50 115–122. 10.1177/070674370505000207
    1. Temple J. L., Legierski C. M., Giacomelli A. M., Salvy S.-J., Epstein L. H. (2008). Overweight children find food more reinforcing and consume more energy than do nonoverweight children. Am. J. Clin. Nut. 87 1121–1127. 10.1093/ajcn/87.5.1121
    1. van Meer F., van der Laan L. N., Viergever M. A., Adan R. A. H., Smeets P. A. M. (2017). Considering healthiness promotes healthier choices but modulates medial prefrontal cortex differently in children compared with adults. NeuroImage 159 325–333. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.007
    1. Verbeken S., Braet C., Bosmans G., Goossens L. (2014). Comparing decision making in average and overweight children and adolescents. Int. J. Obesity 38 547–551. 10.1038/ijo.2013.235
    1. Verbeken S., Braet C., Claus L., Nederkoorn C., Oosterlaan J. (2009). Childhood obesity and impulsivity: an investigation with performance-based measures. Behav. Change 26:153. 10.1375/bech.26.3.153
    1. Verdejo-García A., Pérez-Expósito M., Schmidt-Río-Valle J., Fernández-Serrano M. J., Cruz F., Pérez-García M., et al. (2010). Selective alterations within executive functions in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity 18 1572–1578. 10.1038/oby.2009.475
    1. West S. G., Finch J. F., Curran P. J. (1995). “Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: problems and remedies,” in Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues, and Applications, ed. Hoyle R. H. (Sage Publications, Inc; ), 56–75.
    1. Worthy D. A., Hawthorne M. J., Otto A. R. (2013a). Heterogeneity of strategy use in the iowa gambling task: a comparison of win-stay/lose-shift and reinforcement learning models. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 20 364–371. 10.3758/s13423-012-0324-9
    1. Worthy D. A., Maddox W. T. (2012). Age-based differences in strategy use in choice tasks. Front. Neurosci. 5:145. 10.3389/fnins.2011.00145
    1. Worthy D. A., Pang B., Byrne K. A. (2013b). Decomposing the roles of perseveration and expected value representation in models of the iowa gambling task. Front. Psychol. 4:640. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00640
    1. Yeomans M. (2000). Rating changes over the course of meals: what do they tell us about motivation to eat? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24 249–259. 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00078-0

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe