Exploring Self-Consciousness From Self- and Other-Image Recognition in the Mirror: Concepts and Evaluation

Gaëlle Keromnes, Sylvie Chokron, Macarena-Paz Celume, Alain Berthoz, Michel Botbol, Roberto Canitano, Foucaud Du Boisgueheneuc, Nemat Jaafari, Nathalie Lavenne-Collot, Brice Martin, Tom Motillon, Bérangère Thirioux, Valeria Scandurra, Moritz Wehrmann, Ahmad Ghanizadeh, Sylvie Tordjman, Gaëlle Keromnes, Sylvie Chokron, Macarena-Paz Celume, Alain Berthoz, Michel Botbol, Roberto Canitano, Foucaud Du Boisgueheneuc, Nemat Jaafari, Nathalie Lavenne-Collot, Brice Martin, Tom Motillon, Bérangère Thirioux, Valeria Scandurra, Moritz Wehrmann, Ahmad Ghanizadeh, Sylvie Tordjman

Abstract

A historical review of the concepts of self-consciousness is presented, highlighting the important role of the body (particularly, body perception but also body action), and the social other in the construction of self-consciousness. More precisely, body perception, especially intermodal sensory perception including kinesthetic perception, is involved in the construction of a sense of self allowing self-other differentiation. Furthermore, the social other, through very early social and emotional interactions, provides meaning to the infant's perception and contributes to the development of his/her symbolization capacities. This is a necessary condition for body image representation and awareness of a permanent self in a time-space continuum (invariant over time and space). Self-image recognition impairments in the mirror are also discussed regarding a comprehensive developmental theory of self-consciousness. Then, a neuropsychological and neurophysiological approach to self-consciousness reviews the role of complex brain activation/integration pathways and the mirror neuron system in self-consciousness. Finally, this article offers new perspectives on self-consciousness evaluation using a double mirror paradigm to study self- and other- image and body recognition.

Keywords: body action; body image; body perception; body-self; development; intermodal sensory perception; self; self-consciousness.

References

    1. Abely P. (1930). Le signe du miroir dans les psychoses et plus spécialement dans la démence précoce. Ann. Med. Psychol. 1 28–36.
    1. Adrian E. D. (1932). “The activity of nerve fibres,” in Nobel Lectures, Physiology or Medicine (Amsterdam: Elsevier Publishing Company; ), 1922–1941.
    1. Aspell J. E., Lenggenhager B., Blanke O. (2009). Keeping in touch with one’s self: multisensory mechanisms of self-consciousness. PLoS One 4:e6488. 10.1371/journal.pone.0006488
    1. Aspell J. E., Lenggenhager B., Blanke O. (2012). “Multisensory perception and bodily self-consciousness: from out-of-body to inside-body experience,” in The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes, eds Murray M. M., Wallace M. T. (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; ).
    1. Bisiach E., Rusconi M. L., Vallar G. (1991). Remission of somatoparaphrenic delusion through vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 29 1029–1031. 10.1016/0028-3932(91)90066-H
    1. Blakemore S. J., Frith C. D., Wolpert D. M. (1999). Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11 551–559. 10.1162/089892999563607
    1. Blanke O. (2007). I and me: self-portraiture in brain damage. Front. Neurol. Neurosci. 22:14–29. 10.1159/000102822
    1. Blanke O., Mohr C. (2005). Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin: Implications for neurocognitive mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self-consciousness. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 1 184–199. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.05.008
    1. Blanke O., Mohr C., Michel C. M., Pascual-Leone A., Brugger P., Seeck M., et al. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self-processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. J. Neurosci. J. Soc. Neurosci. 19 550–557. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-04.2005
    1. Bonnier P. (1904). Le Sens Des Attitudes. Paris: C. Naud.
    1. Bonnot O., Tanguy M.-L., Consoli A., Cornic F., Graindorge C., Laurent C., et al. (2008). Does catatonia influence the phenomenology of childhood onset schizophrenia beyond motor symptoms? Psychiatry Res. 158 356–362. 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.09.006
    1. Bortolon C., Capdevielle D., Altman R., Macgregor A., Attal J., Raffard S. (2017). Mirror self-face perception in individuals with schizophrenia: feelings of strangeness associated with one’s own image. Psychiatry Res. 253 205–210. 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.03.055
    1. Botvinick M., Cohen J. (1998). Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature 391:20. 10.1038/35784
    1. Brandt T., Dieterich M., Danek A. (1994). Vestibular cortex lesions affect the perception of verticality. Ann. Neurol. 35 403–412. 10.1002/ana.410350406
    1. Calvo-Merino B., Glaser D. E., Grèzes J., Passingham R. E., Haggard P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb. Cortex 15 1243–1249. 10.1093/cercor/bhi007
    1. Cappa S., Sterzi R., Vallar G., Bisiach E. (1987). Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 25 775–782. 10.1016/0028-3932(87)90115-1
    1. Caputo G. B., Ferruci R., Bortolomasi M., Giacopuzzi M., Priori A., Zago S. (2012). Visual perception during mirror gazing at one’s face in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 140 46–50. 10.1016/j.schres.2012.06.029
    1. Chambon V., Wenke D., Fleming S. M., Prinz W., Haggard P. (2013). An online neural substrate for sense of agency. Cereb. Cortex 23 1031–1037. 10.1093/cercor/bhs059
    1. Contreras M., Ceric F., Torrealba F. (2007). Inactivation of the interoceptive insula disrupts drug craving and malaise induced by lithium. Science 26 655–658. 10.1126/science.1145590
    1. Craig A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3 655–666. 10.1038/nrn894
    1. Craig A. D. (2009). How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 59–70. 10.1038/nrn2555
    1. Craig A. D., Reiman E. M., Evans A., Bushnell M. C. (1996). Functional imaging of an illusion of pain. Nature 21 258–260. 10.1038/384258a0
    1. Damasio A. R., Grabowski T. J., Bechara A., Damasio H., Ponto L. L., Parvizi J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat. Neurosci. 3 1049–1056. 10.1038/79871
    1. Damasio A. R., Tiercelin C., Larsonneur C. (1999). Le Sentiment Même de Soi : Corps, Émotion, Conscience. Édition: Coll. Sciences. Paris: Editions Odile Jacob, 380.
    1. Decety J., Grèzes J. (2006). The power of simulation: imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Res. 1079 4–14. 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.115
    1. Decety J., Sommerville J. A. (2003). Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7 527–533. 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.004
    1. Delmas F. A. (1929). Le signe du miroir dans la démence précoce. Ann. Med. Psychol. 1 227–233.
    1. Devue C., Collette F., Balteau E., Degueldre C., Luxen A., Maquet P., et al. (2007). Here I am: the cortical correlates of visual self-recognition. Brain Res. 1143 169–182. 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.055
    1. Dolto F., Nasio J. D. (2002). L’enfant Du Miroir. Paris: Payot et Rivages.
    1. Farrer C., Bouchereau M., Jeannerod M., Franck N. (2008). Effect of distorted visual feedback on the sense of agency. Behav. Neurol. 19 53–57. 10.1155/2008/425267
    1. Farrer C., Franck N., Georgieff N., Frith C. D., Decety J., Jeannerod M. (2003). Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study. Neuroimage 18 324–333. 10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00041-1
    1. Feldman R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48 329–354. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x
    1. Gallagher S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4 14–21. 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01417-5
    1. Gallagher S. (2005). How the Body Shapes the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 294.
    1. Gernet I. (2007). Corps et subjectivité. Evol. Psychiatr. 72 338–345. 10.1016/j.evopsy.2007.04.003
    1. Graziano M. S. A. (2018). The temporoparietal junction and awareness. Neurosci. Conscious. 4:niy005. 10.1093/nc/niy005
    1. Haag G., Botbol M., Graignic R., Perez-Diaz F., Bronsard G., Kermarrec S., et al. (2010). The Autism Psychodynamic Evaluation of Changes (APEC) scale: a reliability and validity study on a newly developed standardized psychodynamic assessment for youth with Pervasive Developmental Disorders. J. Physiol. Paris 104 323–336. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2010.10.002
    1. Haag G., Tordjman S., Duprat A., Urwand S., Jardin F., Clément M.-C., et al. (2005). Psychodynamic assessment of changes in children with autism under psychoanalytic treatment. Int. J. Psychoanal. 86 335–352. 10.1516/WAB4-DW0R-8N9B-1UH8
    1. Harrington A., Spitzer M. (1989). Disordered recognition and perception of human face in acute schizophrenia and experimental psychosis. Compr. Psychiatry 30 376–384. 10.1016/0010-440X(89)90003-5
    1. Head H., Holmes H. G. (1911. –1912). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 34 102–254. 10.1093/brain/34.2-3.102
    1. Hickok G. (2014). The Myth of Mirror Neurons: the Real Neuroscience of Communication and Cognition. New York, NY: W.W. Norton, 288.
    1. Ionta S., Gassert R., Blanke O. (2011). Multi-sensory and sensorimotor foundation of bodily selfconsciousness - an interdisciplinary approach. Front. Psychol. 2:383. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00383
    1. Jones B. C., Mormède P. (2002). Neurobehavioral Genetics: Methods and Application. Broca Raton: CRC Press.
    1. Jones P. (1997). The early origins of schizophrenia. Br. Med. Bull. 53 135–155. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a011596
    1. Karnath H. O., Baier B. (2010). Right insula for our sense of limb ownership and self-awareness of actions. Brain Struct. Funct. 214 411–417. 10.1007/s00429-010-0250-4
    1. Karnath H. O., Fruhmann Berger M., Küker W., Rorden C. (2004). The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. Cereb. Cortex 14 1164–1172. 10.1093/cercor/bhh076
    1. Keromnes G., Martin B., Tordjman S. (2017). Exploration de la conscience de soi à partir de la reconnaissance de l’image de soi et de l’autre dans le miroir : intérêts dans les troubles schizophréniques et autistiques. Psychiatr. Française 48 57–94.
    1. Keromnes G., Motillon T., Coulon N., Berthoz A., Du Boisgueheneuc F., Wehrmann M., et al. (2018). Self-other rcognition impairments in individuals with schizophrenia: a new experimental paradigm using a double mirror. NPJ Schizophr. 4:24. 10.1038/s41537-018-0065-5
    1. Kiverstein J. (2007). Could a robot have a subjective point of view? J. Conscious. Stud. 14 127–139.
    1. Kong J., White N. S., Kwong K. K., Vangel M. G., Rosman I. S., Gracely R. H., et al. (2006). Using fMRI to dissociate sensory encoding from cognitive evaluation of heat pain intensity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27 715–721. 10.1002/hbm.20213
    1. Lacan J. (1966). Le Stade Du Miroir Comme Formateur De La Fonction Du Je Telle Qu’elle Nous Est Révélée Dans L’expérience Psychanalytique in Ecrits. Paris: Seuil, 93–94.
    1. Lhermitte J. (1939). L’image De Notre Corps. Paris: L’Harmattan.
    1. Lueck A., Dutton G. N. (2015). Vision and the Brain. New York, NY: AFB Press.
    1. Maine de Biran P. (1834). Nouvelles Considérations Sur Les Rapports Du Physique Et Du Moral De L’homme. Paris: Ladrange.
    1. Martin B., Wittmann M., Franck N., Cermolacce M., Berna F., Giersch A. (2014). Temporal structure of consciousness and minimal self in schizophrenia. Front. Psychol. 5:1175. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01175
    1. Meaulle D. (2007). Le signe du miroir : reflets cliniques et théoriques. Evol. Psychol. 72 81–97. 10.1016/j.evopsy.2007.01.005
    1. Melchior-Bonnet S. (2011). L’invention du reflet. TDC 1008 18–19.
    1. Merleau-Ponty M. (1964). Les relations avec autrui chez l’enfant. Bull. Psychol. 18 295–336.
    1. Mishara A. L., Lysaker P. H., Schwartz M. A. (2014). Self-disturbances in schizophrenia: history, phenomenology, and relevant findings from research on metacognition. Schizophr. Bull. 40 5–12. 10.1093/schbul/sbt169
    1. Müller U., Runions K. (2003). The origins of understanding self and other: James Mark Baldwin’s theory. Dev. Rev. 23 29–54. 10.1016/S0273-2297(03)00004-2
    1. Murata A., Wen W., Asama H. (2016). The body and objects represented in the ventral stream of the parieto-premotor network. Neurosci. Res. 104 4–15. 10.1016/j.neures.2015.10.010
    1. Nadel J. (2011). Imiter Pour Grandir. Développement Du Bébé Et De L’enfant Avec Autisme. Paris: Dunod.
    1. Nadel J., Baudonnière P. M., Fontaine A. M. (1983). Les comportements sociaux imitatifs. Recherches Psychologie Sociale 5 15–29.
    1. Nadel-Brulfert J., Baudonnière P. M. (1982). The social function of reciprocal imitation in 2 years-old peers. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 5 95–109. 10.1177/016502548200500105
    1. Naqvi N. H., Rudrauf D., Damasio H., Bechara A. (2007). Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science 315 531–534. 10.1126/science.1135926
    1. Nathanson M., Bergman P. S., Gordon G. G. (1952). Denial of illness; its occurrence in one hundred consecutive cases of hemiplegia. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 68 380–387. 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1952.02320210090010
    1. Neisser U. (1991). Two perceptually given aspects of the self and their development. Dev. Rev. 11 197–209. 10.1016/0273-2297(91)90009-D
    1. Nordenskiôld E. (1926). Miroirs convexes et concaves en Amérique. J. Soc. Am. 18 103–110. 10.3406/jsa.1926.3607
    1. Parnas J., Henriksen M. G. (2014). Disordered self in the schizophrenia spectrum: a clinical and research perspective. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 22 251–265. 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000040
    1. Pasqualini I., Llobera J., Blanke O. (2013). “Seeing” and “feeling” architecture: how bodily self-consciousness alters architectonic experience and affects the perception of interiors. Front. Psychol. 4:354. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00354
    1. Pfeiffer C., Serino A., Blanke O. (2014). The vestibular system: a spatial reference for bodily self-consciousness. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:31. 10.3389/fnint.2014.00031
    1. Phillips M. L., Young A. W., Senior C., Brammer M., Andrew C., Calder A. J., et al. (1997). A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature 389 495–498. 10.1038/39051
    1. Piaget J. (1936). La Naissance De L’intelligence Chez L’enfant. Neuchatel: Delachaux et Niestlé.
    1. Piaget J. (1956). Les Stades Du Développement Intellectuel De L’enfant Et De L’adolescent. Le problème Des Stades En Psychologie De L’enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de Paris, 33–42.
    1. Prinz W. (2013). Self in the mirror. Conscious. Cogn. 22 1105–1113. 10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.007
    1. Rizzolatti G., Sinigaglia C. (2008). Neurones Miroirs (Les). Paris: Editions Odile Jacob, 252.
    1. Rochat P. (2003). Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life. Conscious. Cogn. 12 717–731. 10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00081-3
    1. Rochat P. (2018). The ontogeny of human self-consciousness. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27 345–50 10.1177/0963721418760236
    1. Rode G., Charles N., Perenin M. T., Vighetto A., Trillet M., Aimard G. (1992). Partial remission of hemiplegia and somatoparaphrenia through vestibular stimulation in a case of unilateral neglect. Cortex 28 203–208. 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80048-2
    1. Rustin E., Soulairac A., Shentoub S. A. (1954). Comportement de l’enfant arriéré devant le miroir. Enfance 7 333–340. 10.3406/enfan.1954.1469
    1. Schilder P. (1968). L’image Du Corps. Paris: Gallimard.
    1. Seth A. K., Izhikevich E., Reeke G. N., Edelman G. M. (2006). Theories and measures of consciousness: an extended framework. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103 10799–10804. 10.1073/pnas.0604347103
    1. Sforza A., Bufalari I., Haggard P., Aglioti S. M. (2010). My face in yours: visuo-tactile facial stimulation influences sense of identity. Soc. Neurosci. 5 148–162. 10.1080/17470910903205503
    1. Sherrington C. S. (1906). The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. New York, NY: Charles Scribner’s Sons.
    1. Sirigu A., Daprati E., Pradat-Diehl P., Franck N., Jeannerod M. (1999). Perception of self-generated movement following left parietal lesion. Brain 122(Pt 10), 1867–1874. 10.1093/brain/122.10.1867
    1. Slowinski P., Alderisio F., Zhai C., Shen Y., Tino P., Bortolon C., et al. (2017). Unravelling socio-motor biomarkers in schizophrenia. NPJ Schizophr. 3:8. 10.1038/s41537-016-0009-x
    1. Starobinski J. (1977). Le concept de cénesthésie et les idées neuropsychologiques de Moritz Schiff. Gesnerus 34 2–20.
    1. Tackett J. L., Balsis S., Oltmanns T. F., Krueger R. F. (2009). A unifying perspective on personality pathology across the lifespan: developmental considerations for the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Dev. Psychopathol. 21 687–713. 10.1017/S095457940900039X
    1. Taylor J. G. (2001). The central role of the parietal lobes in consciousness. Conscious Cogn. 10 379–417. 10.1006/ccog.2000.0495
    1. Testa A., Giannuzzi R., Sollazzo F., Petrongolo L., Bernardini L., Daini S. (2013). Psychiatric emergencies (part I): psychiatric disorders causing organic symptoms. Eur. Rev. Med. Pharmacol. Sci. 17(Suppl. 1), 55–64.
    1. Thirioux B., Wehrmann M., Langbour N., Jaafari N., Berthoz A. (2016). Identifying oneself with the self of someone else impairs the egocentered visuo-spatial mechanisms: a new Double Mirror Paradigm to study self-other distinction and interaction. Front. Psychol. 7:1283. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01283
    1. Tononi G., Edelman G. M. (2000). Schizophrenia and the mechanisms of conscious integration. Brain Res. Rev. 31 391–400. 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00056-9
    1. Tordjman S., Maillhes A. S. (2009). Les troubles du développement de l’image du corps dans la petite enfance : une dimension commune partagée par la schizophrénie et l’autisme ? Neuropsychiatr. Enfance Adolesc. 57 6–13. 10.1016/j.neurenf.2008.09.005
    1. Tsakiris M., Hesse M. D., Boy C., Haggard P., Fink G. R. (2007). Neural signatures of body ownership: a sensory network for bodily self-consciousness. Cereb. Cortex 17 2235–2244. 10.1093/cercor/bhl131
    1. Vallar G., Bottini G., Sterzi R. (2003). Anosognosia for left-sided motor and sensory deficits, motor neglect, and sensory hemiinattention: is there a relationship? Prog. Brain Res. 142 289–301. 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)42020-7
    1. Van Veluw S. J., Chance S. A. (2014). Differentiating between self and others: an ALE metaanalysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind. Brain Imaging Behav. 8 24–38. 10.1007/s11682-013-9266-8
    1. Vygotsky L. S. (1933). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Sov. Psychol. 5 6–18. 10.2753/RPO1061-040505036
    1. Vygotsky L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
    1. Wallon H. (1934). Les Origines Du Caractère Chez L’enfant, 5e Edn. Paris: PUF.
    1. Wallon H. (1959a). Kinesthésie et image visuelle du corps propre chez l’enfant. Enfance 12 252–263. 10.3406/enfan.1959.1440
    1. Wallon H. (1959b). Le rôle de l’autre dans la conscience du moi. Enfance 12 277–286. 10.3406/enfan.1959.1443
    1. Wallon H. (1984). L’enfant Turbulent. Paris: P.U.F.
    1. Webb T. W., Graziano M. S. A. (2015). The attention schema theory: a mechanistic account of subjective awareness. Front. Psychol. 6:500. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00500
    1. Zazzo R. (1948). Images du corps et conscience de soi. Enfance 1 29–43. 10.3406/enfan.1948.1295

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe