Mirror Visual Feedback to Improve Bradykinesia in Parkinson's Disease

Gaia Bonassi, Elisa Pelosin, Carla Ogliastro, Cecilia Cerulli, Giovanni Abbruzzese, Laura Avanzino, Gaia Bonassi, Elisa Pelosin, Carla Ogliastro, Cecilia Cerulli, Giovanni Abbruzzese, Laura Avanzino

Abstract

Mirror visual feedback (MVF) therapy has been applied to improve upper limb function in stroke. When combined with motor training, MVF improves the performance of the trained and untrained hand by enhancing the excitability of both primary motor cortices (M1s). Bradykinesia is a typical feature of Parkinson's disease (PD), characterized by slowness in the execution of movement. This condition is often asymmetrical and possibly supported by a volitional hypoactivation of M1. MVF therapy could tentatively treat bradykinesia since the untrained hand, which benefits from the exercise, is generally more severely impaired in undertaking sequential movements. Aim of the study was to evaluate whether MVF therapy may improve bradykinesia of the more affected hand in PD patients. Twelve PD patients and twelve healthy controls performed for 10 minutes a finger sequence, receiving MVF of the more affected/nondominant hand. Before and after MVF training, participants performed a finger sequence at their spontaneous pace with both hands. M1 excitability was assessed in the trained and untrained hemispheres by means of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Movement speed increased after MVF training in either hand of both groups. MVF therapy enhanced cortical excitability of M1s in both groups. Our preliminary data support the use of MVF therapy to improve bradykinesia in PD patients.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Experimental paradigm.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mirror visual feedback (MVF) training effect on behavioural data. Groups (Parkinson's disease (PD) patients, healthy subjects (HS)) and hand (trained, untrained) are indicated in the abscissa. Data recorded at baseline (before training) and after MVF training session are reported. Ordinate indicates the mean values of (a) number of finger movements performed in one minute during the assessments; (b) intertapping interval expressed in milliseconds; (c) touch duration expressed in milliseconds; and (d) % of correct sequences. Vertical bars indicate standard error of the mean (SEM). Asterisks indicate that in both groups the number of finger movements performed in one minute significantly increased and ITI significantly decreased after MVF training (p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Behavioural data, showing Δ score of the numbers of finger movements performed in one minute (number of finger movements/min after MVF training − number of finger movements/min before MVF training) in the trained and untrained hands of both Parkinson's disease (PD) patients (grey bars) and healthy subjects (HS) groups (white bars). Vertical bars indicate standard error of the mean (SEM).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Correlation analysis between individual changes in the number of finger tapping movements induced by mirror visual feedback practice and individual scores at the Parkinson's Fatigue Scale-16 (PFS-16) questionnaire in Parkinson's disease patients. There is a significant positive correlation between the improvement in the less affected/trained hand (a) and the clinical score (r = 0.64; p = 0.024), indicating that the higher the fatigability, the lower the performance improvement. In (b) the nonsignificant correlation between the improvement of the more affected/untrained hand and PFS-16 is depicted (r = 0.54, p = 0.07).
Figure 5
Figure 5
Input-Output (IO) curves measured in the first dorsal interosseus (FDI) muscle, of the trained (a and c) and the untrained (b and d) M1s before and after mirror visual feedback (MVF) training. Data of both groups, Parkinson's disease (PD) patients (a and b) and healthy subjects (HS) (c and d), who underwent MVF training, are shown. MEP amplitudes, in mV, are depicted from 90% to 130% S1mV (the stimulus intensity needed to evoke MEPs of approximately 0.8−1 mV peak-to-peak amplitude). Vertical bars indicate standard error of the mean (SEM).
Figure 6
Figure 6
Data from the control experiment (training without MVF). (a) shows the behavioural data, expressed as performance gain (Δ score of the numbers of finger movements performed in one minute) in the untrained hand of PD patients enrolled in the main experiment (PD mirror) and PD patients enrolled in the control experiment (PD control). In (b) the Input-Output (IO) curves of the untrained M1 of the PD control group, before and after training, are depicted. MEP amplitudes, in mV, are depicted from 90% to 130% S1mV (the stimulus intensity needed to evoke MEPs of approximately 0.8−1 mV peak-to-peak amplitude). Vertical bars indicate standard error of the mean (SEM). p < 0.05.

References

    1. Wilson S. A. K. The croonian lectures on some disorders of motility and of muscle tone. The Lancet. 1925;206(5315):53–62. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)17142-4.
    1. Marsden C. D. The mysterious motor function of the basal ganglia: the Robert Wartenberg Lecture. Neurology. 1982;32(5):514–539. doi: 10.1212/wnl.32.5.514.
    1. Rodriguez-Oroz M. C., Jahanshahi M., Krack P., et al. Initial clinical manifestations of Parkinson's disease: features and pathophysiological mechanisms. The Lancet Neurology. 2009;8(12):1128–1139. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70293-5.
    1. Nakamura R., Nagasaki H., Narabayashi H. Disturbances of rhythm formation in patients with Parkinson's disease: Part I. Characteristics of tapping response to the periodic signals. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1978;46(1):63–75. doi: 10.2466/pms.1978.46.1.63.
    1. Pastor M. A., Artieda J., Jahanshahi M., Obeso J. A. Time estimation and reproduction is abnormal in Parkinson's disease. Brain. 1992;115(1):211–225. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.1.211.
    1. Hausdorff J. M., Cudkowicz M. E., Firtion R., Wei J. Y., Goldberger A. L. Gait variability and basal ganglia disorders: stride-to-stride variations of gait cycle timing in Parkinson's disease and Huntington's disease. Movement Disorders. 1998;13(3):428–437. doi: 10.1002/mds.870130310.
    1. Espay A. J., Giuffrida J. P., Chen R., et al. Differential response of speed, amplitude, and rhythm to dopaminergic medications in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders. 2011;26(14):2504–2508. doi: 10.1002/mds.23893.
    1. Pelosin E., Bove M., Ruggeri P., Avanzino L., Abbruzzese G. Reduction of bradykinesia of finger movements by a single session of action observation in Parkinson disease. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2013;27(6):552–560. doi: 10.1177/1545968312471905.
    1. Deconinck F. J. A., Smorenburg A. R. P., Benham A., Ledebt A., Feltham M. G., Savelsbergh G. J. P. Reflections on mirror therapy: a systematic review of the effect of mirror visual feedback on the brain. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2015;29(4):349–361. doi: 10.1177/1545968314546134.
    1. Ramachandran V. S., Altschuler E. L. The use of visual feedback, in particular mirror visual feedback, in restoring brain function. Brain. 2009;132(7):1693–1710. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp135.
    1. Ramachandran V. S., Rodgers-Ramachandran D. Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1996;263(1369):377–386. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1996.0058.
    1. Sütbeyaz S., Yavuzer G., Sezer N., Koseoglu B. F. Mirror therapy enhances lower-extremity motor recovery and motor functioning after stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2007;88(5):555–559. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.02.034.
    1. Moseley G. L. Graded motor imagery is effective for long-standing complex regional pain syndrome: a randomised controlled trial. Pain. 2004;108(1-2):192–198. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.01.006.
    1. Hoff M., Kaminski E., Rjosk V., et al. Augmenting mirror visual feedback-induced performance improvements in older adults. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;41(11):1475–1483. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12899.
    1. Nojima I., Mima T., Koganemaru S., Thabit M. N., Fukuyama H., Kawamata T. Human motor plasticity induced by mirror visual feedback. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32(4):1293–1300. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5364-11.2012.
    1. Avanzino L., Raffo A., Pelosin E., et al. Training based on mirror visual feedback influences transcallosal communication. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;40(3):2581–2588. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12615.
    1. Nojima I., Oga T., Fukuyama H., Kawamata T., Mima T. Mirror visual feedback can induce motor learning in patients with callosal disconnection. Experimental Brain Research. 2013;227(1):79–83. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3486-4.
    1. Berardelli A., Rothwell J. C., Thompson P. D., Hallett M. Pathophysiology of bradykinesia in parkinson's disease. Brain. 2001;124(11):2131–2146. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.11.2131.
    1. Stocchi F., Abbruzzese G., Ceravolo R., et al. Prevalence of fatigue in Parkinson disease and its clinical correlates. Neurology. 2014;83(3):215–220. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000587.
    1. Elbers R., van Wegen E. E. H., Rochester L., et al. Is impact of fatigue an independent factor associated with physical activity in patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease? Movement Disorders. 2009;24(10):1512–1518. doi: 10.1002/mds.22664.
    1. Tremblay F., Tremblay L. E. Cortico-motor excitability of the lower limb motor representation: a comparative study in Parkinson's disease and healthy controls. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2002;113(12):2006–2012. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00301-2.
    1. Valls-Solé J., Pascual-Leone A., Brasil-Neto J. P., Cammarota A., McShane A., Hallett M. Abnormal facilitation of the response to transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 1994;44(4):735–741. doi: 10.1212/WNL.44.4.735.
    1. Ellaway P. H., Davey N. J., Maskill D. W., Dick J. P. R. The relation between bradykinesia and excitability of the motor cortex assessed using transcranial magnetic stimulation in normal and parkinsonian subjects. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Electromyography and Motor Control. 1995;97(3):169–178. doi: 10.1016/0924-980x(94)00336-6.
    1. Heldman D. A., Giuffrida J. P., Chen R., et al. The modified bradykinesia rating scale for Parkinson's disease: reliability and comparison with kinematic measures. Movement Disorders. 2011;26(10):1859–1863. doi: 10.1002/mds.23740.
    1. Brown R. G., Dittner A., Findley L., Wessely S. C. The Parkinson fatigue scale. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders. 2005;11(1):49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2004.07.007.
    1. Oldfield R. C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.
    1. Franz E. A., Packman T. Fooling the brain into thinking it sees both hands moving enhances bimanual spatial coupling. Experimental Brain Research. 2004;157(2):174–180.
    1. Möller C., Arai N., Lücke J., Ziemann U. Hysteresis effects on the input-output curve of motor evoked potentials. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2009;120(5):1003–1008. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.03.001.
    1. Grèzes J., Decety J. Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping. 2000;12(1):1–19.
    1. Rizzolatti G., Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2004;27:169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230.
    1. Buccino G., Solodkin A., Small S. L. Functions of the mirror neuron system: implications for neurorehabilitation. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 2006;19(1):55–63. doi: 10.1097/00146965-200603000-00007.
    1. Matthys K., Smits M., Van der Geest J. N., et al. Mirror-induced visual illusion of hand movements: a Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2009;90(4):675–681. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2008.09.571.
    1. Haith A. M., Krakauer J. W. Model-based and model-free mechanisms of human motor learning. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology. 2013;782:1–21. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5465-6_1.
    1. Vahdat S., Darainy M., Ostry D. J. Structure of plasticity in human sensory and motor networks due to perceptual learning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34(7):2451–2463. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4291-13.2014.
    1. Lou J.-S., Kearns G., Benice T., Oken B., Sexton G., Nutt J. Levodopa improves physical fatigue in Parkinson's disease: a double-blind, placebo controlled, crossover study. Movement Disorders. 2003;18(10):1108–1114. doi: 10.1002/mds.10505.
    1. Agostino R., Berardelli A., Formica A., Accornero N., Manfredi M. Sequential arm movements in patients with Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease and dystonia. Brain. 1992;115(5):1481–1495. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.5.1481.
    1. Devanne H., Lavoie B. A., Capaday C. Input-output properties and gain changes in the human corticospinal pathway. Experimental Brain Research. 1997;114(2):329–338. doi: 10.1007/PL00005641.
    1. Ridding M. C., Rothwell J. C. Stimulus/response curves as a method of measuring motor cortical excitability in man. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1997;105(5):340–344. doi: 10.1016/s0924-980x(97)00041-6.
    1. Fink G. R., Marshall J. C., Halligan P. W., et al. The neural consequences of conflict between intention and the senses. Brain. 1999;122(3):497–512. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.3.497.
    1. Grafton S. T., Hazeltine E., Ivry R. B. Motor sequence learning with the nondominant left hand: a PET functional imaging study. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;146(3):369–378. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1181-y.
    1. Perez M. A., Tanaka S., Wise S. P., et al. Neural substrates of intermanual transfer of a newly acquired motor skill. Current Biology. 2007;17(21):1896–1902. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.09.058.
    1. Perez M. A., Wise S. P., Willingham D. T., Cohen L. G. Neurophysiological mechanisms involved in transfer of procedural knowledge. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(5):1045–1053. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4128-06.2007.
    1. Läppchen C. H., Ringer T., Blessin J., et al. Optical illusion alters M1 excitability after mirror therapy: a TMS study. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;108(10):2857–2861. doi: 10.1152/jn.00321.2012.
    1. Osman M., Bird G., Heyes C. Action observation supports effector-dependent learning of finger movement sequences. Experimental Brain Research. 2005;165(1):19–27. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2275-0.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe