Hippocampal and prefrontal processing of network topology to simulate the future

Amir-Homayoun Javadi, Beatrix Emo, Lorelei R Howard, Fiona E Zisch, Yichao Yu, Rebecca Knight, Joao Pinelo Silva, Hugo J Spiers, Amir-Homayoun Javadi, Beatrix Emo, Lorelei R Howard, Fiona E Zisch, Yichao Yu, Rebecca Knight, Joao Pinelo Silva, Hugo J Spiers

Abstract

Topological networks lie at the heart of our cities and social milieu. However, it remains unclear how and when the brain processes topological structures to guide future behaviour during everyday life. Using fMRI in humans and a simulation of London (UK), here we show that, specifically when new streets are entered during navigation of the city, right posterior hippocampal activity indexes the change in the number of local topological connections available for future travel and right anterior hippocampal activity reflects global properties of the street entered. When forced detours require re-planning of the route to the goal, bilateral inferior lateral prefrontal activity scales with the planning demands of a breadth-first search of future paths. These results help shape models of how hippocampal and prefrontal regions support navigation, planning and future simulation.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Figures

Figure 1. Illustration of the three centrality…
Figure 1. Illustration of the three centrality measures in a sample network.
The network was chosen to illustrate how the three measures of centrality record different properties of the network. Note each measure identifies different streets as having the highest value. (a) The highest degree centrality street reflects the fact that this street has six streets connected to it. (b) The highest closeness centrality streets reflect the fact that these streets are topologically closest to all other streets in the network. (c) The highest betweenness centrality street indicates that this street would be travelled most frequently when travelling from any one street to another.
Figure 2. Graph-theoretic analysis of London (UK)…
Figure 2. Graph-theoretic analysis of London (UK) street network centrality and the fMRI navigation task.
(a) Plots of central London (UK) street segment centrality measures (degree, closeness and betweenness). We used a segment-based approach known as space syntax. Here degree centrality measures the number of connecting segments to any segment, closeness measures how far any two segments are and betweenness measures the number of shortest paths from all segments to all other segments that pass through that segment. See Supplementary Table 1 for the relationship between measures in Soho. White bounded region in each plot indicates the region of Soho learned and navigated during fMRI scanning. See Supplementary Fig. 1 for the frequency of each value of centrality for Central London and this region of Soho. (b) Plots of segment centrality measures for the streets navigated in Soho. Thicker lines display an example of one of the 10 routes navigated during fMRI. (c) Top: degree centrality of the street segments in the example route plotted with each of the six Street Entry Events marked. Bottom: movie frames from our fMRI navigation task at the six Street Entry Events in the example route above.
Figure 3. Posterior hippocampal activity is correlated…
Figure 3. Posterior hippocampal activity is correlated with the change in degree centrality during navigation.
(a) Top left: degree centrality plotted for each street segment for an example route (see Fig. 2c). Right: axonometric projection of the buildings in Soho plotted on a map of Soho. Degree centrality of the route is plotted on the map and projected above. Above the route the graph plots the change in degree centrality for each boundary transition and the top graph plots the evoked response in the right posterior hippocampus at each of the individual boundary transitions (1–6). Analysis of this plot was not used for statistical inference (which was carried out within the statistical parametric mapping framework), but is shown to illustrate the analytic approach. (b,c) Right posterior hippocampal activity correlated significantly with the change in degree centrality for Navigation and Navigation>Control during Street Entry Events. Statistical parametric maps are displayed with threshold P<0.005 uncorrected on the mean structural image. (d) Parameter estimates for the mean activity in the right posterior hippocampus ROI for Navigation (t23=4.24, P=0.0003), Control (t23=1.17, P=0.25) and Navigation>Control (t23=4.64, P=0.0001) comparisons for a model containing categorical change in degree centrality (see Supplementary Table 2). (e) Parameter estimates for the mean activity in the right posterior hippocampus ROI for Navigation>Control condition for a model containing degree centrality (t23=2.28, P=0.03), betweenness centrality (t23=0.53, P=0.59) and closeness centrality (t23=0.14, P=0.88) measures (Supplementary Table 3 and Supplementary Fig. 3). Error bars denote the s.e.m. See Supplementary Fig. 4C for anterior hippocampal ROI mean responses.
Figure 4. Posterior hippocampal activity correlated with…
Figure 4. Posterior hippocampal activity correlated with the change in degree centrality specifically at Street Entry Events.
Top: perspective view of Soho showing part of the example route (Fig. 2a) shown to illustrate the three examples of the different time points examined. During navigation routes, right posterior hippocampal activity was significantly more correlated with the change in degree centrality at Street Entry Events than at Decision Points (t23=2.34, P=0.02) or at Travel Period Events (t23=4.01, P=0.001), *Significance at a threshold of P<0.05 corrected for ROI. Error bars denote the s.e.m.
Figure 5. Inferior lateral prefrontal activity correlates…
Figure 5. Inferior lateral prefrontal activity correlates with the demands of a breadth-first search at Detours.
(a) Diagrams of an example street network contrasting scenarios of lower and higher demand breadth-first search. Breadth-first search assumes the search space (street segments) as a tree and considers all possible solutions within one level before proceeding to the subsequent level. In these diagrams, covering the first layer of the search, the lower demand scenario shows less possible paths, while the higher demand scenario shows a greater number of possible paths. For details see Methods. (b) The statistical parametric map showing correlation (P<0.05 FWE-corrected) of the left and right lateral PFC with planning demands for the first layer of the decision tree (Navigation>Control). We found bilateral lateral PFC activity correlated with planning demands (P<0.001 uncorrected) during Detours in navigation routes, but not in control routes. We found no significant correlations when the planning demands of first and second layer combined were entered in the analysis. The statistical parametric maps are displayed on the mean structural image at a threshold of P<0.005 uncorrected and five voxels minimum cluster size. See Supplementary Table 11 for details of activations. Comparison of parameter estimates of peak voxel in the right lateral PFC showed a significantly greater response at Detours compared with Decision Points (t23=3.49, P=0.002).

References

    1. Hassabis D. & Maguire E. A. The construction system of the brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1263–1271 (2009).
    1. Erdem U. M. & Hasselmo M. A goal-directed spatial navigation model using forward trajectory planning based on grid cells. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 916–931 (2012).
    1. Byrne P., Becker S. & Burgess N. Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol. Rev. 114, 340 (2007).
    1. Ólafsdóttir H. F., Barry C., Saleem A. B., Hassabis D. & Spiers H. J. Hippocampal place cells construct reward related sequences through unexplored space. Elife 4, e06063 (2015).
    1. Schacter D. L. et al.. The future of memory: remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron 76, 677–694 (2012).
    1. Spiers H. J., Hayman R. M. A., Jovalekic A., Marozzi E. & Jeffery K. J. Place field repetition and purely local remapping in a multicompartment environment. Cereb. Cortex 25, 10–25 (2015).
    1. Dabaghian Y., Brandt V. L. & Frank L. M. Reconceiving the hippocampal map as a topological template. Elife 3, e03476 (2014).
    1. Wu X. & Foster D. J. Hippocampal replay captures the unique topological structure of a novel environment. J. Neurosci. 34, 6459–6469 (2014).
    1. Shallice T. & Burgess P. W. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain 114, 727–741 (1991).
    1. Spiers H. J. & Gilbert S. J. Solving the detour problem in navigation: a model of prefrontal and hippocampal interactions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 125 (2015).
    1. Huys Q. J. et al.. Bonsai trees in your head: how the Pavlovian system sculpts goal-directed choices by pruning decision trees. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8, e1002410 (2012).
    1. Huys Q. J. et al.. Interplay of approximate planning strategies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 3098–3103 (2015).
    1. Russell S. & Norvig P. AI a modern approach. Learning 2, 4 (2005).
    1. Elliott R. & Lesk M. in Proceedings of Association for the Advancement of Artificial Intelligence Conference on Artificial Intelligence 258–261AAAI-82 (1982).
    1. Hillier B. & Hanson J. The Social Logic of Space Cambridge University Press (1989).
    1. Hillier B. Space is the Machine: a Configurational Theory of Architecture Cambridge University Press (1996).
    1. Sabidussi G. The centrality index of a graph. Psychometrika 31, 581–603 (1966).
    1. Howard L. R. et al.. The hippocampus and entorhinal cortex encode the path and Euclidean distances to goals during navigation. Curr. Biol. 24, 1331–1340 (2014).
    1. Emo B. Seeing the axial line: evidence from wayfinding experiments. Behav. Sci. 4, 167–180 (2014).
    1. Spiers H. J. & Barry C. Neural systems supporting navigation. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 1, 47–55 (2015).
    1. Johnson A. & Redish A. D. Neural ensembles in CA3 transiently encode paths forward of the animal at a decision point. J. Neurosci. 27, 12176–12189 (2007).
    1. Pfeiffer B. E. & Foster D. J. Hippocampal place-cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals. Nature 497, 74–79 (2013).
    1. Johnson A., van der Meer M. A. & Redish A. D. Integrating hippocampus and striatum in decision-making. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 692–697 (2007).
    1. Maguire E. A., Woollett K. & Spiers H. J. London taxi drivers and bus drivers: a structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus 16, 1091–1101 (2006).
    1. Bohbot V. D. et al.. Spatial memory deficits in patients with lesions to the right hippocampus and to the right parahippocampal cortex. Neuropsychologia 36, 1217–1238 (1998).
    1. Doeller C. F., King J. A. & Burgess N. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 5915–5920 (2008).
    1. Baumann O. & Mattingley J. B. Dissociable representations of environmental size and complexity in the human hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 33, 10526–10533 (2013).
    1. Spiers H. J. et al.. Unilateral temporal lobectomy patients show lateralized topographical and episodic memory deficits in a virtual town. Brain 124, 2476–2489 (2001).
    1. Tavares R. M. et al.. A map for social navigation in the human brain. Neuron 87, 231–243 (2015).
    1. Poppenk J., Evensmoen H. R., Moscovitch M. & Nadel L. Long-axis specialization of the human hippocampus. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 230–240 (2013).
    1. Nadel L., Hoscheidt S. & Ryan L. R. Spatial cognition and the hippocampus: the anterior–posterior axis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 22–28 (2013).
    1. Slone E., Burles F. & Iaria G. Environmental layout complexity affects neural activity during navigation in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 43, 1146–1155 (2016).
    1. Schapiro A. C., Turk-Browne N. B., Norman K. A. & Botvinick M. M. Statistical learning of temporal community structure in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 26, 3–8 (2016).
    1. Dalton R. C. The secret is to follow your nose route path selection and angularity. Environ. Behav. 35, 107–131 (2003).
    1. Strange B. A., Duggins A., Penny W., Dolan R. J. & Friston K. J. Information theory, novelty and hippocampal responses: unpredicted or unpredictable? Neural Netw. 18, 225–230 (2005).
    1. Harrison L. M., Duggins A. & Friston K. J. Encoding uncertainty in the hippocampus. Neural Netw. 19, 535–546 (2006).
    1. McNamee D., Wolpert D. & Lengyel M. in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 29 (NIPS, Barcelona, Spain, 2016).
    1. Balaguer J., Spiers H., Hassabis D. & Summerfield C. Neural mechanisms of hierarchical planning in a virtual subway network. Neuron 90, 893–903 (2016).
    1. Poucet B. et al.. Is there a pilot in the brain? Contribution of the self-positioning system to spatial navigation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 292 (2015).
    1. Spiers H. J. & Maguire E. A. Thoughts, behaviour, and brain dynamics during navigation in the real world. NeuroImage 31, 1826–1840 (2006).
    1. Hegarty M., Montello D. R., Richardson A. E., Ishikawa T. & Lovelace K. Spatial abilities at different scales: individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence 34, 151–176 (2006).
    1. Neal Z. P. The Connected City: How Networks are Shaping the Modern Metropolis Routledge (2012).
    1. Freeman L. C. A set of measures of centrality based on betweenness. Sociometry 40, 35–41 (1977).
    1. Hillier B., Penn A., Hanson J., Grajewski T. & Xu J. Natural movement: or, configuration and attraction in urban pedestrian movement. Environ. Plann. B Plann. Des. 20, 29–66 (1993).
    1. Hillier B. & Iida S. in Spatial Information Theory 475–490Springer (2005).
    1. Penn A. Space syntax and spatial cognition or why the axial line? Environ. Behav. 35, 30–65 (2003).
    1. Emo B. Real-world wayfinding experiments: Individual preferences, decisions and the space syntax approach at street corners Doctoral thesis, UCL (University College London (2014).
    1. Emo B. The Visual Properties of Spatial Configuration. Masters thesis, UCL (University College London, 2010).
    1. Peponis J., Zimring C. & Choi Y. K. Finding the building in wayfinding. Environ. Behav. 22, 555–590 (1990).
    1. Golledge R. G. in Proceedings of the International Conference COSIT '95, Semmering, Austria, 21 -23 September 1995 Vol. 988 (eds Frank, A. & Kuhn, W.) 207–222 (Berlin, Heidelberg, Germany, Springer, (1995).
    1. Zacharias J. Path choice and visual stimuli: signs of human activity and architecture. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 341–352 (2001).
    1. Dalton R. C., Troffa R., Zacharias J. & Hölscher C. in Urban Diversities–Environmental and Social Issues, Advances in People-Environment Studies Vol. 2 (eds Bonaiuto, M. et al..) 67-76 (Göttingen, Germany, Hogrefe, 2011).
    1. Pinelo J. Towards a Spatial Congruence Theory: How Spatial Cognition can Inform Urban Planning and Design. Doctoral thesis, UCL (University College London, 2010).
    1. Josephs O., Turner R. & Friston K. Event-related fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 243–248 (1997).
    1. Mumford J. A., Poline J.-B. & Poldrack R. A. Orthogonalization of regressors in fMRI models. PLoS ONE 10, e0126255 (2015).
    1. Spiers H. J. & Maguire E. A. A navigational guidance system in the human brain. Hippocampus 17, 618–626 (2007).
    1. Maguire E. A. et al.. Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. Science 280, 921–924 (1998).
    1. Maguire E. A., Frackowiak R. S. & Frith C. D. Recalling routes around London: activation of the right hippocampus in taxi drivers. J. Neurosci. 17, 7103–7110 (1997).
    1. Howard L. R., Kumaran D., Ólafsdóttir H. F. & Spiers H. J. Double dissociation between hippocampal and parahippocampal responses to object-background context and scene novelty. J. Neurosci. 31, 5253–5261 (2011).
    1. Brett M., Anton J.-L., Valabregue R. & Poline J.-B. Region of interest analysis using the MarsBar toolbox for SPM 99. Neuroimage 16, S497 (2002).
    1. Maldjian J. A., Laurienti P. J., Kraft R. A. & Burdette J. H. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19, 1233–1239 (2003).

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe