Induced sensorimotor cortex plasticity remediates chronic treatment-resistant visual neglect

Jacinta O'Shea, Patrice Revol, Helena Cousijn, Jamie Near, Pierre Petitet, Sophie Jacquin-Courtois, Heidi Johansen-Berg, Gilles Rode, Yves Rossetti, Jacinta O'Shea, Patrice Revol, Helena Cousijn, Jamie Near, Pierre Petitet, Sophie Jacquin-Courtois, Heidi Johansen-Berg, Gilles Rode, Yves Rossetti

Abstract

Right brain injury causes visual neglect - lost awareness of left space. During prism adaptation therapy, patients adapt to a rightward optical shift by recalibrating right arm movements leftward. This can improve left neglect, but the benefit of a single session is transient (~1 day). Here we show that tonic disinhibition of left motor cortex during prism adaptation enhances consolidation, stabilizing both sensorimotor and cognitive prism after-effects. In three longitudinal patient case series, just 20 min of combined stimulation/adaptation caused persistent cognitive after-effects (neglect improvement) that lasted throughout follow-up (18-46 days). Moreover, adaptation without stimulation was ineffective. Thus stimulation reversed treatment resistance in chronic visual neglect. These findings challenge consensus that because the left hemisphere in neglect is pathologically over-excited it ought to be suppressed. Excitation of left sensorimotor circuits, during an adaptive cognitive state, can unmask latent plastic potential that durably improves resistant visual attention deficits after brain injury.

Keywords: human; motor memory consolidation; neglect rehabilitation; neuroscience; prism adaptation.

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewing editor, eLife.

No competing interests declared.

Figures

Figure 1.. Visuospatial neglect and prism adaptation.
Figure 1.. Visuospatial neglect and prism adaptation.
(A–C) Examples of neglect behavior by patients in the present study. (A) Gardening task: arrange the flowers uniformly around the garden; (B) patient has neglected the left of his body; (C) Ota-Girardi task: cancel all targets on the page. (D) Prism Adaptation. (1) Baseline: visual feedback. Participants make rapid centre-out pointing movements to targets located at 10 degrees left or right. Vision of the hand start position is occluded. (2) Baseline: no visual feedback. Participants point at a central target. Vision of the hand is occluded throughout. (3) Adaptation: early prism exposure. Participants repeat (1) wearing 10° right-shifting prisms and initially make large rightward pointing errors. They use error feedback to correct their movements from right to left. (4) Adaptation: late prism exposure. After sustained prism exposure participants successfully realign hand-eye coordination leftward to regain baseline accuracy (i.e. they adapt). (5) Adaptation: prism after-effect. As participants adapt, this induces a leftward bias (prism after-effect), measured as the change from baseline (2). (6) Washout. After prism removal when participants point as in (1), the effect of adaptation is seen as a leftward error that is quickly corrected to restore baseline accuracy. (7) Retention. The magnitude of any remaining prism after-effect is measured as in (2).
Figure 2.. M1 a-tDCS during Prism Adaptation…
Figure 2.. M1 a-tDCS during Prism Adaptation (PA) enhanced consolidation of the prism after-effect.
The x-axis represents baseline accuracy (zero error), with prism after-effect (AE) data representing changes from baseline. For each panel N = 9. Asterisk indicates significant difference between Anodal and Sham (pA) Pointing accuracy in healthy volunteers when anodal (red) or sham (blue) tDCS was applied to M1 during Adaptation (Experiment 1). Black wedges indicate blocks throughout which prisms were worn. During Adaptation (prism exposure, (E1–E6) and Washout (prisms removed, (W1–W6), participants saw the outcome of the trial, so could correct their errors. The AE was measured without visual feedback (AE1-15, shaded light grey). Solid lines show pointing accuracy averaged across participants (shading =±1 SEM). Adaptation and Washout lasted 20 min each. Retention lasted 6 min after 10 min of blindfolded rest. Relative to sham, anodal tDCS increased AE persistence throughout Washout and Retention: note the leftward shift in AE7-15 (no visual feedback), whereas accuracy in interleaved blocks W1-6 is indistinguishable. (B–E) Prism after-effect in different stimulation conditions. Panel (B) summarizes group mean AEs (±1 SEM) for the dataset shown in (A) (AE1-15 only) and also shows data for reversed polarity stimulation (cathodal, green). Other panels plot the same summary AE data for stimulation of: (C) M1 before Adaptation (Experiment 2); (D) right posterior parietal cortex (PPC) during Adaptation (Experiment 3); E) right cerebellum (CB) during Adaptation (Experiment 4).
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.. Control experiment 5:…
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.. Control experiment 5: M1 a-tDCS alone does not cause a leftward shift in pointing behavior.
Data are group mean open-loop pointing errors (±1 SEM; N = 10) for sham and anodal tDCS conditions in the identical behavioural protocol to that shown in Figure 2 (ie: Adaptation, Washout, Retention) except that sham (not prism) glasses were used (Experiment 5). There is no leftward shift in pointing behavior in either condition. This confirms that M1 a-tDCS alone does not induce the sustained leftward shift in pointing behavior shown in Figure 2A,B and Figure 3.
Figure 3.. M1 a-tDCS during Adaptation enhanced…
Figure 3.. M1 a-tDCS during Adaptation enhanced prism after-effect retention across days.
Pointing errors in healthy volunteers (N = 10) when anodal (red) or sham (blue) stimulation was applied during Adaptation (Experiment 6). Compared to the experiment of Figure 2A, there was no active washout phase. Instead, retention was measured daily over 5 days after adaptation. Solid lines show pointing accuracy averaged across participants (±1 SEM, shading). As in the experiment shown in Figure 2A, the AE persisted throughout the measured retention period only when M1 a-tDCS had been applied during Adaptation. Asterisks indicate significantly larger AEs in the anodal versus sham condition throughout Retention (p

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation-enhanced retention…

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation-enhanced retention persists across days.

Figure shows group mean open-loop…

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation-enhanced retention persists across days.
Figure shows group mean open-loop pointing errors (±1 SEM) over 4 days prior to prism adaptation (baseline), and 4 days after PA +sham tDCS (blue) or anodal tDCS (red). Pointing is significantly left-shifted in the 4 days after M1 anodal tDCS compared to both the sham and baseline conditions. Asterisk reflects p

Figure 4.. Inter-individual differences in neurochemical response…

Figure 4.. Inter-individual differences in neurochemical response to stimulation (M1 GABA change) co-varied with changes…

Figure 4.. Inter-individual differences in neurochemical response to stimulation (M1 GABA change) co-varied with changes in prism after-effect retention caused by M1 a-tDCS.
(A) Experiment 7: X-axis shows individuals' percent change in GABA concentration after M1 a-tDCS (post-pre), quantified as ratios of total creatine. Y-axis shows individuals' percent change in AE retention 24 hr after PA + tDCS (anodal-sham). Correlation (N = 10) shows quantitative covariation between the behavioural and neurochemical effects of M1 a-tDCS. Note the data pass through the origin. (B) Representative image for a single participant showing voxel placement in the hand knob region of M1 and spectral quality. Frequency spectrum labels (ppm = parts per million) indicate peaks for GABA, Glutamate (Glu), total Creatine (TCr = creatine + phosphocreatine) and N-acetylaspartate (NAA).

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. AE retention scores…

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. AE retention scores (%) 24 hr after PA was combined with…

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. AE retention scores (%) 24 hr after PA was combined with anodal or sham tDCS in Experiment 7.
Bars represent individuals and group mean ±1 SEM (N = 16). Apparently missing bars represent zero retention. Participants 1–10 also had usable MRS data and are shown in the behavior-neurochemistry correlation in Figure 4. Figure 4 plots the anodal-sham difference in retention ratios.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.. MRS data from…

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.. MRS data from occipital cortex voxel in Experiment 7.

Representative image…

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.. MRS data from occipital cortex voxel in Experiment 7.
Representative image for a single participant showing voxel placement in the occipital cortex (control region). Frequency spectrum labels (ppm = parts per million) indicate peaks for GABA, Glutamate (Glu), total Creatine (TCr = creatine + phosphocreatine) and N-acetylaspartate (NAA).

Figure 5.. Stimulation-enhanced retention in chronic neglect.

Figure 5.. Stimulation-enhanced retention in chronic neglect.

The prism after-effect over time for anodal (red)…

Figure 5.. Stimulation-enhanced retention in chronic neglect.
The prism after-effect over time for anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation of M1 during Adaptation in a single case neglect patient (Patient 1). Data are displayed as described for Figure 2B except that sham data are the mean (±95% confidence intervals) across 3 sessions of the protocol, and anodal data are from a single session. AE retention was significantly enhanced by stimulation in this neglect patient, similar to the healthy volunteers shown in Figure 2A,B. Asterisk indicates significantly greater retention for the planned contrast of anodal versus sham in Retention blocks 1 and 3 (*p

Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.

( A–C…

Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.

( A–C ) T1-weighted structural scans of Patients 1–3, respectively.…

Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.
(A–C) T1-weighted structural scans of Patients 1–3, respectively. (D) Grey matter lesion overlap between the three patients in group mean MNI space. (E) ‘Disconnectome map’ shows white matter damage overlap between the three patients. Red indicates white matter damage overlap in all three patients in the right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (rIFOF), the right superior longitudinal fasciculus (rSLF) and the right anterior thalamic radiation (rATR).

Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads…

Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.

( A )…

Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.
(A) Behavioural measures of neglect in a single case chronic neglect patient (Patient 1) before and after PA sessions combining M1 anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation during adaptation. Datapoints are mean % neglect score ±1 SEM. A score of ~50% indicates the patient completed the rightmost half of the six tests in the battery. (B) As (A), but with a different battery of 10 neglect test measures, reversal of the order of anodal and sham stimulation, and no Washout phase during prism adaptation. (C, D) Percentage changes in performance on a battery of neglect tests over time after PA +M1 a-tDCS, averaged across all three patients (±1 SEM) (C), and for each individual case (D). Asterisks indicate significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS compared to sham (p<0.05).

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.

(…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.
(A) Behavioural measures of neglect in Patient 2 before and after PA sessions combining M1 anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation during adaptation. Data are mean % neglect score ±1 SEM. A baseline score of ~60% indicates the patient completed the rightmost two thirds of the 10 tests in the battery. One of the sham baseline scores is missing for Patient 2 as he forgot to attend testing. Asterisk indicates significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS compared to sham (p<0.05). (B) As (A), but for Patient 3, who underwent the reversed order of PA +anodal versus sham stimulation. Bracketed asterisk indicates significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS, but the contrast of anodal versus sham was not significant.
All figures (12)
Similar articles
Cited by
References
    1. Adamson J, Beswick A, Ebrahim S. Is stroke the most common cause of disability? Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases. 2004;13:171–177. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2004.06.003. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Barrett AM, Goedert KM, Basso JC. Prism adaptation for spatial neglect after stroke: translational practice gaps. Nature Reviews Neurology. 2012;8:567–577. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.170. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Block H, Bastian A, Celnik P. Virtual lesion of angular gyrus disrupts the relationship between visuoproprioceptive weighting and realignment. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2013;25:636–648. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00340. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bowen A, Hazelton C, Pollock A, Lincoln NB. Cognitive rehabilitation for spatial neglect following stroke. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews. 2013;7:CD003586. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003586.pub3. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Buch ER, Santarnecchi E, Antal A, Born J, Celnik PA, Classen J, Gerloff C, Hallett M, Hummel FC, Nitsche MA, Pascual-Leone A, Paulus WJ, Reis J, Robertson EM, Rothwell JC, Sandrini M, Schambra HM, Wassermann EM, Ziemann U, Cohen LG. Effects of tDCS on motor learning and memory formation: A consensus and critical position paper. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2017;128:589–603. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.01.004. - DOI - PubMed
Show all 80 references
Publication types
MeSH terms
Related information
[x]
Cite
Copy Download .nbib
Format: AMA APA MLA NLM

NCBI Literature Resources

MeSH PMC Bookshelf Disclaimer

The PubMed wordmark and PubMed logo are registered trademarks of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (HHS). Unauthorized use of these marks is strictly prohibited.

Follow NCBI
Figure 3—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation-enhanced retention…
Figure 3—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation-enhanced retention persists across days.
Figure shows group mean open-loop pointing errors (±1 SEM) over 4 days prior to prism adaptation (baseline), and 4 days after PA +sham tDCS (blue) or anodal tDCS (red). Pointing is significantly left-shifted in the 4 days after M1 anodal tDCS compared to both the sham and baseline conditions. Asterisk reflects p

Figure 4.. Inter-individual differences in neurochemical response…

Figure 4.. Inter-individual differences in neurochemical response to stimulation (M1 GABA change) co-varied with changes…

Figure 4.. Inter-individual differences in neurochemical response to stimulation (M1 GABA change) co-varied with changes in prism after-effect retention caused by M1 a-tDCS.
(A) Experiment 7: X-axis shows individuals' percent change in GABA concentration after M1 a-tDCS (post-pre), quantified as ratios of total creatine. Y-axis shows individuals' percent change in AE retention 24 hr after PA + tDCS (anodal-sham). Correlation (N = 10) shows quantitative covariation between the behavioural and neurochemical effects of M1 a-tDCS. Note the data pass through the origin. (B) Representative image for a single participant showing voxel placement in the hand knob region of M1 and spectral quality. Frequency spectrum labels (ppm = parts per million) indicate peaks for GABA, Glutamate (Glu), total Creatine (TCr = creatine + phosphocreatine) and N-acetylaspartate (NAA).

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. AE retention scores…

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. AE retention scores (%) 24 hr after PA was combined with…

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. AE retention scores (%) 24 hr after PA was combined with anodal or sham tDCS in Experiment 7.
Bars represent individuals and group mean ±1 SEM (N = 16). Apparently missing bars represent zero retention. Participants 1–10 also had usable MRS data and are shown in the behavior-neurochemistry correlation in Figure 4. Figure 4 plots the anodal-sham difference in retention ratios.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.. MRS data from…

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.. MRS data from occipital cortex voxel in Experiment 7.

Representative image…

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.. MRS data from occipital cortex voxel in Experiment 7.
Representative image for a single participant showing voxel placement in the occipital cortex (control region). Frequency spectrum labels (ppm = parts per million) indicate peaks for GABA, Glutamate (Glu), total Creatine (TCr = creatine + phosphocreatine) and N-acetylaspartate (NAA).

Figure 5.. Stimulation-enhanced retention in chronic neglect.

Figure 5.. Stimulation-enhanced retention in chronic neglect.

The prism after-effect over time for anodal (red)…

Figure 5.. Stimulation-enhanced retention in chronic neglect.
The prism after-effect over time for anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation of M1 during Adaptation in a single case neglect patient (Patient 1). Data are displayed as described for Figure 2B except that sham data are the mean (±95% confidence intervals) across 3 sessions of the protocol, and anodal data are from a single session. AE retention was significantly enhanced by stimulation in this neglect patient, similar to the healthy volunteers shown in Figure 2A,B. Asterisk indicates significantly greater retention for the planned contrast of anodal versus sham in Retention blocks 1 and 3 (*p

Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.

( A–C…

Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.

( A–C ) T1-weighted structural scans of Patients 1–3, respectively.…

Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.
(A–C) T1-weighted structural scans of Patients 1–3, respectively. (D) Grey matter lesion overlap between the three patients in group mean MNI space. (E) ‘Disconnectome map’ shows white matter damage overlap between the three patients. Red indicates white matter damage overlap in all three patients in the right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (rIFOF), the right superior longitudinal fasciculus (rSLF) and the right anterior thalamic radiation (rATR).

Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads…

Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.

( A )…

Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.
(A) Behavioural measures of neglect in a single case chronic neglect patient (Patient 1) before and after PA sessions combining M1 anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation during adaptation. Datapoints are mean % neglect score ±1 SEM. A score of ~50% indicates the patient completed the rightmost half of the six tests in the battery. (B) As (A), but with a different battery of 10 neglect test measures, reversal of the order of anodal and sham stimulation, and no Washout phase during prism adaptation. (C, D) Percentage changes in performance on a battery of neglect tests over time after PA +M1 a-tDCS, averaged across all three patients (±1 SEM) (C), and for each individual case (D). Asterisks indicate significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS compared to sham (p<0.05).

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.

(…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.
(A) Behavioural measures of neglect in Patient 2 before and after PA sessions combining M1 anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation during adaptation. Data are mean % neglect score ±1 SEM. A baseline score of ~60% indicates the patient completed the rightmost two thirds of the 10 tests in the battery. One of the sham baseline scores is missing for Patient 2 as he forgot to attend testing. Asterisk indicates significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS compared to sham (p<0.05). (B) As (A), but for Patient 3, who underwent the reversed order of PA +anodal versus sham stimulation. Bracketed asterisk indicates significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS, but the contrast of anodal versus sham was not significant.
All figures (12)
Similar articles
Cited by
References
    1. Adamson J, Beswick A, Ebrahim S. Is stroke the most common cause of disability? Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases. 2004;13:171–177. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2004.06.003. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Barrett AM, Goedert KM, Basso JC. Prism adaptation for spatial neglect after stroke: translational practice gaps. Nature Reviews Neurology. 2012;8:567–577. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.170. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Block H, Bastian A, Celnik P. Virtual lesion of angular gyrus disrupts the relationship between visuoproprioceptive weighting and realignment. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2013;25:636–648. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00340. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bowen A, Hazelton C, Pollock A, Lincoln NB. Cognitive rehabilitation for spatial neglect following stroke. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews. 2013;7:CD003586. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003586.pub3. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Buch ER, Santarnecchi E, Antal A, Born J, Celnik PA, Classen J, Gerloff C, Hallett M, Hummel FC, Nitsche MA, Pascual-Leone A, Paulus WJ, Reis J, Robertson EM, Rothwell JC, Sandrini M, Schambra HM, Wassermann EM, Ziemann U, Cohen LG. Effects of tDCS on motor learning and memory formation: A consensus and critical position paper. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2017;128:589–603. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.01.004. - DOI - PubMed
Show all 80 references
Publication types
MeSH terms
Related information
[x]
Cite
Copy Download .nbib
Format: AMA APA MLA NLM
Figure 4.. Inter-individual differences in neurochemical response…
Figure 4.. Inter-individual differences in neurochemical response to stimulation (M1 GABA change) co-varied with changes in prism after-effect retention caused by M1 a-tDCS.
(A) Experiment 7: X-axis shows individuals' percent change in GABA concentration after M1 a-tDCS (post-pre), quantified as ratios of total creatine. Y-axis shows individuals' percent change in AE retention 24 hr after PA + tDCS (anodal-sham). Correlation (N = 10) shows quantitative covariation between the behavioural and neurochemical effects of M1 a-tDCS. Note the data pass through the origin. (B) Representative image for a single participant showing voxel placement in the hand knob region of M1 and spectral quality. Frequency spectrum labels (ppm = parts per million) indicate peaks for GABA, Glutamate (Glu), total Creatine (TCr = creatine + phosphocreatine) and N-acetylaspartate (NAA).
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. AE retention scores…
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. AE retention scores (%) 24 hr after PA was combined with anodal or sham tDCS in Experiment 7.
Bars represent individuals and group mean ±1 SEM (N = 16). Apparently missing bars represent zero retention. Participants 1–10 also had usable MRS data and are shown in the behavior-neurochemistry correlation in Figure 4. Figure 4 plots the anodal-sham difference in retention ratios.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2.. MRS data from…
Figure 4—figure supplement 2.. MRS data from occipital cortex voxel in Experiment 7.
Representative image for a single participant showing voxel placement in the occipital cortex (control region). Frequency spectrum labels (ppm = parts per million) indicate peaks for GABA, Glutamate (Glu), total Creatine (TCr = creatine + phosphocreatine) and N-acetylaspartate (NAA).
Figure 5.. Stimulation-enhanced retention in chronic neglect.
Figure 5.. Stimulation-enhanced retention in chronic neglect.
The prism after-effect over time for anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation of M1 during Adaptation in a single case neglect patient (Patient 1). Data are displayed as described for Figure 2B except that sham data are the mean (±95% confidence intervals) across 3 sessions of the protocol, and anodal data are from a single session. AE retention was significantly enhanced by stimulation in this neglect patient, similar to the healthy volunteers shown in Figure 2A,B. Asterisk indicates significantly greater retention for the planned contrast of anodal versus sham in Retention blocks 1 and 3 (*p

Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.

( A–C…

Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.

( A–C ) T1-weighted structural scans of Patients 1–3, respectively.…

Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.
(A–C) T1-weighted structural scans of Patients 1–3, respectively. (D) Grey matter lesion overlap between the three patients in group mean MNI space. (E) ‘Disconnectome map’ shows white matter damage overlap between the three patients. Red indicates white matter damage overlap in all three patients in the right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (rIFOF), the right superior longitudinal fasciculus (rSLF) and the right anterior thalamic radiation (rATR).

Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads…

Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.

( A )…

Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.
(A) Behavioural measures of neglect in a single case chronic neglect patient (Patient 1) before and after PA sessions combining M1 anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation during adaptation. Datapoints are mean % neglect score ±1 SEM. A score of ~50% indicates the patient completed the rightmost half of the six tests in the battery. (B) As (A), but with a different battery of 10 neglect test measures, reversal of the order of anodal and sham stimulation, and no Washout phase during prism adaptation. (C, D) Percentage changes in performance on a battery of neglect tests over time after PA +M1 a-tDCS, averaged across all three patients (±1 SEM) (C), and for each individual case (D). Asterisks indicate significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS compared to sham (p<0.05).

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.

(…

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.
(A) Behavioural measures of neglect in Patient 2 before and after PA sessions combining M1 anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation during adaptation. Data are mean % neglect score ±1 SEM. A baseline score of ~60% indicates the patient completed the rightmost two thirds of the 10 tests in the battery. One of the sham baseline scores is missing for Patient 2 as he forgot to attend testing. Asterisk indicates significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS compared to sham (p<0.05). (B) As (A), but for Patient 3, who underwent the reversed order of PA +anodal versus sham stimulation. Bracketed asterisk indicates significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS, but the contrast of anodal versus sham was not significant.
All figures (12)
Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.
Figure 6.. Patients' Lesion Anatomy.
(A–C) T1-weighted structural scans of Patients 1–3, respectively. (D) Grey matter lesion overlap between the three patients in group mean MNI space. (E) ‘Disconnectome map’ shows white matter damage overlap between the three patients. Red indicates white matter damage overlap in all three patients in the right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (rIFOF), the right superior longitudinal fasciculus (rSLF) and the right anterior thalamic radiation (rATR).
Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads…
Figure 7.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.
(A) Behavioural measures of neglect in a single case chronic neglect patient (Patient 1) before and after PA sessions combining M1 anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation during adaptation. Datapoints are mean % neglect score ±1 SEM. A score of ~50% indicates the patient completed the rightmost half of the six tests in the battery. (B) As (A), but with a different battery of 10 neglect test measures, reversal of the order of anodal and sham stimulation, and no Washout phase during prism adaptation. (C, D) Percentage changes in performance on a battery of neglect tests over time after PA +M1 a-tDCS, averaged across all three patients (±1 SEM) (C), and for each individual case (D). Asterisks indicate significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS compared to sham (p<0.05).
Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during…
Figure 7—figure supplement 1.. M1 stimulation during PA leads to lasting improvements in neglect.
(A) Behavioural measures of neglect in Patient 2 before and after PA sessions combining M1 anodal (red) and sham (blue) stimulation during adaptation. Data are mean % neglect score ±1 SEM. A baseline score of ~60% indicates the patient completed the rightmost two thirds of the 10 tests in the battery. One of the sham baseline scores is missing for Patient 2 as he forgot to attend testing. Asterisk indicates significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS compared to sham (p<0.05). (B) As (A), but for Patient 3, who underwent the reversed order of PA +anodal versus sham stimulation. Bracketed asterisk indicates significant improvement in neglect score after PA +M1 a-tDCS, but the contrast of anodal versus sham was not significant.

References

    1. Adamson J, Beswick A, Ebrahim S. Is stroke the most common cause of disability? Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases. 2004;13:171–177. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2004.06.003.
    1. Barrett AM, Goedert KM, Basso JC. Prism adaptation for spatial neglect after stroke: translational practice gaps. Nature Reviews Neurology. 2012;8:567–577. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.170.
    1. Block H, Bastian A, Celnik P. Virtual lesion of angular gyrus disrupts the relationship between visuoproprioceptive weighting and realignment. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2013;25:636–648. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00340.
    1. Bowen A, Hazelton C, Pollock A, Lincoln NB. Cognitive rehabilitation for spatial neglect following stroke. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews. 2013;7:CD003586. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003586.pub3.
    1. Buch ER, Santarnecchi E, Antal A, Born J, Celnik PA, Classen J, Gerloff C, Hallett M, Hummel FC, Nitsche MA, Pascual-Leone A, Paulus WJ, Reis J, Robertson EM, Rothwell JC, Sandrini M, Schambra HM, Wassermann EM, Ziemann U, Cohen LG. Effects of tDCS on motor learning and memory formation: A consensus and critical position paper. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2017;128:589–603. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.01.004.
    1. Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro MK, Veramonti T, Farne A, Whyte J, Ladavas E, Frassinetti F, Coslett HB. Hemispatial neglect: Subtypes, neuroanatomy, and disability. Neurology. 2004;62:749–756. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000113730.73031.F4.
    1. Corbetta M, Kincade MJ, Lewis C, Snyder AZ, Sapir A. Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1603–1610. doi: 10.1038/nn1574.
    1. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2011;34:569–599. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731.
    1. Edgeworth J, Robertson I, Mac Millan TM. The Balloons Test. London, U.K: Thames Valley Test Company; 1998.
    1. Farnè A, Rossetti Y, Toniolo S, Làdavas E. Ameliorating neglect with prism adaptation: visuo-manual and visuo-verbal measures. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:718–729. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00186-5.
    1. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods. 2007;39:175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146.
    1. Fritsch B, Reis J, Martinowich K, Schambra HM, Ji Y, Cohen LG, Lu B. Direct current stimulation promotes BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications for motor learning. Neuron. 2010;66:198–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.035.
    1. Galea JM, Jayaram G, Ajagbe L, Celnik P. Modulation of cerebellar excitability by polarity-specific noninvasive direct current stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:9115–9122. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2184-09.2009.
    1. Galea JM, Vazquez A, Pasricha N, de Xivry JJ, Celnik P. Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum and motor cortex during adaptive learning: the motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21:1761–1770. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq246.
    1. Gandiga PC, Hummel FC, Cohen LG. Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2006;117:845–850. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003.
    1. Gauthier L, Dehaunt F, Joanette Y. The Bells Test: a quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. International Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology. 1989;11:49–54.
    1. Hadipour-Niktarash A, Lee CK, Desmond JE, Shadmehr R. Impairment of retention but not acquisition of a visuomotor skill through time-dependent disruption of primary motor cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:13413–13419. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2570-07.2007.
    1. Hanajima R, Shadmehr R, Ohminami S, Tsutsumi R, Shirota Y, Shimizu T, Tanaka N, Terao Y, Tsuji S, Ugawa Y, Uchimura M, Inoue M, Kitazawa S. Modulation of error-sensitivity during a prism adaptation task in people with cerebellar degeneration. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2015;114:2460–2471. doi: 10.1152/jn.00145.2015.
    1. Hummel F, Celnik P, Giraux P, Floel A, Wu WH, Gerloff C, Cohen LG. Effects of non-invasive cortical stimulation on skilled motor function in chronic stroke. Brain. 2005;128:490–499. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh369.
    1. Hunter T, Sacco P, Nitsche MA, Turner DL. Modulation of internal model formation during force field-induced motor learning by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of primary motor cortex. The Journal of Physiology. 2009;587:2949–2961. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.169284.
    1. Inoue M, Uchimura M, Kitazawa S. Error signals in motor cortices drive adaptation in reaching. Neuron. 2016;90:1114–1126. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.029.
    1. Jacquin-Courtois S, O'Shea J, Luauté J, Pisella L, Revol P, Mizuno K, Rode G, Rossetti Y. Rehabilitation of spatial neglect by prism adaptation: a peculiar expansion of sensorimotor after-effects to spatial cognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37:594–609. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.007.
    1. Jamil A, Batsikadze G, Kuo HI, Labruna L, Hasan A, Paulus W, Nitsche MA. Systematic evaluation of the impact of stimulation intensity on neuroplastic after-effects induced by transcranial direct current stimulation. The Journal of Physiology. 2017;595:1273–1288. doi: 10.1113/JP272738.
    1. Jehkonen M, Laihosalo M, Kettunen JE. Impact of neglect on functional outcome after stroke: a review of methodological issues and recent research findings. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 2006;24:209–215.
    1. Kapur N, Cole J, Manly T, Viskontas I, Ninteman A, Hasher L, Pascual-Leone A. Positive clinical neuroscience: explorations in positive neurology. The Neuroscientist : A Review Journal Bringing Neurobiology, Neurology and Psychiatry. 2013;19:354–369. doi: 10.1177/1073858412470976.
    1. Kapur N. Paradoxical functional facilitation in brain-behaviour research. A critical review. Brain. 1996;119:1775–1790. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.5.1775.
    1. Kerkhoff G, Schenk T. Rehabilitation of neglect: an update. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:1072–1079. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.024.
    1. Kinsbourne M. Hemi-neglect and hemisphere rivalry. In: Weinstein E. A, Friedland R. P, editors. Hemi-Inattention and Hemisphere Specialization. Vol. 18. New York: Raven Press; 1977. pp. 41–49.
    1. Koch G, Oliveri M, Cheeran B, Ruge D, Lo Gerfo E, Salerno S, Torriero S, Marconi B, Mori F, Driver J, Rothwell JC, Caltagirone C. Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact left-hemisphere of patients with neglect. Brain. 2008;131:3147–3155. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn273.
    1. Kording KP, Tenenbaum JB, Shadmehr R. The dynamics of memory as a consequence of optimal adaptation to a changing body. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:779–786. doi: 10.1038/nn1901.
    1. Kreis R. The trouble with quality filtering based on relative cramér-rao lower bounds. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2016;75:15–18. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25568.
    1. Landi SM, Baguear F, Della-Maggiore V. One week of motor adaptation induces structural changes in primary motor cortex that predict long-term memory one year later. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:11808–11813. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2253-11.2011.
    1. Lenhard A, Lenhard W. Calculation of Effect Sizes. [15 November, 2016];2014
    1. Li CS, Padoa-Schioppa C, Bizzi E. Neuronal correlates of motor performance and motor learning in the primary motor cortex of monkeys adapting to an external force field. Neuron. 2001;30:593–607. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00301-4.
    1. Làdavas E, Giulietti S, Avenanti A, Bertini C, Lorenzini E, Quinquinio C, Serino A. a-tDCS on the ipsilesional parietal cortex boosts the effects of prism adaptation treatment in neglect. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 2015;33:647–662. doi: 10.3233/RNN-140464.
    1. Manly T, Dobler VB, Dodds CM, George MA. Rightward shift in spatial awareness with declining alertness. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1721–1728. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.02.009.
    1. Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ, Thach WT. Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. Brain. 1996;119:1183–1198. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.4.1183.
    1. Mattingley JB, Bradshaw JL, Bradshaw JA, Nettleton NC. Recovery from directional hypokinesia and bradykinesia in unilateral neglect. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1994;16:861–876. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402699.
    1. Mekle R, Mlynárik V, Gambarota G, Hergt M, Krueger G, Gruetter R. MR spectroscopy of the human brain with enhanced signal intensity at ultrashort echo times on a clinical platform at 3T and 7T. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2009;61:1279–1285. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21961.
    1. Miall RC, Christensen LO, Cain O, Stanley J. Disruption of state estimation in the human lateral cerebellum. PLoS Biology. 2007;5:e316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050316.
    1. Michel C, Pisella L, Prablanc C, Rode G, Rossetti Y. Enhancing visuomotor adaptation by reducing error signals: single-step (aware) versus multiple-step (unaware) exposure to wedge prisms. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2007;19:341–350. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.2.341.
    1. Morey RD. Confidence intervals from normalized data: a correction to cousineau (2005) Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology. 2008;4:61–64. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061.
    1. Near J, Andersson J, Maron E, Mekle R, Gruetter R, Cowen P, Jezzard P. Unedited in vivo detection and quantification of γ-aminobutyric acid in the occipital cortex using short-TE MRS at 3 T. NMR in Biomedicine. 2013;26:1353–1362. doi: 10.1002/nbm.2960.
    1. Newport R, Brown L, Husain M, Mort D, Jackson SR. The role of the posterior parietal lobe in prism adaptation: Failure to adapt to optical prisms in a patient with bilateral damage to posterior parietal cortex. Cortex. 2006;42:720–729. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70410-6.
    1. Newport R, Schenk T. Prisms and neglect: what have we learned? Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:1080–1091. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.023.
    1. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology. 2001;57:1899–1901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899.
    1. Nitsche MA, Seeber A, Frommann K, Klein CC, Rochford C, Nitsche MS, Fricke K, Liebetanz D, Lang N, Antal A, Paulus W, Tergau F. Modulating parameters of excitability during and after transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. The Journal of Physiology. 2005;568:291–303. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.092429.
    1. O'Shea J, Boudrias MH, Stagg CJ, Bachtiar V, Kischka U, Blicher JU, Johansen-Berg H. Predicting behavioural response to TDCS in chronic motor stroke. NeuroImage. 2014a;85:924–933. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.096.
    1. O'Shea J, Gaveau V, Kandel M, Koga K, Susami K, Prablanc C, Rossetti Y. Kinematic markers dissociate error correction from sensorimotor realignment during prism adaptation. Neuropsychologia. 2014b;55:15–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.021.
    1. Oliveri M, Bisiach E, Brighina F, Piazza A, La Bua V, Buffa D, Fierro B. rTMS of the unaffected hemisphere transiently reduces contralesional visuospatial hemineglect. Neurology. 2001;57:1338–1340. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.7.1338.
    1. Ota H, Fujii T, Suzuki K, Fukatsu R, Yamadori A. Dissociation of body-centered and stimulus-centered representations in unilateral neglect. Neurology. 2001;57:2064–2069. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.11.2064.
    1. Panico F, Jacquin-Courtois S, Di Marco J, Perrin C, Trojano L, Rossetti Y. tDCS reactivation of dormant adaptation circuits. Cortex. 2017;94:196–199. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.003.
    1. Pisella L, Michel C, Gréa H, Tilikete C, Vighetto A, Rossetti Y. Preserved prism adaptation in bilateral optic ataxia: strategic versus adaptive reaction to prisms. Experimental Brain Research. 2004;156:399–408. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1746-4.
    1. Pisella L, Rossetti Y, Michel C, Rode G, Boisson D, Pélisson D, Tilikete C. Ipsidirectional impairment of prism adaptation after unilateral lesion of anterior cerebellum. Neurology. 2005;65:150–152. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000167945.34177.5e.
    1. Provencher SW. Automatic quantitation of localized in vivo 1H spectra with LCModel. NMR in Biomedicine. 2001;14:260–264. doi: 10.1002/nbm.698.
    1. Ramsey LE, Siegel JS, Baldassarre A, Metcalf NV, Zinn K, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Normalization of network connectivity in hemispatial neglect recovery. Annals of Neurology. 2016;80:127–141. doi: 10.1002/ana.24690.
    1. Redding GM, Rossetti Y, Wallace B. Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2005;29:431–444. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.004.
    1. Reis J, Robertson E, Krakauer JW, Rothwell J, Marshall L, Gerloff C, Wassermann E, Pascual-Leone A, Hummel F, Celnik PA, Classen J, Floel A, Ziemann U, Paulus W, Siebner HR, Born J, Cohen LG. Consensus: "Can tDCS and TMS enhance motor learning and memory formation?". Brain Stimulation. 2008;1:363–369. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.08.001.
    1. Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, Celnik PA, Krakauer JW. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. PNAS. 2009;106:1590–1595. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805413106.
    1. Richardson AG, Overduin SA, Valero-Cabré A, Padoa-Schioppa C, Pascual-Leone A, Bizzi E, Press DZ. Disruption of primary motor cortex before learning impairs memory of movement dynamics. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:12466–12470. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1139-06.2006.
    1. Rivera-Urbina GN, Batsikadze G, Molero-Chamizo A, Paulus W, Kuo MF, Nitsche MA. Parietal transcranial direct current stimulation modulates primary motor cortex excitability. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;41:845–855. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12840.
    1. Rossetti Y, Koga K, Mano T. Prismatic displacement of vision induces transient changes in the timing of eye-hand coordination. Perception & Psychophysics. 1993;54:355–364. doi: 10.3758/BF03205270.
    1. Rossetti Y, Rode G, Pisella L, Farné A, Li L, Boisson D, Perenin MT. Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature. 1998;395:166–169. doi: 10.1038/25988.
    1. Sawilowsky SS. New effect size rules of thumb. Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods. 2009;8:597–599. doi: 10.22237/jmasm/1257035100.
    1. Serino A, Barbiani M, Rinaldesi ML, Làdavas E. Effectiveness of prism adaptation in neglect rehabilitation: a controlled trial study. Stroke. 2009;40:1392–1398. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.530485.
    1. Simpson R, Devenyi GA, Jezzard P, Hennessy TJ, Near J. Advanced processing and simulation of MRS data using the FID appliance (FID-A)-An open source, MATLAB-based toolkit. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2017;77:23–33. doi: 10.1002/mrm.26091.
    1. Sparing R, Thimm M, Hesse MD, Küst J, Karbe H, Fink GR. Bidirectional alterations of interhemispheric parietal balance by non-invasive cortical stimulation. Brain. 2009;132:3011–3020. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp154.
    1. Stagg CJ, Bachtiar V, Johansen-Berg H. The role of GABA in human motor learning. Current Biology. 2011;21:480–484. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.069.
    1. Stagg CJ, Best JG, Stephenson MC, O'Shea J, Wylezinska M, Kincses ZT, Morris PG, Matthews PM, Johansen-Berg H. Polarity-sensitive modulation of cortical neurotransmitters by transcranial stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:5202–5206. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4432-08.2009.
    1. Stone SP, Halligan PW, Greenwood RJ. The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with an acute right or left hemisphere stroke. Age and Ageing. 1993;22:46–52. doi: 10.1093/ageing/22.1.46.
    1. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell'Acqua F, Forkel SJ, Simmons A, Vergani F, Murphy DG, Catani M. A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:1245–1246. doi: 10.1038/nn.2905.
    1. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Tomaiuolo F, Aiello M, Merola S, Silvetti M, Lecce F, Bartolomeo P, Doricchi F. Damage to white matter pathways in subacute and chronic spatial neglect: a group study and 2 single-case studies with complete virtual "in vivo" tractography dissection. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24:691–706. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs351.
    1. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Urbanski M, Duffau H, Volle E, Lévy R, Dubois B, Bartolomeo P. Direct evidence for a parietal-frontal pathway subserving spatial awareness in humans. Science. 2005;309:2226–2228. doi: 10.1126/science.1116251.
    1. Turner CE, Gant N. The Biochemistry of Creatine. In: Stagg C. J, Rothman D. J, editors. Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy: Tools for Neuroscience Research and Emerging Clinical Applications. London: Elsevier; 2014. pp. 91–103.
    1. van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, Gon JJ. Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. Journal of neurophysiology. 1999;81:1355–1364.
    1. Vuilleumier P, Hester D, Assal G, Regli F. Unilateral spatial neglect recovery after sequential strokes. Neurology. 1996;46:184–189. doi: 10.1212/WNL.46.1.184.
    1. Weiner MJ, Hallett M, Funkenstein HH. Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system. Neurology. 1983;33:766–772. doi: 10.1212/WNL.33.6.766.
    1. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Halligan P. Development of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1987;68:98–102.
    1. Wolf FM. Meta-Analysis: Quantitative Methods for Research Synthesis. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage; 1986.
    1. Zimerman M, Nitsch M, Giraux P, Gerloff C, Cohen LG, Hummel FC. Neuroenhancement of the aging brain: restoring skill acquisition in old subjects. Annals of Neurology. 2013;73:10–15. doi: 10.1002/ana.23761.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe