Spatial Updating Depends on Gravity

Alexander Christoph Stahn, Martin Riemer, Thomas Wolbers, Anika Werner, Katharina Brauns, Stephane Besnard, Pierre Denise, Simone Kühn, Hanns-Christian Gunga, Alexander Christoph Stahn, Martin Riemer, Thomas Wolbers, Anika Werner, Katharina Brauns, Stephane Besnard, Pierre Denise, Simone Kühn, Hanns-Christian Gunga

Abstract

As we move through an environment the positions of surrounding objects relative to our body constantly change. Maintaining orientation requires spatial updating, the continuous monitoring of self-motion cues to update external locations. This ability critically depends on the integration of visual, proprioceptive, kinesthetic, and vestibular information. During weightlessness gravity no longer acts as an essential reference, creating a discrepancy between vestibular, visual and sensorimotor signals. Here, we explore the effects of repeated bouts of microgravity and hypergravity on spatial updating performance during parabolic flight. Ten healthy participants (four women, six men) took part in a parabolic flight campaign that comprised a total of 31 parabolas. Each parabola created about 20-25 s of 0 g, preceded and followed by about 20 s of hypergravity (1.8 g). Participants performed a visual-spatial updating task in seated position during 15 parabolas. The task included two updating conditions simulating virtual forward movements of different lengths (short and long), and a static condition with no movement that served as a control condition. Two trials were performed during each phase of the parabola, i.e., at 1 g before the start of the parabola, at 1.8 g during the acceleration phase of the parabola, and during 0 g. Our data demonstrate that 0 g and 1.8 g impaired pointing performance for long updating trials as indicated by increased variability of pointing errors compared to 1 g. In contrast, we found no support for any changes for short updating and static conditions, suggesting that a certain degree of task complexity is required to affect pointing errors. These findings are important for operational requirements during spaceflight because spatial updating is pivotal for navigation when vision is poor or unreliable and objects go out of sight, for example during extravehicular activities in space or the exploration of unfamiliar environments. Future studies should compare the effects on spatial updating during seated and free-floating conditions, and determine at which g-threshold decrements in spatial updating performance emerge.

Keywords: parabolic flight; precuneus; spaceflight; spatial navigation; spatial updating; vestibular system; weightlessness.

Copyright © 2020 Stahn, Riemer, Wolbers, Werner, Brauns, Besnard, Denise, Kühn and Gunga.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
The characteristic profile of a parabolic flight maneuver. At standard cruising altitude (about 12,000 ft) the aircraft is pulled up at a 47° angle, inducing a gravito-inertial acceleration (GIA) of 1.5–1.8 g, after which the engine’s thrust is limited to compensate air-drag, entering a phase of free-fall comparable to 0 g, and hence, weightlessness. This phase is completed by another phase of hypergravity before returning to 1 g again. After an initial test parabola, the maneuver is repeated a total of 30 times with 3–5 min breaks between parabolas and a longer (about 8 min) break after the 16th parabola.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Experimental setup in the Airbus A 310 Zero-G. Participants were buckled up in standard aircraft chairs with feet fixed to their ground floor with foot straps. The laptops were mounted to a plexiglass plate that was strapped to the participants’ upper legs that allowed them to maintain the same position throughout testing. Testing was performed during 15 parabolas, providing a total of 90 trials (30 trials during 1 g, 1.8 g, and 0 g, respectively). Photo credit: Novespace/ESA.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Experimental paradigm. Trials comprise an encoding phase (A), a delay phase (B), and a retrieval phase (C). Each trial started with a static presentation of the virtual environment and two objects located at two different positions during which participants had to memorize the location and identity of the objects. Next, all objects gradually sank into the ground until they completely disappeared. In the delay phase, participants either experienced a forward movement of 25 m or 45 m (updating trials) or remained at their position (static trials). In the subsequent retrieval phase, one of the two objects was presented in the center of the screen, and participants had to turn a 3D-arrow towards the object’s original position in the encoding phase.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Mean variable (A) and signed (B) pointing errors, and reaction time (RT; C) during 0 g, 1 g, and 1.8 g and different trial conditions (static, updating short, updating long). Variable pointing error was computed as the standard deviation of the signed pointing errors for each participant and each g-level and task condition using circular statistics. Data are estimated means and standard errors. Note that no contrasts were performed between task conditions for RTs because RTs were a logical consequence of task condition, i.e., the 3D arrow had to be moved a shorter angular distance for static and short updating trials compared to long updating trials (for details see “Behavioral and Statistical Analysis” in “Materials and Methods” section). *P < 0.05. **P < 0.01. ***P < 0.001. ##P < 0.01 for 0 g vs. 1 g. ###P < 0.01 for 0 g vs. 1 g. †P < 0.05 for 0 g vs. 1.8 g. ††P < 0.01 for 0 g vs. 1.8 g. ‡P < 0.05 for 1 g vs. 1.8 g.

References

    1. Anderson E. J., Mannan S. K., Rees G., Sumner P., Kennard C. (2010). Overlapping functional anatomy for working memory and visual search. Exp. Brain Res. 200, 91–107. 10.1007/s00221-009-2000-5
    1. André-Deshays C., Israël I., Charade O., Berthoz A., Popov K., Lipshits M. (1993). Gaze control in microgravity. 1. Saccades, pursuit, eye-head coordination. J. Vestib. Res. 3, 331–343.
    1. Beaton K. H., Huffman W. C., Schubert M. C. (2015). Binocular misalignments elicited by altered gravity provide evidence for nonlinear central compensation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:81. 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00081
    1. Blokland A., Sambeth A., Prickaerts J., Riedel W. J. (2016). Why an m1 antagonist could be a more selective model for memory impairment than scopolamine. Front. Neurol. 7:167. 10.3389/fneur.2016.00167
    1. Boon P. J., Zeni S., Theeuwes J., Belopolsky A. V. (2018). Rapid updating of spatial working memory across saccades. Sci. Rep. 8:1072. 10.1038/s41598-017-18779-9
    1. Borel L., Lopez C., Péruch P., Lacour M. (2008). Vestibular syndrome: a change in internal spatial representation. Neurophysiol. Clin. 38, 375–389. 10.1016/j.neucli.2008.09.002
    1. Brümmer V., Schneider S., Vogt T., Strüder H., Carnahan H., Askew C. D., et al. . (2011). Coherence between brain cortical function and neurocognitive performance during changed gravity conditions. J. Vis. Exp. 51:2670. 10.3791/2670
    1. Cheron G., Leroy A., Palmero-Soler E., De Saedeleer C., Bengoetxea A., Cebolla A. M., et al. . (2014). Gravity influences top-down signals in visual processing. PLoS One 9:e82371. 10.1371/journal.pone.0082371
    1. Clément G., André-Deshays C., Lathan C. E. (1989). Effects of gravitoinertial force variations on vertical gaze direction during oculomotor reflexes and visual fixation. Aviat Space Environ Med. 60, 1194–1198.
    1. Clément G., Demel M. (2012). Perceptual reversal of bi-stable figures in microgravity and hypergravity during parabolic flight. Neurosci. Lett. 507, 143–146. 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.12.006
    1. Clément G., Loureiro N., Sousa D., Zandvliet A. (2016). Perception of egocentric distance during gravitational changes in parabolic flight. PLoS One 11:e0159422. 10.1371/journal.pone.0159422
    1. Clément G., Reschke M. F., Verrett C. M., Wood S. J. (1992a). Effects of gravitoinertial force variations on optokinetic nystagmus and on perception of visual stimulus orientation. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 63,771–777.
    1. Clément G., Wood S. J., Reschke M. F. (1992b). Effects of microgravity on the interaction of vestibular and optokinetic nystagmus in the vertical plane. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 63, 778–784.
    1. Cullen K. E., Taube J. S. (2017). Our sense of direction: progress, controversies and challenges. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1465–1473. 10.1038/nn.4658
    1. Della-Justina H. M., Gamba H. R., Lukasova K., Nucci-da-Silva M. P., Winkler A. M., Amaro E. (2015). Interaction of brain areas of visual and vestibular simultaneous activity with fmri. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 237–252. 10.1007/s00221-014-4107-6
    1. Fisher N. I. (1993). Statistical Analysis of Circular Data. Cambridge; New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
    1. Frissen I., Campos J. L., Souman J. L., Ernst M. O. (2011). Integration of vestibular and proprioceptive signals for spatial updating. Exp. Brain Res. 212, 163–176. 10.1007/s00221-011-2717-9
    1. Glasauer S., Mittelstaedt H. (1998). Perception of spatial orientation in microgravity. Brain Res. Rev. 28, 185–193. 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00038-1
    1. Grabherr L., Mast F. W. (2010). Effects of microgravity on cognition: the case of mental imagery. J. Vestib. Res. 20, 53–60. 10.3233/ves-2010-0364
    1. Grabherr L., Karmali F., Bach S., Indermaur K., Metzler S., Mast F. W. (2007). Mental own-body and body-part transformations in microgravity. J. Vestib. Res. 17, 279–287.
    1. Gramann K., Müller H. J., Schönebeck B., Debus G. (2006). The neural basis of ego-and allocentric reference frames in spatial navigation: evidence from spatio-temporal coupled current density reconstruction. Brain Res. 1118, 116–129. 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.005
    1. Hitier M., Besnard S., Smith P. F. (2014). Vestibular pathways involved in cognition. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:59. 10.3389/fnint.2014.00059
    1. Karmali F., Shelhamer M. (2008). The dynamics of parabolic flight: flight characteristics and passenger percepts. Acta Astronaut. 63, 594–602. 10.1016/j.actaastro.2008.04.009
    1. Kirby K. N., Gerlanc D. (2013). BootES: an R package for bootstrap confidence intervals on effect sizes. Behav. Res. Methods 45, 905–927. 10.3758/s13428-013-0330-5
    1. Klein T., Wollseiffen P., Sanders M., Claassen J., Carnahan H., Abeln V., et al. . (2019). The influence of microgravity on cerebral blood flow and electrocortical activity. Exp. Brain Res. 237, 1057–1062. 10.1007/s00221-019-05490-6
    1. Lackner J. R., DiZio P. (2000). Human orientation and movement control in weightless and artificial gravity environments. Exp. Brain Res. 130, 2–26. 10.1007/s002210050002
    1. Leichnetz G. R. (2001). Connections of the medial posterior parietal cortex (area 7m) in the monkey. Anat. Rec. 263, 215–236. 10.1002/ar.1082
    1. Markham C. H., Diamond S. G., Stoller D. F. (2000). Parabolic flight reveals independent binocular control of otolith-induced eye torsion. Arch. Ital. Biol. 138, 73–86.
    1. McIntyre J., Zago M., Berthoz A., Lacquaniti F. (2001). Does the brain model newton’s laws? Nat. Neurosci. 4, 693–694. 10.1038/89477
    1. Müller N. G., Riemer M., Brandt L., Wolbers T. (2018). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals a causal role of the human precuneus in spatial updating. Sci. Rep. 8:10171. 10.1038/s41598-018-28487-7
    1. Nicogossian A. E., Williams R. S., Huntoon C. L., Doarn C., Polk J. D., Schneider V. S. (2016). Space Physiology and Medicine: From Evidence to Practice. New York, NY: Springer.
    1. Pfeiffer C., Serino A., Blanke O. (2014). The vestibular system: a spatial reference for bodily self-consciousness. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:31. 10.3389/fnint.2014.00031
    1. R Core Team (2016). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Available online at: . Accessed March 27, 2020
    1. Reschke M. F., Clément G. (2018). Vestibular and sensorimotor dysfunction during space flight. Curr. Pathobiol. Rep. 6, 177–183. 10.1007/s40139-018-0173-y
    1. Riemer M., Hölzl R., Kleinböhl D. (2014). Interrelations between the perception of time and space in large-scale environments. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 1317–1325. 10.1007/s00221-014-3848-6
    1. Roy A. (2006). Estimating correlation coefficient between two variables with repeated observations using mixed effects model. Biom. J. 48, 286–301. 10.1002/bimj.200510192
    1. Schneider S., Brümmer V., Carnahan H., Dubrowski A., Askew C. D., Strüder H. K. (2008). What happens to the brain in weightlessness? A first approach by eeg tomography. NeuroImage 42, 1316–1323. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.010
    1. Svoboda J., Popelikova A., Stuchlik A. (2017). Drugs interfering with muscarinic acetylcholine receptors and their effects on place navigation. Front. Psychiatry 8:215. 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00215
    1. Tagliabue M., McIntyre J. (2011). Necessity is the mother of invention: reconstructing missing sensory information in multiple, concurrent reference frames for eye-hand coordination. J. Neurosci. 31, 1397–1409. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0623-10.2011
    1. Tagliabue M., McIntyre J. (2013). When kinesthesia becomes visual: a theoretical justification for executing motor tasks in visual space. PLoS One 8:e68438. 10.1371/journal.pone.0068438
    1. Tagliabue M., McIntyre J. (2014). A modular theory of multisensory integration for motor control. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 8:1. 10.3389/fncom.2014.00001
    1. Theeuwes J., Belopolsky A., Olivers C. N. (2009). Interactions between working memory, attention and eye movements. Acta Psychol. Amst. 132, 106–114. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.01.005
    1. van de Pol M., Wright J. (2009). A simple method for distinguishing within-versus between-subject effects using mixed models. Anim. Behav. 77, 753–758. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.11.006
    1. Van Ombergen A., Wuyts F. L., Jeurissen B., Sijbers J., Vanhevel F., Jillings S., et al. . (2017). Intrinsic functional connectivity reduces after first-time exposure to short-term gravitational alterations induced by parabolic flight. Sci. Rep. 7:3061. 10.1038/s41598-017-03170-5
    1. Wolbers T., Hegarty M., Büchel C., Loomis J. M. (2008). Spatial updating: how the brain keeps track of changing object locations during observer motion. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 1223–1230. 10.1038/nn.2189

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe