Subjective Cognitive Decline in Older Adults: An Overview of Self-Report Measures Used Across 19 International Research Studies

Laura A Rabin, Colette M Smart, Paul K Crane, Rebecca E Amariglio, Lorin M Berman, Mercé Boada, Rachel F Buckley, Gaël Chételat, Bruno Dubois, Kathryn A Ellis, Katherine A Gifford, Angela L Jefferson, Frank Jessen, Mindy J Katz, Richard B Lipton, Tobias Luck, Paul Maruff, Michelle M Mielke, José Luis Molinuevo, Farnia Naeem, Audrey Perrotin, Ronald C Petersen, Lorena Rami, Barry Reisberg, Dorene M Rentz, Steffi G Riedel-Heller, Shannon L Risacher, Octavio Rodriguez, Perminder S Sachdev, Andrew J Saykin, Melissa J Slavin, Beth E Snitz, Reisa A Sperling, Caroline Tandetnik, Wiesje M van der Flier, Michael Wagner, Steffen Wolfsgruber, Sietske A M Sikkes, Laura A Rabin, Colette M Smart, Paul K Crane, Rebecca E Amariglio, Lorin M Berman, Mercé Boada, Rachel F Buckley, Gaël Chételat, Bruno Dubois, Kathryn A Ellis, Katherine A Gifford, Angela L Jefferson, Frank Jessen, Mindy J Katz, Richard B Lipton, Tobias Luck, Paul Maruff, Michelle M Mielke, José Luis Molinuevo, Farnia Naeem, Audrey Perrotin, Ronald C Petersen, Lorena Rami, Barry Reisberg, Dorene M Rentz, Steffi G Riedel-Heller, Shannon L Risacher, Octavio Rodriguez, Perminder S Sachdev, Andrew J Saykin, Melissa J Slavin, Beth E Snitz, Reisa A Sperling, Caroline Tandetnik, Wiesje M van der Flier, Michael Wagner, Steffen Wolfsgruber, Sietske A M Sikkes

Abstract

Research increasingly suggests that subjective cognitive decline (SCD) in older adults, in the absence of objective cognitive dysfunction or depression, may be a harbinger of non-normative cognitive decline and eventual progression to dementia. Little is known, however, about the key features of self-report measures currently used to assess SCD. The Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (SCD-I) Working Group is an international consortium established to develop a conceptual framework and research criteria for SCD (Jessen et al., 2014, Alzheimers Dement 10, 844-852). In the current study we systematically compared cognitive self-report items used by 19 SCD-I Working Group studies, representing 8 countries and 5 languages. We identified 34 self-report measures comprising 640 cognitive self-report items. There was little overlap among measures- approximately 75% of measures were used by only one study. Wide variation existed in response options and item content. Items pertaining to the memory domain predominated, accounting for about 60% of items surveyed, followed by executive function and attention, with 16% and 11% of the items, respectively. Items relating to memory for the names of people and the placement of common objects were represented on the greatest percentage of measures (56% each). Working group members reported that instrument selection decisions were often based on practical considerations beyond the study of SCD specifically, such as availability and brevity of measures. Results document the heterogeneity of approaches across studies to the emerging construct of SCD. We offer preliminary recommendations for instrument selection and future research directions including identifying items and measure formats associated with important clinical outcomes.

Keywords: Cognition; cognitive complaints; dementia; early detection; memory complaints; mild cognitive impairment; preclinical Alzheimer’s disease; questionnaire; subjective cognition; subjective cognitive impairment; subjective memory complaints.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Summary of response types and options. Notes: *14 out of 34 questionnaires include multiple response types. **Includes: <10 each of “very poor” to “very well”; “major problems” to “no problems”; “no” to “most of the time”; “no” to “definitely”; “no” to “yes, a lot worse”; “no problems” to “always”; and “more often” to “about the same”. Includes one each of “no” to “yes, that worries me seriously”; “less than 6 months ago” to “more than two years ago”; “very gradually” to “suddenly”; “steadily worsened” to “got worse and then leveled off”; “not at all” to “highly”; “excellent” to “poor”; “no” to “very much”; agree/disagree; good/poor; same/worse; and suddenly/gradually”. ***Includes: “always” to “never”; “frequently” to “never”; “always or almost always” to “never or almost never”; “very often” to “never”, and “often” to “never”. †Includes: “better than ever before” to “worse than ever before”; “much better” to “much worse”; “very strong improvement” to “very sharp decline”; “ much improved” to “much worse”; “much better now” to “much poorer now”; and “better than when I was younger” to “definitely worse than when I was younger”.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Percentage of items by cognitive domain. Notes: Memory includes short-term, long-term, episodic, semantic, and prospective memory and learning new information; Attention includes basic, sustained, focused, and divided attention, working memory, concentration, processing speed, and alertness; Language includes expressive and receptive language, word finding, reading, and spelling; Executive includes executive functions such as organizing, planning, initiating, switching, multi-tasking, reasoning, problem solving, decision-making, impulsivity, and self-regulation; Calculation includes basic calculation and arithmetic; Orientation includes orientation to person, time, place, and situation; General refers to memory and other cognitive abilities grouped together in a single item; and Visuospatial includes visuoperception, route finding, and directional orientation.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe