Randomized trial of iReadMore word reading training and brain stimulation in central alexia

Zoe V J Woodhead, Sheila J Kerry, Oscar M Aguilar, Yean-Hoon Ong, John S Hogan, Katerina Pappa, Alex P Leff, Jennifer T Crinion, Zoe V J Woodhead, Sheila J Kerry, Oscar M Aguilar, Yean-Hoon Ong, John S Hogan, Katerina Pappa, Alex P Leff, Jennifer T Crinion

Abstract

Central alexia is an acquired reading disorder co-occurring with a generalized language deficit (aphasia). We tested the impact of a novel training app, 'iReadMore', and anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the left inferior frontal gyrus, on word reading ability in central alexia. The trial was registered at www.clinicaltrials.gov (NCT02062619). Twenty-one chronic stroke patients with central alexia participated. A baseline-controlled, repeated-measures, crossover design was used. Participants completed two 4-week blocks of iReadMore training, one with anodal stimulation and one with sham stimulation (order counterbalanced between participants). Each block comprised 34 h of iReadMore training and 11 stimulation sessions. Outcome measures were assessed before, between and after the two blocks. The primary outcome measures were reading ability for trained and untrained words. Secondary outcome measures included semantic word matching, sentence reading, text reading and a self-report measure. iReadMore training resulted in an 8.7% improvement in reading accuracy for trained words (95% confidence interval 6.0 to 11.4; Cohen's d = 1.38) but did not generalize to untrained words. Reaction times also improved. Reading accuracy gains were still significant (but reduced) 3 months after training cessation. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (compared to sham), delivered concurrently with iReadMore, resulted in a 2.6% (95% confidence interval -0.1 to 5.3; d = 0.41) facilitation for reading accuracy, both for trained and untrained words. iReadMore also improved performance on the semantic word-matching test. There was a non-significant trend towards improved self-reported reading ability. However, no significant changes were seen at the sentence or text reading level. In summary, iReadMore training in post-stroke central alexia improved reading ability for trained words, with good maintenance of the therapy effect. Anodal stimulation resulted in a small facilitation (d = 0.41) of learning and also generalized to untrained items.10.1093/brain/awy138_video1awy138media15796149281001.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Study design. G1 = Group1: received tDCS in Block 1 and sham in Block 2. G2 = Group2: received sham in Block 1 and tDCS in Block 2.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) flow diagram..
Figure 3
Figure 3
Patient structural MRI images and lesion overlap map. Crosshairs indicate the approximate location of the stimulation site. Bottom right tiles show the lesion overlay map with voxels where at least two patients had damage. The highest lesion overlap (n = 20) was seen in two areas: (i) the superior longitudinal fasciculus underlying the supramarginal gyrus; and (ii) the junction of the superior longitudinal, inferior longitudinal and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculi underlying the posterior superior temporal sulcus.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Therapy effects on word reading ability. Change over time in (A) mean word reading accuracy (n = 21) and (B) reaction times (n = 20). There were four different word lists: words trained in Block 1 (blue), words trained in Block 2 (red), untrained words (black) and the unmatched list of high-frequency, low-imageability core words (purple). Error bars indicate within-subject standard error of the mean (SEM). Training Block 1 was administered between T3 and T4; Block 2 was administered between T4 and T5.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Change in word reading ability after therapy. Effects of iReadMore and tDCS on change in (A) word reading accuracy (n = 21) and (B) word reading reaction times (RT) (n = 20). Block 1 change was calculated as accuracy or reaction time at T4 − T3; Block 2 change was T5 − T4. G1 = cross-over group 1; G2 = cross-over group 2. Error bars represent the within-subject SEM.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Change in word reading accuracy and self-reported reading by participant. (A) Raw percentage change in word reading accuracy for trained (black) and untrained (grey) words, averaged over Block 1 (T4 − T3) and Block 2 (T5 − T4). For trained words, this represents the average of the change in the 90 words trained in Block 1 between T3 and T4 and the change in the 90 (different) words trained in Block 2 between T4 and T5. For untrained words, this represents the change in the 90 untrained words over the same two time-periods. Participants are ordered according to tDCS group, followed by ascending CAT naming accuracy. (B) The CDP measures self-report ability in silent word, sentence, text and mail reading. Score for each level is out of 4, giving a total score out of 16. Change in CDP score is the difference between T5 (after training) minus T3 (before training). Positive scores represent improvements in self-reported reading ability. CDP data were unavailable for Patient P4.

References

    1. Adair JC, Nadeau SE, Conway TW, Gonzalez-Rothi LJ, Heilman PC, Green IA, et al. Alterations in the functional anatomy of reading induced by rehabilitation of an alexic patient. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 2000; 13: 303–11.
    1. Baker JM, Rorden C, Fridriksson J. Using transcranial direct-current stimulation to treat stroke patients with aphasia. Stroke 2010; 41: 1229–36.
    1. Brookshire CE, Conway T, Pompon RH, Oelke M, Kendall DL. Effects of intensive phonomotor treatment on reading in eight individuals with aphasia and phonological alexia. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2014a; 23(Suppl): S300–11.
    1. Brookshire CE, Wilson JP, Nadeau SE, Gonzalez Rothi LJ, Kendall DL. Frequency, nature, and predictors of alexia in a convenience sample of individuals with chronic aphasia. Aphasiology 2014b; 28: 1464–80.
    1. Brown J, Hux K, Fairbanks S. Reading recovery: a case study using a multicomponent treatment for acquired alexia. Aphasiology 2015; 30: 23–44.
    1. Brysbaert M, New B. Moving beyond Kucera and Francis: a critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behav Res Methods 2009; 41: 977–90.
    1. Callaghan MF, Josephs O, Herbst M, Zaitsev M, Todd N, Weiskopf N. An evaluation of prospective motion correction for high resolution quantitative MRI. Front Neurosci 2015; 9: 97.
    1. Campana S, Caltagirone C, Marangolo P. Combining voxel-based lesion symptom mapping (VLSM) with A-tDCS language treatment: predicting outcome of recovery in nonfluent chronic aphasia. Brain Stimul 2015; 8: 769–76.
    1. Cherney LR. Aphasia, alexia, and oral reading. Top Stroke Rehabil 2004; 11: 22–36.
    1. Coltheart M. Acquired dyslexias and the computational modelling of reading. Cogn Neuropsychol 2006; 23: 96–109.
    1. Coltheart M, Rastle K, Perry C, Langdon R, Ziegler J. DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol Rev 2001; 108: 204–56.
    1. Conway T, Heilman P, Rothi L, Alexander AW, Adair J, Crosson BA, et al. Treatment of a case of phonological alexia with agraphia using the Auditory Discrimination in Depth (ADD) program. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1998; 4: 608–20.
    1. Cornelissen PL, Kringelbach ML, Ellis AW, Whitney C, Holliday IE, Hansen PC. Activation of the left ineferior frontal gyrus in the first 200 ms of reading: evidence from magnetoencephalography (MEG). PLoS One 2009; 4: e5359.
    1. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH. Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 1196–208.
    1. Crisp J, Howard D, Lambon Ralph MA. More evidence for a continuum between phonological and deep dyslexia: novel data from three measures of direct orthography-to-phonology translation. Aphasiology 2011; 25: 615–41.
    1. Crisp J, Lambon Ralph MA. Unlocking the nature of the phonological? Deep dyslexia continuum: the keys to reading aloud are in phonology and semantics. J Cogn Neurosci 2006; 18: 348–62.
    1. Datta A, Zhou X, Su Y, Parra LC, Bikson M. Validation of finite element model of transcranial electrical stimulation using scalp potentials: implications for clinical dose. J Neural Eng 2013; 10: 036018.
    1. De Partz MP. Re-education of a deep dyslexic patient: rationale of the method and results. Cogn Neuropsychol 1986; 3: 149–77.
    1. Ellis A, Young A. Human cognitive neuropsychology: a textbook with readings. Hove: Psychology Press; 1988.
    1. Fertonani A, Miniussi C. Transcranial electrical stimulation: what we know and do not know about mechanisms. Neuroscientist 2017; 23: 109–23.
    1. Fregni F, Nitsche MA, Loo CK, Brunoni AR, Marangolo P, Leite J, et al. Regulatory considerations for the clinical and research use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): review and recommendations from an expert panel. Clin Res Regul Aff 2015; 32: 22–35
    1. Friedman RB, Lott SN. Successful blending in a phonological reading treatment for deep alexia. Aphasiology 2002; 16: 355–72.
    1. Friedman RB, Robinson SR. Whole-word training therapy in a stable surface alexic patient: it works. Aphasiology 1991; 5: 521–8.
    1. Friedman RB, Sample DM, Lott SN. The role of level of representation in the use of paired associate learning for rehabilitation of alexia. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 223–34.
    1. Fritsch B, Reis J, Martinowich K, Schambra HM, Ji Y, Cohen LG, et al. Direct current stimulation promotes BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications for motor learning. Neuron 2010; 66: 198–204.
    1. Gandiga PC, Hummel FC, Cohen LG. Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol 2006; 117: 845–50.
    1. Hickok G, Poeppel D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 2007; 8: 393–402.
    1. Hoffman P, Lambon Ralph MA, Woollams AM. Triangulation of the neurocomputational architecture underpinning reading aloud. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2015; 112: E3719–28.
    1. Kendall DL, Conway T, Rosenbek J, Gonzalez-Rothi L. Case study phonological rehabilitation of acquired phonologic alexia. Aphasiology 2003; 17: 1073–95.
    1. Kendall DL, McNeil MR, Small SL. Rule-based treatment for acquired phonological dyslexia. Aphasiology 1998; 12: 587–600.
    1. Keuleers E, Brysbaert M. Wuggy: a multilingual pseudoword generator. Behav Res Methods 2010; 42: 627–33.
    1. Kim M, Beaudoin-Parsons D. Training phonological reading in deep alexia: does it improve reading words with low imageability? Clin Linguist Phon 2007; 21: 321–51.
    1. Kiran S, Thompson C, Hashimoto N. Training grapheme to phoneme conversion in patients with oral reading and naming deficits: a model-based approach. Aphasiology 2001; 15: 855–76.
    1. Kurland J, Cortes CR, Wilke M, Sperling AJ, Lott SN, Tagamets MA, et al. Neural mechanisms underlying learning following semantic mediation treatment in a case of phonologic alexia. Brain Imaging Behav 2008; 2: 147–62.
    1. Lacey EH, Jiang X, Friedman RB, Snider SF, Parra LC, Huang Y. Transcranial direct current stimulation for pure alexia: effects on brain and behavior. Brain Stimul 2015; 8: 305–9.
    1. Leff AP, Behrmann M. Treatment of reading impairment after stroke. Neurology 2008; 21: 644–8.
    1. Lott SN, Sample DM, Oliver RT, Lacey EH, Friedman RB. A patient with phonologic alexia can learn to read “much” from “mud pies”. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46: 2515–23.
    1. Marangolo P, Fiori V, Calpagnano MA, Campana S, Razzano C, Caltagirone C, et al. tDCS over the left inferior frontal cortex improves speech production in aphasia. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7: 539.
    1. Marangolo P, Marinelli CV, Bonifazi S, Fiori V, Ceravolo MG, Provinciali L, et al. Electrical stimulation over the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) determines long-term effects in the recovery of speech apraxia in three chronic aphasics. Behav Brain Res 2011; 225: 498–504.
    1. Meinzer M, Darkow R, Lindenberg R, Flöel A. Electrical stimulation of the motor cortex enhances treatment outcome in post-stroke aphasia. Brain 2016; 139: 1152–63.
    1. Mitchum C, Berndt R. Diagnosis and treatment of the non-lexical route in acquired dyslexia: an illustration of the cognitive neuropsychological approach. J Neurolinguistics 1991; 6: 103–37.
    1. Moyer SB. Rehabilitation of alexia: a case study. Cortex 1979; 15: 139–44.
    1. Neale MD. The Neale analysis of reading ability. 3rd ednMelbourne, Australia: ACER; 1999.
    1. Nickels L. The autocue? Self-generated phonemic cues in the treatment of a disorder of reading and naming. Cogn Neuropsychol 1992; 9: 155–82.
    1. Parkin BL, Ekhtiari H, Walsh VF. Non-invasive human brain stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: a primer. Neuron 2015; 87: 932–45.
    1. Patterson K, Lambon Ralph MA. Selective disorders of reading? Curr Opin Neurobiol 1999; 9: 235–9.
    1. Plaut DC, McClelland JL, Seidenberg MS, Patterson K. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychol Rev 1996; 103: 56–115.
    1. Reis J, Fischer JT, Prichard G, Weiller C, Cohen LG, Fritsch B. Time- but not sleep-dependent consolidation of tDCS-enhanced visuomotor skills. Cereb Cortex 2015; 25: 109–17.
    1. Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, et al. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009; 106: 1590–5.
    1. Riley EA, Thompson CK. Training pseudoword reading in acquired dyslexia: a phonological complexity approach. Aphasiology 2014; 29: 129–50.
    1. Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, Baddeley BT, Yiend J. ‘Oops!’: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 1997; 35: 747–58.
    1. Schlaug G, Renga V, Nair D. Transcranial direct current stimulation in stroke recovery. Arch Neurol 2008; 65: 1571–6.
    1. Schneider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A. E-prime reference guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc; 2012.
    1. Seghier ML, Patel E, Prejawa S, Ramsden S, Selmer A, Lim L, et al. The PLORAS database: a data repository for predicting language outcome and recovery after stroke. Neuroimage 2016; 124: 1208–12.
    1. Seghier ML, Ramlackhansingh A, Crinion J, Leff AP, Price CJ. Lesion identification using unified segmentation-normalisation models and fuzzy clustering. Neuroimage 2008; 41: 1253–66.
    1. Ska B, Garneau-Beaumont D, Chesneau S, Damien B. Diagnosis and rehabilitation attempt of a patient with acquired deep dyslexia. Brain Cogn 2003; 53: 359–63.
    1. Stadie N, Rilling E. Evaluation of lexically and nonlexically based reading treatment in a deep dyslexic. Cogn Neuropsychol 2006; 23: 643–72.
    1. Stagg CJ, Nitsche MA. Physiological basis of transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroscientist 2011; 17: 37–53.
    1. Swinburn K, Byng S. The communication disability profile. London: Connect Press; 2006.
    1. Swinburn K, Porter G, Howard D. The comprehensive aphasia test. Hove: Psychology Press; 2005.
    1. Tsapkini K, Frangakis C, Gomez Y, Davis C, Hillis AE. Augmentation of spelling therapy with transcranial direct current stimulation in primary progressive aphasia: preliminary results and challenges. Aphasiology 2014; 28: 1112–30.
    1. Vestito L, Rosellini S, Mantero M, Bandini F. Long-term effects of transcranial direct-current stimulation in chronic post-stroke aphasia: a Pilot Study. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8: 785.
    1. Warrington EK, Shallice T. Word-form dyslexia. Brain 1980; 100: 99–112.
    1. Wheat KL, Cornelissen P, Frost SJ, Hansen P. During visual word recognition, phonology is accessed within 100 ms and may be mediated by a speech production code: evidence from magnetoencephalography. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 5229–33.
    1. Whitworth A, Webster J, Howard D. A cognitive neuropsychological approach to assessment and intervention in aphasia: a clinician's guide. 2nd edn. Hove: Psychology Press; 2014.
    1. Woodhead ZVJ, Barnes GR, Penny W, Moran R, Teki S, Price CJ, et al. Reading front to back: MEG evidence for early feedback effects during word recognition. Cereb Cortex 2014; 24: 817–25.
    1. Woodhead ZVJ, Penny W, Barnes GR, Crewes H, Wise RJ, Price CJ, et al. Reading therapy strengthens top-down connectivity in patients with pure alexia. Brain 2013; 136: 2579–91.
    1. Woollams AM, Lambon Ralph MA, Plaut DC, Patterson K. SD-squared: on the association between semantic dementia and surface dyslexia. Psychol Rev 2007; 114: 316–39.
    1. Yampolsky S, Waters G. Treatment of single word oral reading in an individual with deep dyslexia. Aphasiology 2002; 16: 455–71.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe