Localization of Brain Networks Engaged by the Sustained Attention to Response Task Provides Quantitative Markers of Executive Impairment in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis

Roisin McMackin, Stefan Dukic, Emmet Costello, Marta Pinto-Grau, Antonio Fasano, Teresa Buxo, Mark Heverin, Richard Reilly, Muthuraman Muthuraman, Niall Pender, Orla Hardiman, Bahman Nasseroleslami, Roisin McMackin, Stefan Dukic, Emmet Costello, Marta Pinto-Grau, Antonio Fasano, Teresa Buxo, Mark Heverin, Richard Reilly, Muthuraman Muthuraman, Niall Pender, Orla Hardiman, Bahman Nasseroleslami

Abstract

Objective: To identify cortical regions engaged during the sustained attention to response task (SART) and characterize changes in their activity associated with the neurodegenerative condition amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS).

Methods: High-density electroencephalography (EEG) was recorded from 33 controls and 23 ALS patients during a SART paradigm. Differences in associated event-related potential peaks were measured for Go and NoGo trials. Sources active during these peaks were localized, and ALS-associated differences were quantified.

Results: Go and NoGo N2 and P3 peak sources were localized to the left primary motor cortex, bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and lateral posterior parietal cortex (PPC). NoGo trials evoked greater bilateral medial PPC activity during N2 and lesser left insular, PPC and DLPFC activity during P3. Widespread cortical hyperactivity was identified in ALS during P3. Changes in the inferior parietal lobule and insular activity provided very good discrimination (AUROC > 0.75) between patients and controls. Activation of the right precuneus during P3 related to greater executive function in ALS, indicative of a compensatory role.

Interpretation: The SART engages numerous frontal and parietal cortical structures. SART-EEG measures correlate with specific cognitive impairments that can be localized to specific structures, aiding in differential diagnosis.

Keywords: EEG; amyotrophic lateral sclerosis; attention; hyperactivity; source localization.

© The Author(s) 2020. Published by Oxford University Press.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean Go (blue) and NoGo (red) trial ERPs in controls and ALS patients. N2 peaks are visible in the NoGo trial ERP in Fz and Cz in the 220–350-ms window. P3 peaks are present in the 350–550-ms window in both Go and NoGo trial ERPs in all electrodes. Green asterisks represent significantly larger P3 peak amplitudes in NoGo versus Go trials. Red asterisks represent significantly larger (more negative) N2 peak amplitudes in NoGo versus Go trials. Black asterisks represent significant differences in NoGo-Go N2 peak amplitude between ALS patients and controls. **P < 0.01, ****P < 0.0001. CON: controls.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Correlations between NoGo minus Go (NoGo-Go) N2 peak amplitude in Cz and cognitive task performance. (A) Correlation with response time and NoGo trial accuracy in controls demonstrates that those with smaller NoGo versus Go N2 peak differences had significantly faster response times and better NoGo accuracy. (B) Correlation with patient ECAS total and ALS-specific score demonstrates that those with smaller (less negative) N2 peak differences had lower ECAS scores.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Correlations between P3 peak characteristics and SART performance. In controls, (A) later responses correlate with later P3 peaks in Fz during NoGo trials, (B) better NoGo accuracy inversely correlates with Go P3 peak size in Cz, (C) Go accuracy positively correlates with NoGo P3 peak amplitude in Pz, and (D) overall accuracy inversely correlates with NoGo P3 peak amplitude in Fz. In all participants, (E) later responses correlate with longer peak latency and (F) smaller peak amplitude during Go trials in Cz. In patients, (G) greater overall accuracy correlates with longer Go P3 peak latency in Cz.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Primary sources (regions with top 5% power) of N2 during Go trials, NoGo trials, and NoGo trials relative to Go trials (“difference”) in controls (first rows) and patients (second rows).
Figure 5
Figure 5
Primary sources (regions with 5% power) of P3 during Go trials, NoGo trials, and NoGo trials relative to Go trials (“difference”) in controls (first rows) and patients (second rows).
Figure 6
Figure 6
P3 sources with statistically significant differences in activity in ALS compared with controls. Differences between NoGo and Go trial source activity during the P3 peak were compared between ALS patients and controls. All highlighted areas represent significant (FDR = 10%, type II error=0.38 Bayesian Posterior probability=0.87) increases in power with heat map values representing AUROC = –0.5 (i.e., perfect discrimination = 0.5). Orthogonal MRI scans show only those differences with an AUROC >0.75, that is, very good discriminators. AUROC: area under the receivership operating curve.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Greater behavioral inhibition in ALS is associated with increased right precuneus activity during NoGo P3 relative to Go P3. Higher CWIT inhibition score indicates poorer behavioral inhibition.

References

    1. Bellgrove MA, Hawi Z, Gill M, Robertson IH. 2006. The cognitive genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): sustained attention as a candidate phenotype. Cortex. 426:838–845.
    1. Benjamini Y. 2010. Discovering the false discovery rate. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: series B (Statistical Methodology). 72:405–416.
    1. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological). 57:289–300.
    1. Beste C, Saft C, Andrich J, Gold R, Falkenstein M. 2008. Response inhibition in Huntington’s disease-a study using ERPs and sLORETA. Neuropsychologia. 46:1290–1297.
    1. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. 2006. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 129:564–583.
    1. Chen Q, Garcea FE, Jacobs RA, Mahon BZ. 2018. Abstract representations of object-directed action in the left inferior parietal lobule. Cerebral Cortex. 28:2162–2174.
    1. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Holdnack J. 2004. Reliability and validity of the Delis-Kaplan executive function system: an update. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 10:301–303.
    1. Delorme A, Makeig S. 2004. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 134:9–21.
    1. Douw L, Nieboer D, Stam CJ, Tewarie P, Hillebrand A. 2018. Consistency of magnetoencephalographic functional connectivity and network reconstruction using a template versus native MRI for co-registration. Human Brain Mapping. 39:104–119.
    1. Dukic S, McMackin R, Buxo T, Fasano A, Chipika R, Pinto-Grau M, Costello E, Schuster C, Hammond M, Heverin M, et al. . 2019. Patterned functional network disruption in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Human Brain Mapping. 40:4827–4842.
    1. Efron B. 2009. Empirical Bayes estimates for large-scale prediction problems. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 104:1015–1028.
    1. Elamin M, Phukan J, Bede P, Jordan N, Byrne S, Pender N, Hardiman O. 2011. Executive dysfunction is a negative prognostic indicator in patients with ALS without dementia. Neurology. 76:1263–1269.
    1. Fonov V, Evans AC, Botteron K, Almli CR, McKinstry RC, Collins DL. 2011. Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage. 54:313–327.
    1. Fuchs M, Kastner J, Wagner M, Hawes S, Ebersole JS. 2002. A standardized boundary element method volume conductor model. Clinical Neurophysiology. 113:702–712.
    1. Gan L, Cookson MR, Petrucelli L, La Spada AR. 2018. Converging pathways in neurodegeneration, from genetics to mechanisms. Nature Neuroscience. 21:1300–1309.
    1. Gregory JM, McDade K, Bak TH, Pal S, Chandran S, Smith C, Abrahams S. 2019. Executive, language and fluency dysfunction are markers of localised TDP-43 cerebral pathology in non-demented ALS. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, Psychiatry.
    1. Hajian-Tilaki K. 2013. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analysis for medical diagnostic test evaluation. Caspian Journal of Internal Medicine. 4:627–635.
    1. Hart EP, Dumas EM, Reijntjes R, Van Der Hiele K, Van Den Bogaard SJA, Middelkoop HAM, Roos RAC, Van Dijk JG. 2012. Deficient sustained attention to response task and P300 characteristics in early Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neurology. 259:1191–1198.
    1. Hart EP, Dumas EM, van Zwet EW, van der Hiele K, Jurgens CK, Middelkoop HA, van Dijk JG, Roos RA. 2015. Longitudinal pilot-study of sustained attention to response task and P300 in manifest and pre-manifest Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neuropsychology. 9:10–20.
    1. Huntley JD, Hampshire A, Bor D, Owen AM, Howard RJ. 2017. The importance of sustained attention in early Alzheimer's disease. International journal of geriatric psychiatry. 328:860–867.
    1. Jin CY, Borst JP, van Vugt MK. 2019. Predicting task-general mind-wandering with EEG. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 194:1059–1073.
    1. Jonmohamadi Y, Poudel G, Innes C, Weiss D, Krueger R, Jones R. 2014. Comparison of beamformers for EEG source signal reconstruction. Biomedical Signal Processing and Control. 14:175–188.
    1. Jurgens CK, Van Der Hiele K, Reijntjes R, Van De Wiel L, Witjes-Ané MNW, Van Der J, Roos RAC, Middelkoop HAM, van Dijk JG. 2011. Basal ganglia volume is strongly related to P3 event-related potential in premanifest Huntington’s disease. European Journal of Neurology. 18:1105–1108.
    1. Kam JW, Mickleborough MJ, Eades C, Handy TC. 2015. Migraine and attention to visual events during mind wandering. Experimental Brain Research. 233:1503–1510.
    1. Kilpeläinen R, Luoma L, Herrgård E, Yppärilä H, Partanen J, Karhu J. 1999. Persistent frontal P300 brain potential suggests abnormal processing of auditory information in distractible children. Neuroreport. 10:3405–3410.
    1. Kim H-Y. 2015. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: post-hoc multiple comparisons. Restorative Dentistry and Endodontics. 40:172–176.
    1. Ludolph A, Drory V, Hardiman O, Nakano I, Ravits J, Robberecht W, Shefner J, WFN Research Group On ALS/MND . 2015. A revision of the El Escorial criteria - 2015. Amyotroph Lateral Sclerosis and Frontotemporal Degeneration. 16:291–292.
    1. Lulé D, Ludolph AC, Kassubek J. 2009. MRI-based functional neuroimaging in ALS: an update. Amyotroph Lateral Sclerosis. 10:258–268.
    1. McMackin R, Bede P, Pender N, Hardiman O, Nasseroleslami B. 2019a. Neurophysiological markers of network dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuroimage Clinical. 22:101706.
    1. McMackin R, Dukic S, Broderick M, Iyer PM, Pinto-Grau M, Mohr K, Chipika R, Coffey A, Buxo T, Schuster C, et al. . 2019b. Dysfunction of attention switching networks in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neuroimage Clinical. 22:101707.
    1. McMackin R, Muthuraman M, Groppa S, Babiloni C, Taylor J-P, Kiernan MC, Nasseroleslami B, Hardiman O. 2019c. Measuring network disruption in neurodegenerative diseases: new approaches using signal analysis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 90:1011–1020.
    1. Menon P, Yiannikas C, Kiernan MC, Vucic S. 2019. Regional motor cortex dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology. 6:1373–1382.
    1. O’Connor AR, Han S, Dobbins IG. 2010. The inferior parietal lobule and recognition memory: expectancy violation or successful retrieval? Journal of Neuroscience. 30:2924–2934.
    1. O'Gráda C, Barry S, McGlade N, Behan C, Haq F, Hayden J, O'Donoghue T, Peel R, Morris D.W, O'Callaghan E, et al. . 2009. Does the ability to sustain attention underlie symptom severity in schizophrenia? Schizophrenia research. 107(2-3):319–323.
    1. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen J-M. 2011. FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience. 2011:1–9.
    1. Phukan J, Elamin M, Bede P, Jordan N, Gallagher L, Byrne S, Lynch C, Pender N, Hardiman O. 2012. The syndrome of cognitive impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a population-based study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 83:102–108.
    1. Pinto-Grau M, Burke T, Lonergan K, McHugh C, Mays I, Madden C, Vajda A, Heverin M, Elamin M, Hardiman O, et al. . 2017. Screening for cognitive dysfunction in ALS: validation of the Edinburgh cognitive and behavioural ALS screen (ECAS) using age and education adjusted normative data. Amyotroph Lateral Sclerosis Frontotemporal Degeneration. 18:99–106.
    1. Proudfoot M, Colclough GL, Quinn A, Wuu J, Talbot K, Benatar M, Nobre AC, Woolrich MW, Turner MR. 2018. Increased cerebral functional connectivity in ALS: a resting-state magnetoencephalography study. Neurology. 90:e1418–e1424.
    1. Richard Clark C, McFarlane AC, Weber DL, Battersby M. 1996. Enlarged frontal P300 to stimulus change in panic disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 39:845–856.
    1. Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, Baddeley BT, Yiend J. 1997. `oops!’: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia. 35:747–758.
    1. Staub B, Doignon-Camus N, Marques-Carneiro JE, Bacon É, Bonnefond A. 2015. Age-related differences in the use of automatic and controlled processes in a situation of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia. 75:607–616.
    1. Turner MR, Agosta F, Bede P, Govind V, Lulé D, Verstraete E. 2012. Neuroimaging in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Biomarkers in Medicine. 6:319–337.
    1. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Mazoyer B, Joliot M. 2002. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 15:273–289.
    1. Uddin LQ, Nomi JS, Hebert-Seropian B, Ghaziri J, Boucher O. 2017. Structure and function of the human insula. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 34:300–306.
    1. van Dinteren R, Arns M, Jongsma MLA, Kessels RPC. 2014. Combined frontal and parietal P300 amplitudes indicate compensated cognitive processing across the lifespan. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 6:294.
    1. Van Veen BD, van Drongelen W, Yuchtman M, Suzuki A. 1997. Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering. 44:867–880.
    1. Varjačić A, Mantini D, Levenstein J, Slavkova ED, Demeyere N, Gillebert CR. 2018. The role of left insula in executive set-switching: lesion evidence from an acute stroke cohort. Cortex, In Memory of Professor Glyn Humphreys. 107:92–101.
    1. Zordan L, Sarlo M, Stablum F. 2008. ERP components activated by the “GO!” and “WITHHOLD!” conflict in the random sustained attention to response task. Brain and Cognition. 66:57–64.

Source: PubMed

3
Subscribe